51
|
Southgate V, Begus K. Motor activation during the prediction of nonexecutable actions in infants. Psychol Sci 2013; 24:828-35. [PMID: 23678509 PMCID: PMC3938142 DOI: 10.1177/0956797612459766] [Citation(s) in RCA: 41] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/08/2012] [Accepted: 07/24/2012] [Indexed: 11/23/2022] Open
Abstract
Although it is undeniable that the motor system is recruited when people observe others' actions, the inferences that the brain generates from motor activation and the mechanisms involved in the motor system's recruitment are still unknown. Here, we challenged the popular hypothesis that motor involvement in action observation enables the observer to identify and predict an agent's goal by matching observed actions with existing and corresponding motor representations. Using a novel neural indication of action prediction--sensorimotor-cortex activation measured by electroencephalography--we demonstrated that 9-month-old infants recruit their motor system whenever a context suggests an impending action, but that this recruitment is not dependent on being able to match the observed action with a corresponding motor representation. Our data are thus inconsistent with the view that action prediction depends on motor correspondence; instead, they support an alternative view in which motor activation is the result of, rather than the cause of, goal identification.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Victoria Southgate
- Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, University of London, England.
| | | |
Collapse
|
52
|
Vannuscorps G, Andres M, Pillon A. When does action comprehension need motor involvement? Evidence from upper limb aplasia. Cogn Neuropsychol 2013; 30:253-83. [DOI: 10.1080/02643294.2013.853655] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
|
53
|
Jola C, Grosbras MH. In the here and now: Enhanced motor corticospinal excitability in novices when watching live compared to video recorded dance. Cogn Neurosci 2013; 4:90-8. [PMID: 24073734 DOI: 10.1080/17588928.2013.776035] [Citation(s) in RCA: 35] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
|
54
|
Senna I, Bolognini N, Maravita A. Grasping with the foot: goal and motor expertise in action observation. Hum Brain Mapp 2013; 35:1750-60. [PMID: 23671004 DOI: 10.1002/hbm.22289] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/04/2012] [Revised: 02/20/2013] [Accepted: 02/20/2013] [Indexed: 11/08/2022] Open
Abstract
Action observation typically induces an online inner simulation of the observed movements. Here we investigate whether action observation merely activates, in the observer, the muscles involved in the observed movement or also muscles that are typically used to achieve the observed action goal. In a first experiment, hand and foot motor areas were stimulated by means of transcranial magnetic stimulation, while participants viewed a typical hand action (grasping) or a nonspecific action (stepping over an object) performed by either a hand or a foot. Hand motor evoked potentials (MEPs) increased for grasping and stepping over actions performed by the hand and for grasping actions performed by the foot. Conversely, foot MEPs increased only for actions performed by the foot. In a second experiment, participants viewed a typical hand action (grasping a pencil) and a typical foot action (pressing a foot-pedal) performed by either a hand or a foot. Again, hand MEPs increased not only during the observation of both actions performed by the hand but also for grasping actions performed by the foot. Foot MEPs increased not only during the observation of grasping and pressing actions performed by the foot but also for pressing actions performed by the hand. This evidence indicates that motor activations by action observation occur also in the muscles typically used to perform the observed action, even when the action is executed by an unusual effector, hence suggesting a double coding of observed actions: a strict somatotopic coding and an action goal coding based on the observer's motor expertise.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Irene Senna
- Department of Psychology, University of Milano-Bicocca, Milano, Italy
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
55
|
The effect of limb crossing and limb congruency on multisensory integration in peripersonal space for the upper and lower extremities. Conscious Cogn 2013; 22:545-55. [PMID: 23579198 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2013.02.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/14/2012] [Revised: 02/16/2013] [Accepted: 02/18/2013] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
Abstract
The present study investigated how multisensory integration in peripersonal space is modulated by limb posture (i.e. whether the limbs are crossed or uncrossed) and limb congruency (i.e. whether the observed body part matches the actual position of one's limb). This was done separately for the upper limbs (Experiment 1) and the lower limbs (Experiment 2). The crossmodal congruency task was used to measure peripersonal space integration for the hands and the feet. It was found that the peripersonal space representation for the hands but not for the feet is dynamically updated based on both limb posture and limb congruency. Together these findings show how dynamic cues from vision, proprioception, and touch are integrated in peripersonal limb space and highlight fundamental differences in the way in which peripersonal space is represented for the upper and lower extremity.
