51
|
Pelaccia T, Tardif J, Triby E, Ammirati C, Bertrand C, Charlin B, Dory V. Insights into emergency physicians' minds in the seconds before and into a patient encounter. Intern Emerg Med 2015; 10:865-73. [PMID: 26232196 DOI: 10.1007/s11739-015-1283-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/21/2015] [Accepted: 07/08/2015] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
Abstract
Clinical reasoning is a core competency in medical practice. No study has explored clinical reasoning occurring before a clinical encounter, when physicians obtain preliminary information about the patient, and during the first seconds of the observation phase. This paper aims to understand what happens in emergency physicians' minds when they acquire initial information about a patient, and when they first meet a patient. The authors carried out in-depth interviews based on the video recordings of emergency situations filmed in an "own-point-of-view-perspective". 15 expert emergency physicians were interviewed between 2011 and 2012. Researchers analysed data using an interpretive approach based on thematic analysis and constant comparison. Almost all participants used a few critical pieces of information to generate hypotheses even before they actually met the patient. Pre-encounter hypotheses played a key role in the ensuing encounter by directing initial data gathering. Initial data, collected within the first few seconds of the encounter, included the patient's position on the stretcher, the way they had been prepared, their facial expression, their breathing, and their skin colour. Physicians also rapidly appraised the seriousness of the patient's overall condition, which determined their initial goals, i.e. initiating emergency treatment or pursuing the diagnostic investigation. The study brings new insights on what happens at the very beginning of the encounter between emergency physicians and patients. The results obtained from an innovative methodological approach open avenues for the development of clinical reasoning in learners.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Thierry Pelaccia
- Faculty of Medicine, Centre for Training and Research in Health Sciences Education (CFR-PS), University of Strasbourg, 67000, Strasbourg, France.
- Prehospital Emergency Care Service (SAMU 67), Centre for Emergency Care Teaching (CESU 67), Strasbourg University Hospital, PO Box 426, 67200, Strasbourg, France.
| | - Jacques Tardif
- Department of Pedagogy, Faculty of Education, University of Sherbrooke, Sherbrooke, QC, J1K 2R1, Canada.
| | - Emmanuel Triby
- Faculty of Educational Sciences, University of Strasbourg, 67000, Strasbourg, France.
| | - Christine Ammirati
- Department of Emergency Medicine, Amiens University Hospital, 80054, Amiens, France.
| | - Catherine Bertrand
- Prehospital Emergency Care Service (SAMU 94), Henri-Mondor Hospital, Public Hospitals of Paris, 94000, Créteil, France.
| | - Bernard Charlin
- Faculty of Medicine, Centre of Pedagogy Applied to Health Sciences (CPASS), University of Montreal, Montréal, QC, H3T 1J4, Canada.
| | - Valérie Dory
- Undergraduate Medical Education and Centre for Medical Education, McGill University, Montreal, QC, H3A 1A3, Canada.
| |
Collapse
|
52
|
Pinnock R, Young L, Spence F, Henning M, Hazell W. Can Think Aloud Be Used to Teach and Assess Clinical Reasoning in Graduate Medical Education? J Grad Med Educ 2015; 7:334-7. [PMID: 26457135 PMCID: PMC4597940 DOI: 10.4300/jgme-d-14-00601.1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Ralph Pinnock
- Corresponding author: Ralph Pinnock, MBChB, FRACP, MClinEd, Dunedin Public Hospital, Paediatrics and Child Health, Great King Street, Dunedin 9054, New Zealand, 00.64.3.4740999,
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
53
|
Postma TC, White JG. Developing clinical reasoning in the classroom - analysis of the 4C/ID-model. EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF DENTAL EDUCATION : OFFICIAL JOURNAL OF THE ASSOCIATION FOR DENTAL EDUCATION IN EUROPE 2015; 19:74-80. [PMID: 24810116 DOI: 10.1111/eje.12105] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 03/18/2014] [Indexed: 06/03/2023]
Abstract
