51
|
Abstract
Adverse life events can induce two kinds of memory with opposite valence, dependent on timing: "negative" memories for stimuli preceding them and "positive" memories for stimuli experienced at the moment of "relief." Such punishment memory and relief memory are found in insects, rats, and man. For example, fruit flies (Drosophila melanogaster) avoid an odor after odor-shock training ("forward conditioning" of the odor), whereas after shock-odor training ("backward conditioning" of the odor) they approach it. Do these timing-dependent associative processes share molecular determinants? We focus on the role of Synapsin, a conserved presynaptic phosphoprotein regulating the balance between the reserve pool and the readily releasable pool of synaptic vesicles. We find that a lack of Synapsin leaves task-relevant sensory and motor faculties unaffected. In contrast, both punishment memory and relief memory scores are reduced. These defects reflect a true lessening of associative memory strength, as distortions in nonassociative processing (e.g., susceptibility to handling, adaptation, habituation, sensitization), discrimination ability, and changes in the time course of coincidence detection can be ruled out as alternative explanations. Reductions in punishment- and relief-memory strength are also observed upon an RNAi-mediated knock-down of Synapsin, and are rescued both by acutely restoring Synapsin and by locally restoring it in the mushroom bodies of mutant flies. Thus, both punishment memory and relief memory require the Synapsin protein and in this sense share genetic and molecular determinants. We note that corresponding molecular commonalities between punishment memory and relief memory in humans would constrain pharmacological attempts to selectively interfere with excessive associative punishment memories, e.g., after traumatic experiences.
Collapse
|
52
|
Appel M, Scholz CJ, Müller T, Dittrich M, König C, Bockstaller M, Oguz T, Khalili A, Antwi-Adjei E, Schauer T, Margulies C, Tanimoto H, Yarali A. Genome-Wide Association Analyses Point to Candidate Genes for Electric Shock Avoidance in Drosophila melanogaster. PLoS One 2015; 10:e0126986. [PMID: 25992709 PMCID: PMC4436303 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0126986] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/02/2014] [Accepted: 04/09/2015] [Indexed: 01/27/2023] Open
Abstract
Electric shock is a common stimulus for nociception-research and the most widely used reinforcement in aversive associative learning experiments. Yet, nothing is known about the mechanisms it recruits at the periphery. To help fill this gap, we undertook a genome-wide association analysis using 38 inbred Drosophila melanogaster strains, which avoided shock to varying extents. We identified 514 genes whose expression levels and/ or sequences co-varied with shock avoidance scores. We independently scrutinized 14 of these genes using mutants, validating the effect of 7 of them on shock avoidance. This emphasizes the value of our candidate gene list as a guide for follow-up research. In addition, by integrating our association results with external protein-protein interaction data we obtained a shock avoidance-associated network of 38 genes. Both this network and the original candidate list contained a substantial number of genes that affect mechanosensory bristles, which are hair-like organs distributed across the fly’s body. These results may point to a potential role for mechanosensory bristles in shock sensation. Thus, we not only provide a first list of candidate genes for shock avoidance, but also point to an interesting new hypothesis on nociceptive mechanisms.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mirjam Appel
- Research Group Molecular Systems Biology of Learning, Leibniz Institute of Neurobiology, Magdeburg, Germany
- Max Planck Institute of Neurobiology, Martinsried, Germany
| | - Claus-Jürgen Scholz
- Laboratory for Microarray Applications, IZKF, University of Würzburg, Würzburg, Germany
| | - Tobias Müller
- Department of Bioinformatics, Biocenter, University of Würzburg, Würzburg, Germany
| | - Marcus Dittrich
- Department of Bioinformatics, Biocenter, University of Würzburg, Würzburg, Germany
- Institute of Human Genetics, University of Würzburg, Würzburg, Germany
| | - Christian König
- Research Group Molecular Systems Biology of Learning, Leibniz Institute of Neurobiology, Magdeburg, Germany
| | | | - Tuba Oguz
- Max Planck Institute of Neurobiology, Martinsried, Germany
| | - Afshin Khalili
- Research Group Molecular Systems Biology of Learning, Leibniz Institute of Neurobiology, Magdeburg, Germany
| | - Emmanuel Antwi-Adjei
- Research Group Molecular Systems Biology of Learning, Leibniz Institute of Neurobiology, Magdeburg, Germany
| | - Tamas Schauer
- Department of Physiological Chemistry, Butenandt Institute and LMU Biomedical Center, Faculty of Medicine, Ludwig-Maximilians-University of Munich, Munich, Germany
| | - Carla Margulies
- Department of Physiological Chemistry, Butenandt Institute and LMU Biomedical Center, Faculty of Medicine, Ludwig-Maximilians-University of Munich, Munich, Germany
| | - Hiromu Tanimoto
- Max Planck Institute of Neurobiology, Martinsried, Germany
- Tohoku University Graduate School of Life Sciences, Sendai, Japan
| | - Ayse Yarali
- Research Group Molecular Systems Biology of Learning, Leibniz Institute of Neurobiology, Magdeburg, Germany
- Max Planck Institute of Neurobiology, Martinsried, Germany
- Center for Behavioral Brain Sciences, Magdeburg, Germany
- * E-mail:
| |
Collapse
|
53
|
Masek P, Worden K, Aso Y, Rubin GM, Keene AC. A dopamine-modulated neural circuit regulating aversive taste memory in Drosophila. Curr Biol 2015; 25:1535-41. [PMID: 25981787 DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.04.027] [Citation(s) in RCA: 66] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/01/2014] [Revised: 02/25/2015] [Accepted: 04/14/2015] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
Abstract
Taste memories allow animals to modulate feeding behavior in accordance with past experience and avoid the consumption of potentially harmful food [1]. We have developed a single-fly taste memory assay to functionally interrogate the neural circuitry encoding taste memories [2]. Here, we screen a collection of Split-GAL4 lines that label small populations of neurons associated with the fly memory center-the mushroom bodies (MBs) [3]. Genetic silencing of PPL1 dopamine neurons disrupts conditioned, but not naive, feeding behavior, suggesting these neurons are selectively involved in the conditioned taste response. We identify two PPL1 subpopulations that innervate the MB α lobe and are essential for aversive taste memory. Thermogenetic activation of these dopamine neurons during training induces memory, indicating these neurons are sufficient for the reinforcing properties of bitter tastant to the MBs. Silencing of either the intrinsic MB neurons or the output neurons from the α lobe disrupts taste conditioning. Thermogenetic manipulation of these output neurons alters naive feeding response, suggesting that dopamine neurons modulate the threshold of response to appetitive tastants. Taken together, these findings detail a neural mechanism underlying the formation of taste memory and provide a functional model for dopamine-dependent plasticity in Drosophila.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Pavel Masek
- Department of Biology, University of Nevada Reno, Reno, NV 89557, USA.
