1
|
Noto T, Nakamura T, Amemiya T. Synergistic Illusions: Enhancing Perceptual Effects of Pseudo-Attraction Force by Kinesthetic Illusory Hand Movement. IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON HAPTICS 2024; 17:729-741. [PMID: 38587964 DOI: 10.1109/toh.2024.3386199] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 04/10/2024]
Abstract
We investigated the enhancement of the perceived force strength in force feedback devices by combining the pulling illusion with kinesthetic illusions. The pulling illusion (i.e., a sensation of being pulled or pushed) is induced by asymmetric vibrations applied to the fingertips, enabling the implementation of small, lightweight, and ungrounded force feedback devices. However, the perceived force intensity is limited. We focused on the kinesthetic illusion, a phenomenon in which the movement of a limb in the direction of muscle extension is illusively perceived by presenting vibrations to tendons or muscles as an illusion that could enhance the perceived strength of the pulling illusion. Moreover, we investigated the perceptual characteristics of force sensation by combining a kinesthetic illusion induced by wrist tendon vibration stimulation with a pulling illusion. The findings demonstrate that the direction of the pulling illusion was accurately perceived, even with simultaneous wrist tendon vibration stimuli. Importantly, the results suggest that tendon vibration on the wrist, rather than cutaneous vibration on the wrist, enhances the perceived force intensity of the pulling illusion at the fingertips. These findings indicate the potential for expanding the expressive capability of the pulling illusion.
Collapse
|
2
|
Grünbaum T, Christensen MS. The functional role of conscious sensation of movement. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2024; 164:105813. [PMID: 39019245 DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2024.105813] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/01/2024] [Revised: 06/25/2024] [Accepted: 07/13/2024] [Indexed: 07/19/2024]
Abstract
This paper proposes a new framework for investigating neural signals sufficient for a conscious sensation of movement and their role in motor control. We focus on signals sufficient for proprioceptive awareness, particularly from muscle spindle activation and from primary motor cortex (M1). Our review of muscle vibration studies reveals that afferent signals alone can induce conscious sensations of movement. Similarly, studies employing peripheral nerve blocks suggest that efferent signals from M1 are sufficient for sensations of movement. On this basis, we show that competing theories of motor control assign different roles to sensation of movement. According to motor command theories, sensation of movement corresponds to an estimation of the current state based on afferent signals, efferent signals, and predictions. In contrast, within active inference architectures, sensations correspond to proprioceptive predictions driven by efferent signals from M1. The focus on sensation of movement provides a way to critically compare and evaluate the two theories. Our analysis offers new insights into the functional roles of movement sensations in motor control and consciousness.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Thor Grünbaum
- Department of Psychology, University of Copenhagen, Denmark; CoInAct Research Group, University of Copenhagen, Denmark; Section for Philosophy, University of Copenhagen, Denmark.
| | - Mark Schram Christensen
- Department of Psychology, University of Copenhagen, Denmark; CoInAct Research Group, University of Copenhagen, Denmark
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Festin C, Ortmayr J, Maierhofer U, Tereshenko V, Blumer R, Schmoll M, Carrero-Rojas G, Luft M, Laengle G, Farina D, Bergmeister KD, Aszmann OC. Creation of a biological sensorimotor interface for bionic reconstruction. Nat Commun 2024; 15:5337. [PMID: 38914540 PMCID: PMC11196281 DOI: 10.1038/s41467-024-49580-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/07/2023] [Accepted: 06/12/2024] [Indexed: 06/26/2024] Open
Abstract
Neuromuscular control of bionic arms has constantly improved over the past years, however, restoration of sensation remains elusive. Previous approaches to reestablish sensory feedback include tactile, electrical, and peripheral nerve stimulation, however, they cannot recreate natural, intuitive sensations. Here, we establish an experimental biological sensorimotor interface and demonstrate its potential use in neuroprosthetics. We transfer a mixed nerve to a skeletal muscle combined with glabrous dermal skin transplantation, thus forming a bi-directional communication unit in a rat model. Morphological analyses indicate reinnervation of the skin, mechanoreceptors, NMJs, and muscle spindles. Furthermore, sequential retrograde labeling reveals specific sensory reinnervation at the level of the dorsal root ganglia. Electrophysiological recordings show reproducible afferent signals upon tactile stimulation and tendon manipulation. The results demonstrate the possibility of surgically creating an interface for both decoding efferent motor control, as well as encoding afferent tactile and proprioceptive feedback, and may indicate the way forward regarding clinical translation of biological communication pathways for neuroprosthetic applications.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Christopher Festin
- Clinical Laboratory for Bionic Extremity Reconstruction, Department of Plastic, Reconstructive and Aesthetic Surgery, Medical University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