OBJECTIVES The purpose of this study is to provide a theoretical rationale for the implementation of the four-component instructional design model for complex learning (4C/ID-model) at the University of Pretoria with the aim of systematically developing the clinical reasoning of undergraduate dental students in a classroom setting. METHODS Recent literature provides a rationale for the use of the 4C/ID-model to teach clinical reasoning in terms of four strategic teaching and learning needs: (i) the need for authenticity in the learning process; (ii) the need to manage cognitive load during the learning process; (iii) the need for repeated practice; (iv) a need for valid assessment, feedback and reflection. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION The literature review indicated that the 4C/ID-model makes provision for teaching and learning in an authentic context and proposes systematically structured methods that could be practically applied to manage cognitive load during repetitive exercises. The model also makes provision for cognitive feedback following assessment in order to eliminate misconceptions about content and to develop the cognitive strategies of the learner. CONCLUSIONS On the basis of the literature review above, it is recommended that the 4C/ID-model be considered as a basis for classroom teaching and learning to develop competence in clinical reasoning in undergraduate dental students, even at pre-clinical level. The model may also have an application in medical education.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- T C Postma
- Department of Dental Management Sciences, School of Dentistry, University of Pretoria, Pretoria, South Africa
| | | |
Collapse
|
54
|
Seidel BM, Campbell S, Bell E. Evidence in clinical reasoning: a computational linguistics analysis of 789,712 medical case summaries 1983-2012. BMC Med Inform Decis Mak 2015; 15:19. [PMID: 25880840 PMCID: PMC4381480 DOI: 10.1186/s12911-015-0136-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/15/2014] [Accepted: 02/03/2015] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
Background Better understanding of clinical reasoning could reduce diagnostic error linked to 8% of adverse medical events and 30% of malpractice cases. To a greater extent than the evidence-based movement, the clinical reasoning literature asserts the importance of practitioner intuition—unconscious elements of diagnostic reasoning. The study aimed to analyse the content of case report summaries in ways that explored the importance of an evidence concept, not only in relation to research literature but also intuition. Methods The study sample comprised all 789,712 abstracts in English for case reports contained in the database PUBMED for the period 1 January 1983 to 31 December 2012. It was hypothesised that, if evidence and intuition concepts were viewed by these clinical authors as essential to understanding their case reports, they would be more likely to be found in the abstracts. Computational linguistics software was used in 1) concept mapping of 21,631,481 instances of 201 concepts, and 2) specific concept analyses examining 200 paired co-occurrences for ‘evidence’ and research ‘literature’ concepts. Results ‘Evidence’ is a fundamentally patient-centred, intuitive concept linked to less common concepts about underlying processes, suspected disease mechanisms and diagnostic hunches. In contrast, the use of research literature in clinical reasoning is linked to more common reasoning concepts about specific knowledge and descriptions or presenting features of cases. ‘Literature’ is by far the most dominant concept, increasing in relevance since 2003, with an overall relevance of 13% versus 5% for ‘evidence’ which has remained static. Conclusions The fact that the least present types of reasoning concepts relate to diagnostic hunches to do with underlying processes, such as what is suspected, raises questions about whether intuitive practitioner evidence-making, found in a constellation of dynamic, process concepts, has become less important. The study adds support to the existing corpus of research on clinical reasoning, by suggesting that intuition involves a complex constellation of concepts important to how the construct of evidence is understood. The list of concepts the study generated offers a basis for reflection on the nature of evidence in diagnostic reasoning and the importance of intuition to that reasoning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Bastian M Seidel
- Wicking Dementia Education and Research Centre, University of Tasmania, Hobart, TAS, 7001, Australia.