Collapse
|
56
|
Tidoni E, Borgomaneri S, di Pellegrino G, Avenanti A. Action simulation plays a critical role in deceptive action recognition. J Neurosci 2013; 33:611-23. [PMID: 23303940 PMCID: PMC6704902 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.2228-11.2013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 91] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/08/2011] [Revised: 11/09/2012] [Accepted: 11/09/2012] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
The ability to infer deceptive intents from nonverbal behavior is critical for social interactions. By combining single-pulse and repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) in healthy humans, we provide both correlational and causative evidence that action simulation is actively involved in the ability to recognize deceptive body movements. We recorded motor-evoked potentials during a faked-action discrimination (FAD) task: participants watched videos of actors lifting a cube and judged whether the actors were trying to deceive them concerning the real weight of the cube. Seeing faked actions facilitated the observers' motor system more than truthful actions in a body-part-specific manner, suggesting that motor resonance was sensitive to deceptive movements. Furthermore, we found that TMS virtual lesion to the anterior node of the action observation network, namely the left inferior frontal cortex (IFC), reduced perceptual sensitivity in the FAD task. In contrast, no change in FAD task performance was found after virtual lesions to the left temporoparietal junction (control site). Moreover, virtual lesion to the IFC failed to affect performance in a difficulty-matched spatial-control task that did not require processing of spatiotemporal (acceleration) and configurational (limb displacement) features of seen actions, which are critical to detecting deceptive intent in the actions of others. These findings indicate that the human IFC is critical for recognizing deceptive body movements and suggest that FAD relies on the simulation of subtle changes in action kinematics within the motor system.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Emmanuele Tidoni
- Istituto di Ricovero e Cura a Carattere Scientifico (IRCCS), Fondazione Santa Lucia, 00179 Rome, Italy
- Dipartimento di Psicologia, Sapienza Università di Roma, 00185 Rome, Italy
| | - Sara Borgomaneri
- Neuroimaging Center, Department of Neuroscience, University of Groningen, 9713 AW Groningen, The Netherlands
- Centro Studi e Ricerche in Neuroscienze Cognitive, Polo Scientifico-Didattico di Cesena, Alma Mater Studiorum Università di Bologna, 47521 Cesena, Italy, and
- Dipartimento di Psicologia, Alma Mater Studiorum Università di Bologna, 40127 Bologna, Italy
| | - Giuseppe di Pellegrino
- Centro Studi e Ricerche in Neuroscienze Cognitive, Polo Scientifico-Didattico di Cesena, Alma Mater Studiorum Università di Bologna, 47521 Cesena, Italy, and
- Dipartimento di Psicologia, Alma Mater Studiorum Università di Bologna, 40127 Bologna, Italy
| | - Alessio Avenanti
- Istituto di Ricovero e Cura a Carattere Scientifico (IRCCS), Fondazione Santa Lucia, 00179 Rome, Italy
- Centro Studi e Ricerche in Neuroscienze Cognitive, Polo Scientifico-Didattico di Cesena, Alma Mater Studiorum Università di Bologna, 47521 Cesena, Italy, and
- Dipartimento di Psicologia, Alma Mater Studiorum Università di Bologna, 40127 Bologna, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
57
|
Fogassi L, Simone L. The Mirror System in Monkeys and Humans and its Possible Motor-Based Functions. ADVANCES IN EXPERIMENTAL MEDICINE AND BIOLOGY 2013; 782:87-110. [DOI: 10.1007/978-1-4614-5465-6_5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/20/2023]
|
58
|
Is gaze following purely reflexive or goal-directed instead? Revisiting the automaticity of orienting attention by gaze cues. Exp Brain Res 2012; 224:93-106. [DOI: 10.1007/s00221-012-3291-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/25/2011] [Accepted: 09/27/2012] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
|
59
|
Kemmerer D, Rudrauf D, Manzel K, Tranel D. Behavioral patterns and lesion sites associated with impaired processing of lexical and conceptual knowledge of actions. Cortex 2012; 48:826-48. [PMID: 21159333 PMCID: PMC3965329 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2010.11.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 161] [Impact Index Per Article: 12.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/15/2009] [Revised: 06/28/2010] [Accepted: 09/24/2010] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
To further investigate the neural substrates of lexical and conceptual knowledge of actions, we administered a battery of six tasks to 226 brain-damaged patients with widely distributed lesions in the left and right cerebral hemispheres. The tasks probed lexical and conceptual knowledge of actions in a variety of verbal and non-verbal ways, including naming, word-picture matching, attribute judgments involving both words and pictures, and associative comparisons involving both words and pictures. Of the 226 patients who were studied, 61 failed one or more of the six tasks, with four patients being impaired on the entire battery, and varied numbers of patients being impaired on varied combinations of tasks. Overall, the 61 patients manifested a complex array of associations and dissociations across the six tasks. The lesion sites of 147 of the 226 patients were also investigated, using formal methods for lesion-deficit statistical mapping and power analysis of lesion overlap maps. Significant effects for all six tasks were found in the following left-hemisphere regions: the inferior frontal gyrus; the ventral precentral gyrus, extending superiorly into what are likely to be hand-related primary motor and premotor areas; and the anterior insula. In addition, significant effects for 4-5 tasks were found in not only the regions just mentioned, but also in several other left-hemisphere areas: the ventral postcentral gyrus; the supramarginal gyrus; and the posterior middle temporal gyrus. These results converge with previous research on the neural underpinnings of action words and concepts. However, the current study goes considerably beyond most previous investigations by providing extensive behavioral and lesion data for an unusually large and diverse sample of brain-damaged patients, and by incorporating multiple measures of verb comprehension. Regarding theoretical implications, the study provides new support for the Embodied Cognition Framework, which maintains that conceptual knowledge is grounded in sensorimotor systems.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- David Kemmerer
- Department of Neurology, Division of Cognitive Neuroscience, University of Iowa College of Medicine, IA, USA.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
60
|
Singer T. The past, present and future of social neuroscience: A European perspective. Neuroimage 2012; 61:437-49. [PMID: 22305955 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.109] [Citation(s) in RCA: 68] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/09/2011] [Revised: 01/20/2012] [Accepted: 01/22/2012] [Indexed: 12/19/2022] Open
|
61
|
Roberts JW, Bennett SJ, Elliott D, Hayes SJ. Top-down and bottom-up processes during observation: Implications for motor learning. Eur J Sport Sci 2012; 14 Suppl 1:S250-6. [DOI: 10.1080/17461391.2012.686063] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/28/2022]
|
62
|
Abstract
To verify if the link between observed hand actions and executed foot actions found in aplasics is essentially induced by the constant use of foot substituting the hand, we investigated if the vision of a grasping hand is able to prime a foot response in normals. Participants were required to detect the time-to-contact of a hand grasping an object either with a suitable or a less suitable movement, an experimental paradigm known to induce a priming effect. Participants responded either with the hand or the foot, while having free or bound hands. Results showed that for hand responses motor priming effect was stronger when the hands were free, whereas for foot responses it was stronger when the hands were bound. These data are interpreted as a further evidence that a difficulty to move affects specific cognitive functions and that the vision of a grasping hand may prime a foot response.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Laila Craighero