- Center for Biomedical Research, Medical University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
| | - Joachim Ortmayr
- Clinical Laboratory for Bionic Extremity Reconstruction, Department of Plastic, Reconstructive and Aesthetic Surgery, Medical University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
| | - Udo Maierhofer
- Clinical Laboratory for Bionic Extremity Reconstruction, Department of Plastic, Reconstructive and Aesthetic Surgery, Medical University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
- Center for Biomedical Research, Medical University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
| | - Vlad Tereshenko
- Clinical Laboratory for Bionic Extremity Reconstruction, Department of Plastic, Reconstructive and Aesthetic Surgery, Medical University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
- Center for Biomedical Research, Medical University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
- Division of Plastic and Reconstructive Surgery, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Roland Blumer
- Center for Anatomy and Cell Biology, Medical University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
| | - Martin Schmoll
- Center for Medical Physics and Biomedical Engineering, Medical University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
| | - Génova Carrero-Rojas
- Center for Anatomy and Cell Biology, Medical University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
| | - Matthias Luft
- Clinical Laboratory for Bionic Extremity Reconstruction, Department of Plastic, Reconstructive and Aesthetic Surgery, Medical University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
- Center for Biomedical Research, Medical University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
- Department of Plastic, Aesthetic and Reconstructive Surgery, University Hospital St. Poelten, Karl Landsteiner University of Health Sciences, Krems, Austria
| | - Gregor Laengle
- Clinical Laboratory for Bionic Extremity Reconstruction, Department of Plastic, Reconstructive and Aesthetic Surgery, Medical University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
- Center for Biomedical Research, Medical University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
- Department of Plastic, Reconstructive and Aesthetic Surgery, Medical University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
| | - Dario Farina
- Department of Bioengineering, Imperial College London, London, United Kingdom
| | - Konstantin D Bergmeister
- Clinical Laboratory for Bionic Extremity Reconstruction, Department of Plastic, Reconstructive and Aesthetic Surgery, Medical University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
- Department of Plastic, Aesthetic and Reconstructive Surgery, University Hospital St. Poelten, Karl Landsteiner University of Health Sciences, Krems, Austria
| | - Oskar C Aszmann
- Clinical Laboratory for Bionic Extremity Reconstruction, Department of Plastic, Reconstructive and Aesthetic Surgery, Medical University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria.
- Department of Plastic, Reconstructive and Aesthetic Surgery, Medical University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria.
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Bergevin M, Steele J, Payen de la Garanderie M, Feral-Basin C, Marcora SM, Rainville P, Caron JG, Pageaux B. Pharmacological Blockade of Muscle Afferents and Perception of Effort: A Systematic Review with Meta-analysis. Sports Med 2023; 53:415-435. [PMID: 36318384 DOI: 10.1007/s40279-022-01762-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 09/03/2022] [Indexed: 01/27/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND The perception of effort provides information on task difficulty and influences physical exercise regulation and human behavior. This perception differs from other-exercise related perceptions such as pain. There is no consensus on the role of group III/IV muscle afferents as a signal processed by the brain to generate the perception of effort. OBJECTIVE The aim of this meta-analysis was to investigate the effect of pharmacologically blocking muscle afferents on the perception of effort. METHODS Six databases were searched to identify studies measuring the ratings of perceived effort during physical exercise, with and without pharmacological blockade of muscle afferents. Articles were coded based on the operational measurement used to distinguish studies in which perception of effort was assessed specifically (effort dissociated) or as a composite experience including other exercise-related perceptions (effort not dissociated). Articles that did not provide enough information for coding were assigned to the unclear group. RESULTS The effort dissociated group (n = 6) demonstrated a slight increase in ratings of perceived effort with reduced muscle afferent feedback (standard mean change raw, 0.39; 95% confidence interval 0.13-0.64). The group effort not dissociated (n = 2) did not reveal conclusive results (standard mean change raw, - 0.29; 95% confidence interval - 2.39 to 1.8). The group unclear (n = 8) revealed a slight ratings of perceived effort decrease with reduced muscle afferent feedback (standard mean change raw, - 0.27; 95% confidence interval - 0.50 to - 0.04). CONCLUSIONS The heterogeneity in results between groups reveals that the inclusion of perceptions other than effort in its rating influences the ratings of perceived effort reported by the participants. The absence of decreased ratings of perceived effort in the effort dissociated group suggests that muscle afferent feedback is not a sensory signal for the perception of effort.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Maxime Bergevin