| | - Steven Campbell
- School of Health Sciences, University of Tasmania, Launceston, TAS, 7250, Australia
| | - Erica Bell
- Wicking Dementia Education and Research Centre, University of Tasmania, Hobart, TAS, 7001, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
55
|
Pelaccia T, Tardif J, Triby E, Ammirati C, Bertrand C, Dory V, Charlin B. How and When Do Expert Emergency Physicians Generate and Evaluate Diagnostic Hypotheses? A Qualitative Study Using Head-Mounted Video Cued-Recall Interviews. Ann Emerg Med 2014; 64:575-85. [DOI: 10.1016/j.annemergmed.2014.05.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 51] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/10/2013] [Revised: 04/24/2014] [Accepted: 05/05/2014] [Indexed: 01/15/2023]
|
56
|
Cianciolo AT. Deciding 'what to teach' health professionals: a human-centred systems engineering perspective. MEDICAL EDUCATION 2014; 48:1150-1156. [PMID: 25413909 DOI: 10.1111/medu.12570] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/19/2014] [Revised: 06/13/2014] [Accepted: 07/29/2014] [Indexed: 06/04/2023]
Abstract
CONTEXT Recent calls propose the conceptualisation of medical education research as 'an improvement science for complex social interventions'. This involves developing principled, yet contextually grounded, descriptions of health care practice that increase the likelihood of successful intervention. Defining what health professionals should be taught using theoretical perspectives and analytical techniques borrowed from human-centred systems engineering (HCSE) may acknowledge this call by allowing learning objectives and performance assessment criteria to be aligned with the demands of actual work. DISCUSSION Human-centred systems engineering is a multidisciplinary endeavour that seeks to promote the safe, efficient and productive performance of socio-technological systems. Systems theories in HCSE explain how environmental conditions constrain and afford human goal-directed behaviour and are modified by such. Many of the techniques used in HCSE research that are applicable to examining health care practice should be familiar to medical education researchers. This method differs from other empirical approaches that have been applied to the study of health care practice in its emphasis on practical problem solving via intervention design. CONCLUSIONS Learning objectives and performance assessment criteria derived from an HCSE perspective target people's attunement to environmental conditions as they strive to enact goal-directed behaviour. Implementing educational interventions from an HCSE perspective should facilitate a sustained positive impact across contexts because theories of person-environment interaction enable principled adaptations of interventions to local circumstances.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anna T Cianciolo
- Department of Medical Education, Southern Illinois University School of Medicine, Springfield, Illinois, USA
| |
Collapse
|
57
|
Weinberger H, Cohen J, Tadmor B, Singer P. Towards a framework for untangling complexity: The interprofessional decision-making model for the complex patient. J Assoc Inf Sci Technol 2014. [DOI: 10.1002/asi.23176] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/10/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Hadas Weinberger
- Independent researcher, Department of Intensive Care; Rabin Medical Centre; Beilinson Hospital; 49100 Petah Tikva Israel
| | - Jonathan Cohen
- Department of Intensive Care; Rabin Medical Centre; Beilinson Hospital; 49100 Petah Tikva Israel
| | - Boaz Tadmor
- General management; Rabin Medical Centre; Beilinson Hospital; 49100 Petah Tikva Israel
| | - Pierre Singer
- Department of Intensive Care; Institute for Nutrition Research; Rabin Medical Centre; Beilinson Hospital; 49100 Petah Tikva Israel
| |
Collapse
|
58
|
Abstract
Errors in clinical reasoning continue to account for significant morbidity and mortality, despite evidence-based guidelines and improved technology. Experts in clinical reasoning often use unconscious cognitive processes that they are not aware of unless they explain how they are thinking. Understanding the intuitive and analytical thinking processes provides a guide for instruction. How knowledge is stored is critical to expertise in clinical reasoning. Curricula should be designed so that trainees store knowledge in a way that is clinically relevant. Competence in clinical reasoning is acquired by supervised practice with effective feedback. Clinicians must recognise the common errors in clinical reasoning and how to avoid them. Trainees can learn clinical reasoning effectively in everyday practice if teachers provide guidance on the cognitive processes involved in making diagnostic decisions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ralph Pinnock
- Child and Adolescent Health, James Cook University, Townsville, Queensland, Australia
| | | |
Collapse
|
59
|
Sibbald M, de Bruin ABH, van Merrienboer JJG. Twelve tips on engaging learners in checking health care decisions. MEDICAL TEACHER 2014; 36:111-115. [PMID: 24256130 DOI: 10.3109/0142159x.2013.847910] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/02/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Checking diagnostic and management decisions can help reduce medical error, however, little literature explores how this is best taught. AIMS To provide practical advice to direct teaching practices. METHODS The authors conducted a literature review using Medline and PsychInfo using search terms: check or checklist and medical error or diagnostic error, supplemented by a manual search through cited literature. CONCLUSION Twelve tips for teaching how to check diagnostic and management decisions are presented.