- Section of Human Physiology, University of Ferrara, Ferrara, Italy
| | | |
Collapse
|
63
|
Krueger J, Michael J. Gestural coupling and social cognition: Möbius Syndrome as a case study. Front Hum Neurosci 2012; 6:81. [PMID: 22514529 PMCID: PMC3324108 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00081] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/22/2011] [Accepted: 03/22/2012] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Social cognition researchers have become increasingly interested in the ways that behavioral, physiological, and neural coupling facilitate social interaction and interpersonal understanding. We distinguish two ways of conceptualizing the role of such coupling processes in social cognition: strong and moderate interactionism. According to strong interactionism (SI), low-level coupling processes are alternatives to higher-level individual cognitive processes; the former at least sometimes render the latter superfluous. Moderate interactionism (MI) on the other hand, is an integrative approach. Its guiding assumption is that higher-level cognitive processes are likely to have been shaped by the need to coordinate, modulate, and extract information from low-level coupling processes. In this paper, we present a case study on Möbius Syndrome (MS) in order to contrast SI and MI. We show how MS-a form of congenital bilateral facial paralysis-can be a fruitful source of insight for research exploring the relation between high-level cognition and low-level coupling. Lacking a capacity for facial expression, individuals with MS are deprived of a primary channel for gestural coupling. According to SI, they lack an essential enabling feature for social interaction and interpersonal understanding more generally and thus ought to exhibit severe deficits in these areas. We challenge SI's prediction and show how MS cases offer compelling reasons for instead adopting MI's pluralistic model of social interaction and interpersonal understanding. We conclude that investigations of coupling processes within social interaction should inform rather than marginalize or eliminate investigation of higher-level individual cognition.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Joel Krueger
- Center for Subjectivity Research, University of CopenhagenCopenhagen, Denmark
| | - John Michael
- GNOSIS Research Centre, Aarhus UniversityCopenhagen, Denmark
| |
Collapse
|
64
|
Aziz-Zadeh L, Sheng T, Liew SL, Damasio H. Understanding otherness: the neural bases of action comprehension and pain empathy in a congenital amputee. Cereb Cortex 2012; 22:811-9. [PMID: 21734252 PMCID: PMC6276973 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhr139] [Citation(s) in RCA: 37] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/26/2022] Open
Abstract
How do we understand and empathize with individuals whose bodies are drastically different from our own? We investigated the neural processes by which an individual with a radically different body, a congenital amputee who is born without limbs, engages her own sensory-motor representations as a means to understand other people's body actions or emotional states. Our results support the prediction that when the goal of the action is possible for the observer, one's own motor regions are involved in processing action observation, just as when individuals viewed those similar to themselves. However, when the observed actions are not possible, mentalizing mechanisms, relying on a different set of neural structures, are additionally recruited to process the actions. Furthermore, our results indicate that when individuals view others experiencing pain in body parts that they have, the insula and somatosensory cortices are activated, consistent with previous reports. However, when an individual views others experiencing pain in body parts that she does not have, the insula and secondary somatosensory cortices are still active, but the primary somatosensory cortices are not. These results provide a novel understanding for how we understand and empathize with individuals who drastically differ from the self.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lisa Aziz-Zadeh
- Brain and Creativity Institute, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA 90089-2520, USA.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
65
|
Avenanti A, Annella L, Candidi M, Urgesi C, Aglioti SM. Compensatory Plasticity in the Action Observation Network: Virtual Lesions of STS Enhance Anticipatory Simulation of Seen Actions. Cereb Cortex 2012; 23:570-80. [DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhs040] [Citation(s) in RCA: 106] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/14/2022] Open
|
66
|
Imitation of hand and tool actions is effector-independent. Exp Brain Res 2011; 214:539-47. [PMID: 21904930 PMCID: PMC3183242 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-011-2852-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/17/2011] [Accepted: 08/24/2011] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
Following the theoretical notion that tools often extend one’s body, in the present study, we investigated whether imitation of hand or tool actions is modulated by effector-specific information. Subjects performed grasping actions toward an object with either a handheld tool or their right hand. Actions were initiated in response to pictures representing a grip at an object that could be congruent or incongruent with the required action (grip-type congruency). Importantly, actions could be cued by means of a tool cue, a hand cue, and a symbolic cue (effector-type congruency). For both hand and tool actions, an action congruency effect was observed, reflected in faster reaction times if the observed grip type was congruent with the required movement. However, neither hand actions nor tool actions were differentially affected by the effector represented in the picture (i.e., when performing a tool action, the action congruency effect was similar for tool cues and hand cues). This finding suggests that imitation of hand and tool actions is effector-independent and thereby supports generalist rather than specialist theories of imitation.
Collapse
|
67
|
Lago A, Fernandez-del-Olmo M. Movement observation specifies motor programs activated by the action observed objective. Neurosci Lett 2011; 493:102-6. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neulet.2011.02.014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/07/2010] [Revised: 01/21/2011] [Accepted: 02/06/2011] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
|
68
|
Marshall PJ, Meltzoff AN. Neural mirroring systems: exploring the EEG μ rhythm in human infancy. Dev Cogn Neurosci 2011; 1:110-23. [PMID: 21528008 PMCID: PMC3081582 DOI: 10.1016/j.dcn.2010.09.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 137] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/05/2010] [Revised: 09/20/2010] [Accepted: 09/20/2010] [Indexed: 10/19/2022] Open
Abstract
How do human children come to understand the actions of other people? What neural systems are associated with the processing of others' actions and how do these systems develop, starting in infancy? These questions span cognitive psychology and developmental cognitive neuroscience, and addressing them has important implications for the study of social cognition. A large amount of research has used behavioral measures to investigate infants' imitation of the actions of other people; a related but smaller literature has begun to use neurobiological measures to study of infants' action representation. Here we focus on experiments employing electroencephalographic (EEG) techniques for assessing mu rhythm desynchronization in infancy, and analyze how this work illuminates the links between action perception and production prior to the onset of language.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Peter J Marshall
- Department of Psychology, Temple University, Philadelphia, PA 19087, United States.