- École de kinésiologie et des sciences de l'activite physique (EKSAP), Faculté de médecine, Université́ de Montréal, Montreal, QC, Canada.,Centre de recherche de l'Institut universitaire de gériatrie de Montréal (CRIUGM), Montreal, Canada
| | - James Steele
- School of Sport, Health and Social Sciences, Southampton, UK
| | - Marie Payen de la Garanderie
- École de kinésiologie et des sciences de l'activite physique (EKSAP), Faculté de médecine, Université́ de Montréal, Montreal, QC, Canada.,Centre de recherche de l'Institut universitaire de gériatrie de Montréal (CRIUGM), Montreal, Canada
| | - Camille Feral-Basin
- École de kinésiologie et des sciences de l'activite physique (EKSAP), Faculté de médecine, Université́ de Montréal, Montreal, QC, Canada.,Centre de recherche de l'Institut universitaire de gériatrie de Montréal (CRIUGM), Montreal, Canada
| | - Samuele M Marcora
- Department of Biomedical and Neuromotor Sciences (DiBiNeM), University of Bologna, Bologna, Italy
| | - Pierre Rainville
- Centre de recherche de l'Institut universitaire de gériatrie de Montréal (CRIUGM), Montreal, Canada.,Département de stomatologie, Faculté de médecine dentaire, Université de Montréal, Montreal, QC, Canada
| | - Jeffrey G Caron
- École de kinésiologie et des sciences de l'activite physique (EKSAP), Faculté de médecine, Université́ de Montréal, Montreal, QC, Canada.,Centre de recherche interdisciplinaire en réadaptation du Montréal métropolitain, Montreal, QC, Canada
| | - Benjamin Pageaux
- École de kinésiologie et des sciences de l'activite physique (EKSAP), Faculté de médecine, Université́ de Montréal, Montreal, QC, Canada. .,Centre de recherche de l'Institut universitaire de gériatrie de Montréal (CRIUGM), Montreal, Canada. .,Centre interdisciplinaire de recherche sur le cerveau et l'apprentissage (CIRCA), Montreal, QC, Canada.
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Leskovar R, Moore JM, Robertson J, Exell TA, Ostler C, Kyberd PJ. An investigation of proprioception illusion using a stimulator with feedback control. IEEE Int Conf Rehabil Robot 2022; 2022:1-6. [PMID: 36176077 DOI: 10.1109/icorr55369.2022.9896564] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/16/2023]
Abstract
It is possible to create illusions of limb movements using vibrations over the skin. If a muscle is vibrated it can feel as if the limb is moving while it remains still. These illusions have been studied for decades but it is not yet entirely clear how to create them effectively and repeatedly. In this study, three parameters were varied; the frequency of the vibration, the stimulation site and the arm position. A closed loop control of the vibration frequency was used to ensure a fixed frequency over the stimulation time and across the participants. The experiment included twenty-five able-bodied participants (mean age 32±7 years, 9 females). A hanging arm position was introduced with the aim to increase the success rate of illusions compared to other studies. Twenty-four participants felt an illusion across all scenarios. The results highlight that tactile feedback affects the illusion.
Collapse
|
6
|
Prado A, Agrawal SK. Effects of Localized Leg Muscle Vibration Timed to Gait Cycle Percentage During Overground Walking. IEEE Robot Autom Lett 2022. [DOI: 10.1109/lra.2022.3181415] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Antonio Prado
- Robotics and Rehabilitation Laboratory, Department of Mechanical Engineering, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
| | - Sunil K. Agrawal
- Robotics and Rehabilitation Laboratory, Department of Mechanical Engineering, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Maemura K, Nishikawa S, Kiguchi K. A Study on Artificial Kinesthesia Generation by Simultaneous Stimulation of Mechanical Vibration and Mechanical Skin Stretch. IEEE Int Conf Rehabil Robot 2022; 2022:1-6. [PMID: 36176148 DOI: 10.1109/icorr55369.2022.9896609] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/16/2023]
Abstract
Artificially controlled kinesthesia can be applied to many situations because kinesthesia is essential to recognizing body movements. It could be used to generate artificial kinesthesia in rehabilitation or daily motion assist to improve self-efficacy of the robot user. Moreover, the controlled artificial kinesthesia could make people feel as if they are performing the actions of the robotic limbs with their own limbs. Mechanical vibration stimulation is one of the candidates to artificially control kinesthesia. It is known that mechanical vibration stimulation on human muscles or tendons from skin surface evokes an illusion of movement as if the stimulated muscles are extended. That effect of artificial kinesthesia is called Kinesthetic Illusion (KI). In this paper, a method to increase the amount of KI without changing the frequency of the vibration stimulation is investigated by applying mechanical skin stretch stimulation at the same time with the mechanical vibration stimulation. The experiment was conducted by generating KI for flexion motion of the elbow joint on a horizontal plane to evaluate the proposed approach. In the experiments, three out of five subjects showed obvious increase in the amount of KI when skin stretch stimulation was applied at the same time with the mechanical vibration stimulation. The results of this study provide a first step toward artificial kinesthesia control using a wearable robotic device using the mechanical vibration stimulation.