| | - Kurtresha Worden
- Department of Biology, University of Nevada Reno, Reno, NV 89557, USA
| | - Yoshinori Aso
- Janelia Research Campus, 19700 Helix Drive, Ashburn, VA 20147, USA
| | - Gerald M Rubin
- Janelia Research Campus, 19700 Helix Drive, Ashburn, VA 20147, USA
| | - Alex C Keene
- Department of Biology, University of Nevada Reno, Reno, NV 89557, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
54
|
Schleyer M, Miura D, Tanimura T, Gerber B. Learning the specific quality of taste reinforcement in larval Drosophila. eLife 2015; 4. [PMID: 25622533 PMCID: PMC4302267 DOI: 10.7554/elife.04711] [Citation(s) in RCA: 34] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/18/2014] [Accepted: 12/22/2014] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
The only property of reinforcement insects are commonly thought to learn about is its value. We show that larval Drosophila not only remember the value of reinforcement (How much?), but also its quality (What?). This is demonstrated both within the appetitive domain by using sugar vs amino acid as different reward qualities, and within the aversive domain by using bitter vs high-concentration salt as different qualities of punishment. From the available literature, such nuanced memories for the quality of reinforcement are unexpected and pose a challenge to present models of how insect memory is organized. Given that animals as simple as larval Drosophila, endowed with but 10,000 neurons, operate with both reinforcement value and quality, we suggest that both are fundamental aspects of mnemonic processing-in any brain.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michael Schleyer
- Department of Genetics of Learning and Memory, Leibniz Institute for Neurobiology, Magdeburg, Germany
| | - Daisuke Miura
- Department of Biology, Kyushu University, Fukuoka, Japan
| | | | - Bertram Gerber
- Department of Genetics of Learning and Memory, Leibniz Institute for Neurobiology, Magdeburg, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
55
|
Barbagallo B, Garrity PA. Temperature sensation in Drosophila. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2015; 34:8-13. [PMID: 25616212 DOI: 10.1016/j.conb.2015.01.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 68] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/19/2014] [Revised: 01/06/2015] [Accepted: 01/06/2015] [Indexed: 12/11/2022]
Abstract
Animals use thermosensory systems to achieve optimal temperatures for growth and reproduction and to avoid damaging extremes. Thermoregulation is particularly challenging for small animals like the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster, whose body temperature rapidly changes in response to environmental temperature fluctuation. Recent work has uncovered some of the key molecules mediating fly thermosensation, including the Transient Receptor Potential (TRP) channels TRPA1 and Painless, and the Gustatory Receptor Gr28b, an unanticipated thermosensory regulator normally associated with a different sensory modality. There is also evidence the Drosophila phototransduction cascade may have some role in thermosensory responses. Together, the fly's diverse thermosensory molecules act in an array of functionally distinct thermosensory neurons to drive a suite of complex, and often exceptionally thermosensitive, behaviors.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Belinda Barbagallo
- National Center for Behavioral Genomics and Volen Center for Complex Systems, Department of Biology, Brandeis University, Waltham, MA 02458, United States
| | - Paul A Garrity
- National Center for Behavioral Genomics and Volen Center for Complex Systems, Department of Biology, Brandeis University, Waltham, MA 02458, United States.
| |
Collapse
|
56
|
Distinct dopamine neurons mediate reward signals for short- and long-term memories. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2014; 112:578-83. [PMID: 25548178 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1421930112] [Citation(s) in RCA: 167] [Impact Index Per Article: 15.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Drosophila melanogaster can acquire a stable appetitive olfactory memory when the presentation of a sugar reward and an odor are paired. However, the neuronal mechanisms by which a single training induces long-term memory are poorly understood. Here we show that two distinct subsets of dopamine neurons in the fly brain signal reward for short-term (STM) and long-term memories (LTM). One subset induces memory that decays within several hours, whereas the other induces memory that gradually develops after training. They convey reward signals to spatially segregated synaptic domains of the mushroom body (MB), a potential site for convergence. Furthermore, we identified a single type of dopamine neuron that conveys the reward signal to restricted subdomains of the mushroom body lobes and induces long-term memory. Constant appetitive memory retention after a single training session thus comprises two memory components triggered by distinct dopamine neurons.
Collapse
|