| | | |
Collapse
|
69
|
Bolognini N, Rossetti A, Maravita A, Miniussi C. Seeing touch in the somatosensory cortex: a TMS study of the visual perception of touch. Hum Brain Mapp 2011; 32:2104-14. [PMID: 21305659 DOI: 10.1002/hbm.21172] [Citation(s) in RCA: 55] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/05/2010] [Revised: 08/20/2010] [Accepted: 08/26/2010] [Indexed: 12/21/2022] Open
Abstract
Recent studies suggest the existence of a visuo-tactile mirror system, comprising the primary (SI) and secondary (SII) somatosensory cortices, which matches observed touch with felt touch. Here, repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) was used to determine whether SI or SII play a functional role in the visual processing of tactile events. Healthy participants performed a visual discrimination task with tactile stimuli (a finger touching a hand) and a control task (a finger moving without touching). During both tasks, rTMS was applied over either SI or SII, and to the occipital cortex. rTMS over SI selectively reduced subject performance for interpreting whether a contralateral visual tactile stimulus contains a tactile event, whereas SII stimulation impaired visual processing regardless of the tactile component. These findings provide evidence for a multimodal sensory-motor system with mirror properties, where somatic and visual properties of action converge. SI, a cortical area traditionally viewed as modality-specific, is selectively implicated in the visual processing of touch. These results are in line with the existence of a sensory mirror system mediating the embodied simulation concept.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nadia Bolognini
- Department of Psychology, University of Milano-Bicocca, Milan, Italy.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
70
|
Gozzi M, Zamboni G, Krueger F, Grafman J. Interest in politics modulates neural activity in the amygdala and ventral striatum. Hum Brain Mapp 2011; 31:1763-71. [PMID: 20162603 DOI: 10.1002/hbm.20976] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022] Open
Abstract
Studies on political participation have found that a person's interest in politics contributes to the likelihood that he or she will be involved in the political process. Here, we looked at whether or not interest in politics affects patterns of brain activity when individuals think about political matters. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), we scanned individuals (either interested or uninterested in politics based on a self-report questionnaire) while they were expressing their agreement or disagreement with political opinions. After scanning, participants were asked to rate each political opinion presented in the scanner for emotional valence and emotional intensity. Behavioral results showed that those political opinions participants agreed with were perceived as more emotionally intense and more positive by individuals interested in politics relative to individuals uninterested in politics. In addition, individuals interested in politics showed greater activation in the amygdala and the ventral striatum (ventral putamen) relative to individuals uninterested in politics when reading political opinions in accordance with their own views. This study shows that having an interest in politics elicits activations in emotion- and reward-related brain areas even when simply agreeing with written political opinions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marta Gozzi
- Cognitive Neuroscience Section, National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD 20892-1440, USA
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
71
|
|
72
|
Abstract
The paper aims at highlighting how our primary understanding of others' actions is rooted in the mirror mechanism. To this end, the anatomical architecture of the mirror neuron system for action will be outlined as well as its role in grasping goals and intentions in others' motor behaviour. One further step through the looking glass of social cognition will be referring to the ubiquitous emotional colouring of actions and considering its links with the motor domain. This will allow a clearer perspective on the mechanism underlying our abilities for emotional understanding and on cases in which these abilities are amiss, as in autistic spectrum disorders.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Corrado Sinigaglia
- Department of Philosophy University of Milan, via Festa del Perdono 7, Milan I-20122, Italy.
| | | |
Collapse
|
73
|
Ortigue S, Sinigaglia C, Rizzolatti G, Grafton ST. Understanding actions of others: the electrodynamics of the left and right hemispheres. A high-density EEG neuroimaging study. PLoS One 2010; 5:e12160. [PMID: 20730095 PMCID: PMC2921336 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0012160] [Citation(s) in RCA: 81] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/19/2010] [Accepted: 07/21/2010] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Background When we observe an individual performing a motor act (e.g. grasping a cup) we get two types of information on the basis of how the motor act is done and the context: what the agent is doing (i.e. grasping) and the intention underlying it (i.e. grasping for drinking). Here we examined the temporal dynamics of the brain activations that follow the observation of a motor act and underlie the observer's capacity to understand what the agent is doing and why. Methodology/Principal Findings Volunteers were presented with two-frame video-clips. The first frame (T0) showed an object with or without context; the second frame (T1) showed a hand interacting with the object. The volunteers were instructed to understand the intention of the observed actions while their brain activity was recorded with a high-density 128-channel EEG system. Visual event-related potentials (VEPs) were recorded time-locked with the frame showing the hand-object interaction (T1). The data were analyzed by using electrical neuroimaging, which combines a cluster analysis performed on the group-averaged VEPs with the localization of the cortical sources that give rise to different spatio-temporal states of the global electrical field. Electrical neuroimaging results revealed four major steps: 1) bilateral posterior cortical activations; 2) a strong activation of the left posterior temporal and inferior parietal cortices with almost a complete disappearance of activations in the right hemisphere; 3) a significant increase of the activations of the right temporo-parietal region with simultaneously co-active left hemispheric sources, and 4) a significant global decrease of cortical activity accompanied by the appearance of activation of the orbito-frontal cortex. Conclusions/Significance We conclude that the early striking left hemisphere involvement is due to the activation of a lateralized action-observation/action execution network. The activation of this lateralized network mediates the understanding of the goal of object-directed motor acts (mirror mechanism). The successive right hemisphere activation indicates that this hemisphere plays an important role in understanding the intention of others.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Stephanie Ortigue
- 4D Brain Electrodynamics Laboratory, Department of Psychology, UCSB Brain Imaging Center, Institute for Collaborative Biotechnologies, University of California Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, California, United States of America
- Laboratory for Advanced Translational Neuroscience, Department of Psychology, Central New York Medical Center, Syracuse University, Syracuse, New York, United States of America
| | | | - Giacomo Rizzolatti
- Department of Neuroscience, University of Parma, Parma, Italy
- Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia, Unità di Parma, Parma, Italy
- * E-mail:
| | - Scott T. Grafton
- 4D Brain Electrodynamics Laboratory, Department of Psychology, UCSB Brain Imaging Center, Institute for Collaborative Biotechnologies, University of California Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, California, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
74
|