Collapse
|
60
|
Ann Flavell C, Gordon S, Marshman L, Watt K. Inter-rater reliability of classification systems in chronic low back pain populations. PHYSICAL THERAPY REVIEWS 2014. [DOI: 10.1179/1743288x13y.0000000131] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/31/2022]
|
61
|
Kiesewetter J, Ebersbach R, Görlitz A, Holzer M, Fischer MR, Schmidmaier R. Cognitive problem solving patterns of medical students correlate with success in diagnostic case solutions. PLoS One 2013; 8:e71486. [PMID: 23951175 PMCID: PMC3741183 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0071486] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/13/2013] [Accepted: 06/28/2013] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Context Problem-solving in terms of clinical reasoning is regarded as a key competence of medical doctors. Little is known about the general cognitive actions underlying the strategies of problem-solving among medical students. In this study, a theory-based model was used and adapted in order to investigate the cognitive actions in which medical students are engaged when dealing with a case and how patterns of these actions are related to the correct solution. Methods Twenty-three medical students worked on three cases on clinical nephrology using the think-aloud method. The transcribed recordings were coded using a theory-based model consisting of eight different cognitive actions. The coded data was analysed using time sequences in a graphical representation software. Furthermore the relationship between the coded data and accuracy of diagnosis was investigated with inferential statistical methods. Results The observation of all main actions in a case elaboration, including evaluation, representation and integration, was considered a complete model and was found in the majority of cases (56%). This pattern significantly related to the accuracy of the case solution (φ = 0.55; p<.001). Extent of prior knowledge was neither related to the complete model nor to the correct solution. Conclusions The proposed model is suitable to empirically verify the cognitive actions of problem-solving of medical students. The cognitive actions evaluation, representation and integration are crucial for the complete model and therefore for the accuracy of the solution. The educational implication which may be drawn from this study is to foster students reasoning by focusing on higher level reasoning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jan Kiesewetter
- Lehrstuhl für Didaktik und Ausbildungsforschung in der Medizin am Klinikum der Universität München, Ludwig-Maximilians-University, Munich, Germany.
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
62
|
Claessens YE, Wannepain S, Gestin S, Magdelein X, Ferretti E, Guilly M, Charlin B, Pelaccia T. How emergency physicians use biomarkers: insights from a qualitative assessment of script concordance tests. Emerg Med J 2013; 31:238-41. [DOI: 10.1136/emermed-2012-202303] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
|
63
|
Cianciolo AT, Williams RG, Klamen DL, Roberts NK. Biomedical knowledge, clinical cognition and diagnostic justification: a structural equation model. MEDICAL EDUCATION 2013; 47:309-16. [PMID: 23398017 DOI: 10.1111/medu.12096] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/18/2023]
Abstract
CONTEXT The process whereby medical students employ integrated analytic and non-analytic diagnostic strategies is not fully understood. Analysing academic performance data could provide a perspective complementary to that of laboratory experiments when investigating the nature of diagnostic strategy. This study examined the performance data of medical students in an integrated curriculum to determine the relative contributions of biomedical knowledge and clinical pattern recognition to diagnostic strategy. METHODS Structural equation modelling was used to examine the relationship between biomedical knowledge and clinical cognition (clinical information gathering and interpretation) assessed in Years 1 and 2 of medical school and their relative contributions to diagnostic justification assessed at the beginning of Year 4. Modelling was applied to the academic performance data of 133 medical students who received their md degrees in 2011 and 2012. RESULTS The model satisfactorily fit the data. The correlation between biomedical knowledge and clinical cognition was low-moderate (0.26). The paths between these two constructs and diagnostic justification were moderate and slightly favoured biomedical knowledge (0.47 and 0.40 for biomedical knowledge and clinical cognition, respectively). CONCLUSIONS The findings suggest that within the first 2 years of medical school, students possessed separate, but complementary, cognitive tools, comprising biomedical knowledge and clinical pattern recognition, which contributed to an integrated diagnostic strategy at the beginning of Year 4. Assessing diagnostic justification, which requires students to make their thinking explicit, may promote the integration of analytic and non-analytic processing into diagnostic strategy.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anna T Cianciolo
- Department of Medical Education, Southern Illinois University School of Medicine, Springfield, Illinois 62794-9681, USA.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
64
|
|
65
|
Groves M. Understanding clinical reasoning: the next step in working out how it really works. MEDICAL EDUCATION 2012; 46:444-446. [PMID: 22515751 DOI: 10.1111/j.1365-2923.2012.04244.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/31/2023]
Affiliation(s)
- Michele Groves
- Faculty of Health Scineces, Royal Brisbane and Women's Hospital, University of Queensland, Herston, Queensland, Australia.
| |
Collapse
|