Jastorff J, Begliomini C, Fabbri-Destro M, Rizzolatti G, Orban GA. Coding Observed Motor Acts: Different Organizational Principles in the Parietal and Premotor Cortex of Humans. J Neurophysiol 2010; 104:128-40. [DOI: 10.1152/jn.00254.2010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 125] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Understanding actions of conspecifics is a fundamental social ability depending largely on the activation of a parieto-frontal network. Using functional MRI (fMRI), we studied how goal-directed movements (i.e., motor acts) performed by others are coded within this network. In the first experiment, we presented volunteers with video clips showing four different motor acts (dragging, dropping, grasping, and pushing) performed with different effectors (foot, hand, and mouth). We found that the coding of observed motor acts differed between premotor and parietal cortex. In the premotor cortex, they clustered according to the effector used, and in the inferior parietal lobule (IPL), they clustered according to the type of the observed motor act, regardless of the effector. Two subsequent experiments in which we directly contrasted these four motor acts indicated that, in IPL, the observed motor acts are coded according to the relationship between agent and object: Movements bringing the object toward the agent (grasping and dragging) activate a site corresponding approximately to the ventral part of the putative human AIP (phAIP), whereas movements moving the object away from the agent (pushing and dropping) are clustered dorsally within this area. These data provide indications that the phAIP region plays a role in categorizing motor acts according to their behavioral significance. In addition, our results suggest that in the case of motor acts typically done with the hand, the representations of such acts in phAIP are used as templates for coding motor acts executed with other effectors.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jan Jastorff
- Laboratorium voor Neuro- en Psychofysiologie, Medical School, K.U. Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
| | - Chiara Begliomini
- Dipartimento Scienze Biomediche e Terapie Avanzate, Sezione di Fisiologia Umana, Università di Ferrara, Ferrara, Italy
| | - Maddalena Fabbri-Destro
- Dipartimento Scienze Biomediche e Terapie Avanzate, Sezione di Fisiologia Umana, Università di Ferrara, Ferrara, Italy
- Dipartimento di Neuroscienze, Università di Parma, Parma, Italy; and
- Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia, Unità di Parma, Parma, Italy
| | - Giacomo Rizzolatti
- Dipartimento di Neuroscienze, Università di Parma, Parma, Italy; and
- Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia, Unità di Parma, Parma, Italy
| | - Guy A. Orban
- Laboratorium voor Neuro- en Psychofysiologie, Medical School, K.U. Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
| |
Collapse
|
75
|
Fogassi L, Ferrari PF. Mirror systems. WILEY INTERDISCIPLINARY REVIEWS. COGNITIVE SCIENCE 2010; 2:22-38. [PMID: 26301910 DOI: 10.1002/wcs.89] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/11/2022]
Abstract
Mirror neurons are a class of visuomotor neurons, discovered in the monkey premotor cortex and in an anatomically connected area of the inferior parietal lobule, that activate both during action execution and action observation. They constitute a circuit dedicated to match actions made by others with the internal motor representations of the observer. It has been proposed that this matching system enables individuals to understand others' behavior and motor intentions. Here we will describe the main features of mirror neurons in monkeys. Then we will present evidence of the presence of a mirror system in humans and of its involvement in several social-cognitive functions, such as imitation, intention, and emotion understanding. This system may have several implications at a cognitive level and could be linked to specific social deficits in humans such as autism. Recent investigations addressed the issue of the plasticity of the mirror neuron system in both monkeys and humans, suggesting also their possible use in rehabilitation. WIREs Cogn Sci 2011 2 22-38 DOI: 10.1002/wcs.89 For further resources related to this article, please visit the WIREs website.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Leonardo Fogassi
- Departments of Neuroscience and Department of Psychology, Italian Institute of Technology, University of Parma, Italy
| | - Pier Francesco Ferrari
- Departments of Neuroscience and Department of Evolutionary and Functional Biology, Italian Institute of Technology, University of Parma, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
76
|
KANAKOGI YASUHIRO, ITAKURA SHOJI. The link between perception and action in early infancy: From the viewpoint of the direct-matching hypothesis. JAPANESE PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2010. [DOI: 10.1111/j.1468-5884.2010.00429.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
|
77
|
Abstract
The discovery of mirror neurons in motor areas of the brain has led many to assume that our ability to understand other people's behaviour partially relies on vicarious activations of motor cortices. This Review focuses the limelight of social neuroscience on a different set of brain regions: the somatosensory cortices. These have anatomical connections that enable them to have a role in visual and auditory social perception. Studies that measure brain activity while participants witness the sensations, actions and somatic pain of others consistently show vicarious activation in the somatosensory cortices. Neuroscientists are starting to understand how the brain adds a somatosensory dimension to our perception of other people.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Christian Keysers
- Social Brain Laboratory, Department of Neuroscience, University Medical Center Groningen, A. Deusinglaan 2, 9713AW Groningen, The Netherlands.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
78
|
van Elk M, van Schie H, Zwaan R, Bekkering H. The functional role of motor activation in language processing: Motor cortical oscillations support lexical-semantic retrieval. Neuroimage 2010; 50:665-77. [PMID: 20060478 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.123] [Citation(s) in RCA: 105] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/10/2009] [Revised: 12/24/2009] [Accepted: 12/31/2009] [Indexed: 12/31/2022] Open
|
79
|
The functional architecture of the human body: assessing body representation by sorting body parts and activities. Exp Brain Res 2010; 203:119-29. [PMID: 20333367 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-010-2216-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/16/2009] [Accepted: 03/04/2010] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
Abstract
We investigated mental representations of body parts and body-related activities in two subjects with congenitally absent limbs (one with, the other without phantom sensations), a wheelchair sports group of paraplegic participants, and two groups of participants with intact limbs. To analyse mental representation structures, we applied Structure Dimensional Analysis. Verbal labels indicating body parts and related activities were presented in randomized lists that had to be sorted according to a hierarchical splitting paradigm. Participants were required to group the items according to whether or not they were considered related, based on their own body perception. Results of the groups of physically intact and paraplegic participants revealed separate clusters for the lower body, upper body, fingers and head. The participant with congenital phantom limbs also showed a clear separation between upper and lower body (but not between fingers and hands). In the participant without phantom sensations of the absent arms, no such modularity emerged, but the specific practice of his right foot in communication and daily routines was reflected. Sorting verbal labels of body parts and activities appears a useful method to assess body representation in individuals with special body anatomy or function and leads to conclusions largely compatible with other assessment procedures.
Collapse
|
80
|
Rizzolatti G, Sinigaglia C. The functional role of the parieto-frontal mirror circuit: interpretations and misinterpretations. Nat Rev Neurosci 2010; 11:264-74. [PMID: 20216547 DOI: 10.1038/nrn2805] [Citation(s) in RCA: 984] [Impact Index Per Article: 65.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
|
81
|
Heyes C. Where do mirror neurons come from? Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2010; 34:575-83. [PMID: 19914284 DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.11.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 298] [Impact Index Per Article: 19.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/24/2009] [Revised: 11/06/2009] [Accepted: 11/07/2009] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
|
82
|
Van Overwalle F, Baetens K. Understanding others' actions and goals by mirror and mentalizing systems: A meta-analysis. Neuroimage 2009; 48:564-84. [PMID: 19524046 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 873] [Impact Index Per Article: 54.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/13/2009] [Revised: 04/28/2009] [Accepted: 06/01/2009] [Indexed: 12/01/2022] Open
|
83
|
Keysers C, Gazzola V. Expanding the mirror: vicarious activity for actions, emotions, and sensations. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2009; 19:666-71. [PMID: 19880311 DOI: 10.1016/j.conb.2009.10.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 278] [Impact Index Per Article: 17.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/03/2009] [Revised: 09/30/2009] [Accepted: 10/09/2009] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
We often empathically share the states of others. The discovery of 'mirror neurons' suggested a neural mechanism for monkeys to share the actions of others. Here we expand this view by showing that mirror neurons for actions not only exist in the premotor cortex or in monkeys and that vicarious activity can also be measured for the emotions and sensations of others. Although we still need to empirically explore the function and development of these vicarious activations, we should stop thinking of vicarious brain activity as a peculiar property of the premotor cortex: instead it seems to be a very common phenomenon which leads social stimuli to recruit a wide range of seemingly private neural systems.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Christian Keysers
- Department of Neuroscience, University Medical Center Groningen, University of Groningen, A. Deusinglaad 2, 9713AW Groningen, The Netherlands.
| | | |
Collapse
|
84
|
Incarnation and animation: physical versus representational deficits of body integrity. Exp Brain Res 2009; 204:315-26. [DOI: 10.1007/s00221-009-2043-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/05/2009] [Accepted: 10/02/2009] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
|
85
|
Mirror neurons: from discovery to autism. Exp Brain Res 2009; 200:223-37. [DOI: 10.1007/s00221-009-2002-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 173] [Impact Index Per Article: 10.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/12/2009] [Accepted: 08/27/2009] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
|
86
|
Schippers MB, Gazzola V, Goebel R, Keysers C. Playing charades in the fMRI: are mirror and/or mentalizing areas involved in gestural communication? PLoS One 2009; 4:e6801. [PMID: 19710923 PMCID: PMC2728843 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0006801] [Citation(s) in RCA: 61] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/06/2009] [Accepted: 07/24/2009] [Indexed: 11/25/2022] Open
Abstract
Communication is an important aspect of human life, allowing us to powerfully coordinate our behaviour with that of others. Boiled down to its mere essentials, communication entails transferring a mental content from one brain to another. Spoken language obviously plays an important role in communication between human individuals. Manual gestures however often aid the semantic interpretation of the spoken message, and gestures may have played a central role in the earlier evolution of communication. Here we used the social game of charades to investigate the neural basis of gestural communication by having participants produce and interpret meaningful gestures while their brain activity was measured using functional magnetic resonance imaging. While participants decoded observed gestures, the putative mirror neuron system (pMNS: premotor, parietal and posterior mid-temporal cortex), associated with motor simulation, and the temporo-parietal junction (TPJ), associated with mentalizing and agency attribution, were significantly recruited. Of these areas only the pMNS was recruited during the production of gestures. This suggests that gestural communication relies on a combination of simulation and, during decoding, mentalizing/agency attribution brain areas. Comparing the decoding of gestures with a condition in which participants viewed the same gestures with an instruction not to interpret the gestures showed that although parts of the pMNS responded more strongly during active decoding, most of the pMNS and the TPJ did not show such significant task effects. This suggests that the mere observation of gestures recruits most of the system involved in voluntary interpretation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marleen B Schippers
- BCN NeuroImaging Center, University of Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
87
|
Abstract
Why do we feel tears well up when we see a loved one cry? Why do we wince when we see other people hurt themselves? This review addresses these questions from the perspective of embodied simulation: observing the actions and tactile sensations of others activates premotor, posterior parietal and somatosensory regions in the brain of the observer which are also active when performing similar movements and feeling similar sensations. We will show that seeing the emotions of others also recruits regions involved in experiencing similar emotions, although there does not seem to be a reliable mapping of particular emotions onto particular brain regions. Instead, emotion simulation seems to involve a mosaic of affective, motor and somatosensory components. The relative contributions of these components to a particular emotion and their interrelationship are largely unknown, although recent experimental evidence suggests that motor simulation may be a trigger for the simulation of associated feeling states. This mosaic of simulations may be necessary for generating the compelling insights we have into the feelings of others. Through their integration with, and modulation by, higher cognitive functions, they could be at the core of important social functions, including empathy, mind reading and social learning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- J. A. C. J. Bastiaansen
- BCN NeuroImaging Center, University of Groningen, Antonius Deusinglaan 2, 9713 AW Groningen, The Netherlands
- Department of Neuroscience, University Medical Center Groningen, Antonius Deusinglaan 1, 9713 AW Groningen, The Netherlands
- Autism Team North Netherlands, Lentis, Hereweg 80, 9725 AG Groningen, The Netherlands
| | - M. Thioux
- BCN NeuroImaging Center, University of Groningen, Antonius Deusinglaan 2, 9713 AW Groningen, The Netherlands
- Department of Neuroscience, University Medical Center Groningen, Antonius Deusinglaan 1, 9713 AW Groningen, The Netherlands
| | - C. Keysers
- BCN NeuroImaging Center, University of Groningen, Antonius Deusinglaan 2, 9713 AW Groningen, The Netherlands
- Department of Neuroscience, University Medical Center Groningen, Antonius Deusinglaan 1, 9713 AW Groningen, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
88
|
Kokal I, Gazzola V, Keysers C. Acting together in and beyond the mirror neuron system. Neuroimage 2009; 47:2046-56. [PMID: 19524043 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 72] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/12/2008] [Revised: 05/22/2009] [Accepted: 06/01/2009] [Indexed: 11/15/2022] Open
Abstract
Moving a set dinner table often takes two people, and doing so without spilling the glasses requires the close coordination of the two agents' actions. It has been argued that the mirror neuron system may be the key neural locus of such coordination. Instead, here we show that such coordination recruits two separable sets of areas: one that could translate between motor and visual codes and one that could integrate these information to achieve common goals. The former includes regions of the putative mirror neuron system, the latter, regions of the prefrontal, posterior parietal and temporal lobe adjacent to the putative mirror neuron system. Both networks were more active while participants cooperated with a human agent, responding to their actions, compared to a computer that did not, evidencing their social dimension. This finding shows that although the putative mirror neuron system can play a critical role in joint actions by translating both agents' actions into a common code, the flexible remapping of our own actions with those of others required during joint actions seems to be performed outside of the putative mirror neuron system.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Idil Kokal
- BCN NeuroImaging Center, University of Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
89
|
Abstract
This meta-analysis explores the location and function of brain areas involved in social cognition, or the capacity to understand people's behavioral intentions, social beliefs, and personality traits. On the basis of over 200 fMRI studies, it tests alternative theoretical proposals that attempt to explain how several brain areas process information relevant for social cognition. The results suggest that inferring temporary states such as goals, intentions, and desires of other people-even when they are false and unjust from our own perspective--strongly engages the temporo-parietal junction (TPJ). Inferring more enduring dispositions of others and the self, or interpersonal norms and scripts, engages the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), although temporal states can also activate the mPFC. Other candidate tasks reflecting general-purpose brain processes that may potentially subserve social cognition are briefly reviewed, such as sequence learning, causality detection, emotion processing, and executive functioning (action monitoring, attention, dual task monitoring, episodic memory retrieval), but none of them overlaps uniquely with the regions activated during social cognition. Hence, it appears that social cognition particularly engages the TPJ and mPFC regions. The available evidence is consistent with the role of a TPJ-related mirror system for inferring temporary goals and intentions at a relatively perceptual level of representation, and the mPFC as a module that integrates social information across time and allows reflection and representation of traits and norms, and presumably also of intentionality, at a more abstract cognitive level.
Collapse
|
90
|
Abstract
Since the discovery of mirror neurons, much effort has been invested into studying their location and properties in the human brain. Here we review these original findings and introduce the main topics of this special issue of Social Neuroscience. What does the mirror system code? How is the mirror system embedded into the mosaic of circuits that compose our brain? How does the mirror system contribute to communication, language and social interaction? Can the principle of mirror neurons be extended to emotions, sensations and thoughts? Papers using a wide range of methods, including single cell recordings, fMRI, TMS, EEG and psychophysics, collected in this special issue, start to give us some impressive answers.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Christian Keysers
- University of Groningen, and University Medical Center, Groningen, The Netherlands.
| | | |
Collapse
|
91
|
Bekkering H, de Bruijn ERA, Cuijpers RH, Newman-Norlund R, van Schie HT, Meulenbroek R. Joint Action: Neurocognitive Mechanisms Supporting Human Interaction. Top Cogn Sci 2009; 1:340-52. [DOI: 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2009.01023.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 103] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
|
92
|
Turella L, Erb M, Grodd W, Castiello U. Visual features of an observed agent do not modulate human brain activity during action observation. Neuroimage 2009; 46:844-53. [PMID: 19285143 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.03.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 37] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/14/2008] [Revised: 02/25/2009] [Accepted: 03/01/2009] [Indexed: 10/21/2022] Open
Abstract
Recent neuroimaging evidence in macaques has shown that the neural system underlying the observation of hand actions performed by others (i.e., "action observation system") is modulated by whether the observed action is performed by a person in full view or an isolated hand (i.e., type of view manipulation). Although a human homologue of such circuit has been identified, whether in humans the neural processes involved in this capacity are modulated by the type of view remains unknown. Here we used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to investigate whether the "action observation system", with specific reference to the ventral premotor cortex, responds differentially depending on type of view. We also tested this manipulation within regions of the human brain showing overlapping activity for both the observation and the execution of action ("mirror" regions). To this end, the same subjects were requested to observe grasping actions performed under the two types of view (observation conditions) or to perform a grasping action (execution condition). Results from whole-brain analyses indicate that overlapping activity for action observation and execution was evident in a broad network of areas including parietal, premotor and temporal cortices. Activity within such network was evident for both the observation of a person in full view or an isolated hand, but it was not modulated by the type of view. Similarly, results from region of interest (ROI) analyses, performed within the ventral premotor cortex, did confirm that this area responded in a similar fashion following the observation of either an isolated hand or an entire model acting. These findings offer novel insights on what the "action observation" and the "mirror" systems visually code and how the processing underlying such coding may vary across species. Further, they support the hypothesis that action goal is amongst the main determinants for the revelation of action observation activity, and to the existence of a broad system involved in the simulation of action.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Luca Turella
- Section on Experimental MR of the CNS, Department of Neuroradiology, University of Tuebingen, Germany
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
93
|
Willems RM, Hagoort P. Hand preference influences neural correlates of action observation. Brain Res 2009; 1269:90-104. [PMID: 19272363 DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.02.057] [Citation(s) in RCA: 31] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/07/2008] [Revised: 01/28/2009] [Accepted: 02/25/2009] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
Abstract
It has been argued that we map observed actions onto our own motor system. Here we added to this issue by investigating whether hand preference influences the neural correlates of action observation of simple, essentially meaningless hand actions. Such an influence would argue for an intricate neural coupling between action production and action observation, which goes beyond effects of motor repertoire or explicit motor training, as has been suggested before. Indeed, parts of the human motor system exhibited a close coupling between action production and action observation. Ventral premotor and inferior and superior parietal cortices showed differential activation for left- and right-handers that was similar during action production as well as during action observation. This suggests that mapping observed actions onto the observer's own motor system is a core feature of action observation - at least for actions that do not have a clear goal or meaning. Basic differences in the way we act upon the world are not only reflected in neural correlates of action production, but can also influence the brain basis of action observation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Roel M Willems
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Nijmegen, PO Box 9101, 6500 HB Nijmegen, The Netherlands.
| | | |
Collapse
|
94
|
Affiliation(s)
- Marco Del Giudice
- Center for Cognitive Science, Department of Psychology, University of Turin, Torino, Italy.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
95
|
Mirror neurons and their clinical relevance. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2009; 5:24-34. [PMID: 19129788 DOI: 10.1038/ncpneuro0990] [Citation(s) in RCA: 214] [Impact Index Per Article: 13.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/15/2008] [Accepted: 11/13/2008] [Indexed: 12/12/2022]
|
96
|
Gazzola V, Keysers C. The observation and execution of actions share motor and somatosensory voxels in all tested subjects: single-subject analyses of unsmoothed fMRI data. Cereb Cortex 2008; 19:1239-55. [PMID: 19020203 PMCID: PMC2677653 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhn181] [Citation(s) in RCA: 480] [Impact Index Per Article: 28.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/23/2023] Open
Abstract
Many neuroimaging studies of the mirror neuron system (MNS) examine if certain voxels in the brain are shared between action observation and execution (shared voxels, sVx). Unfortunately, finding sVx in standard group analyses is not a guarantee that sVx exist in individual subjects. Using unsmoothed, single-subject analyses we show sVx can be reliably found in all 16 investigated participants. Beside the ventral premotor (BA6/44) and inferior parietal cortex (area PF) where mirror neurons (MNs) have been found in monkeys, sVx were reliably observed in dorsal premotor, supplementary motor, middle cingulate, somatosensory (BA3, BA2, and OP1), superior parietal, middle temporal cortex and cerebellum. For the premotor, somatosensory and parietal areas, sVx were more numerous in the left hemisphere. The hand representation of the primary motor cortex showed a reduced BOLD during hand action observation, possibly preventing undesired overt imitation. This study provides a more detailed description of the location and reliability of sVx and proposes a model that extends the original idea of the MNS to include forward and inverse internal models and motor and sensory simulation, distinguishing the MNS from a more general concept of sVx.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Valeria Gazzola
- University Medical Center Groningen, University of Groningen, Department of Neuroscience, Groningen, The Netherlands.
| | | |
Collapse
|
97
|
Etzel JA, Gazzola V, Keysers C. Testing simulation theory with cross-modal multivariate classification of fMRI data. PLoS One 2008; 3:e3690. [PMID: 18997869 PMCID: PMC2577733 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0003690] [Citation(s) in RCA: 91] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/21/2008] [Accepted: 10/21/2008] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
The discovery of mirror neurons has suggested a potential neural basis for simulation and common coding theories of action perception, theories which propose that we understand other people's actions because perceiving their actions activates some of our neurons in much the same way as when we perform the actions. We propose testing this model directly in humans with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) by means of cross-modal classification. Cross-modal classification evaluates whether a classifier that has learned to separate stimuli in the sensory domain can also separate the stimuli in the motor domain. Successful classification provides support for simulation theories because it means that the fMRI signal, and presumably brain activity, is similar when perceiving and performing actions. In this paper we demonstrate the feasibility of the technique by showing that classifiers which have learned to discriminate whether a participant heard a hand or a mouth action, based on the activity patterns in the premotor cortex, can also determine, without additional training, whether the participant executed a hand or mouth action. This provides direct evidence that, while perceiving others' actions, (1) the pattern of activity in premotor voxels with sensory properties is a significant source of information regarding the nature of these actions, and (2) that this information shares a common code with motor execution.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Joset A Etzel
- BCN Neuroimaging Center, University of Groningen, Department of Neuroscience, University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
98
|
Pazzaglia M, Pizzamiglio L, Pes E, Aglioti SM. The Sound of Actions in Apraxia. Curr Biol 2008; 18:1766-72. [PMID: 19013068 DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.09.061] [Citation(s) in RCA: 103] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/27/2008] [Revised: 09/14/2008] [Accepted: 09/21/2008] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Mariella Pazzaglia
- Dipartimento di Psicologia, Università degli Studi di Roma "La Sapienza", Via dei Marsi 78, I-00185 Rome, Italy.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
99
|
Catmur C, Gillmeister H, Bird G, Liepelt R, Brass M, Heyes C. Through the looking glass: counter-mirror activation following incompatible sensorimotor learning. Eur J Neurosci 2008; 28:1208-15. [PMID: 18783371 DOI: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06419.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 99] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
The mirror system, comprising cortical areas that allow the actions of others to be represented in the observer's own motor system, is thought to be crucial for the development of social cognition in humans. Despite the importance of the human mirror system, little is known about its origins. We investigated the role of sensorimotor experience in the development of the mirror system. Functional magnetic resonance imaging was used to measure neural responses to observed hand and foot actions following one of two types of training. During training, participants in the Compatible (control) group made mirror responses to observed actions (hand responses were made to hand stimuli and foot responses to foot stimuli), whereas the Incompatible group made counter-mirror responses (hand to foot and foot to hand). Comparison of these groups revealed that, after training to respond in a counter-mirror fashion, the relative action observation properties of the mirror system were reversed; areas that showed greater responses to observation of hand actions in the Compatible group responded more strongly to observation of foot actions in the Incompatible group. These results suggest that, rather than being innate or the product of unimodal visual or motor experience, the mirror properties of the mirror system are acquired through sensorimotor learning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Caroline Catmur
- Department of Psychology, University College London, London, UK.
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
100
|
|