Collapse
|
8
|
Vargas L, Huang H, Zhu Y, Hu X. Object Recognition via Evoked Sensory Feedback during Control of a Prosthetic Hand. IEEE Robot Autom Lett 2022; 7:207-214. [PMID: 35784093 PMCID: PMC9248871 DOI: 10.1109/lra.2021.3122897] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/03/2023]
Abstract
Haptic and proprioceptive feedback is critical for sensorimotor integration when we use our hand to perform daily tasks. Here, we evaluated how externally evoked haptic and proprioceptive feedback and myoelectric control strategies affected the recognition of object properties when participants controlled a prosthetic hand. Fingertip haptic sensation was elicited using a transcutaneous nerve stimulation grid to encode the prosthetic's fingertip forces. An array of tactors elicited patterned vibratory stimuli to encode tactile-proprioceptive kinematic information of the prosthetic finger joint. Myoelectric signals of the finger flexor and extensor were used to control the position or velocity of joint angles of the prosthesis. Participants were asked to perform object property (stiffness and size) recognition, by controlling the prosthetic hand with concurrent haptic and tactile-proprioceptive feedback. With the evoked feedback, intact and amputee participants recognized the object stiffness and size at success rates ranging from 50% to 100% in both position and velocity control with no significant difference across control schemes. Our findings show that evoked somatosensory feedback in a non-invasive manner can facilitate closed-loop control of the prosthetic hand and allowed for simultaneous recognition of different object properties. The outcomes can facilitate our understanding on the role of sensory feedback during bidirectional human-machine interactions, which can potentially promote user experience in object interactions using prosthetic hands.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Luis Vargas
- Joint Department of Biomedical Engineering at University of North Carolina-Chapel Hill and NC State University
| | - He Huang
- Joint Department of Biomedical Engineering at University of North Carolina-Chapel Hill and NC State University
| | - Yong Zhu
- Mechanical and Aerospace Engineering Department at NC State University
| | - Xiaogang Hu
- Joint Department of Biomedical Engineering at University of North Carolina-Chapel Hill and NC State University
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Le Franc S, Bonan I, Fleury M, Butet S, Barillot C, Lécuyer A, Cogné M. Visual feedback improves movement illusions induced by tendon vibration after chronic stroke. J Neuroeng Rehabil 2021; 18:156. [PMID: 34717672 PMCID: PMC8556973 DOI: 10.1186/s12984-021-00948-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/20/2021] [Accepted: 10/13/2021] [Indexed: 11/10/2022] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND Illusion of movement induced by tendon vibration is commonly used in rehabilitation and seems valuable for motor rehabilitation after stroke, by playing a role in cerebral plasticity. The aim was to study if congruent visual cues using Virtual Reality (VR) could enhance the illusion of movement induced by tendon vibration of the wrist among participants with stroke. METHODS We included 20 chronic stroke participants. They experienced tendon vibration of their wrist (100 Hz, 30 times) inducing illusion of movement. Three VR visual conditions were added to the vibration: a congruent moving virtual hand (Moving condition); a static virtual hand (Static condition); or no virtual hand at all (Hidden condition). The participants evaluated for each visual condition the intensity of the illusory movement using a Likert scale, the sensation of wrist's movement using a degree scale and they answered a questionnaire about their preferred condition. RESULTS The Moving condition was significantly superior to the Hidden condition and to the Static condition in terms of illusion of movement (p < 0.001) and the wrist's extension (p < 0.001). There was no significant difference between the Hidden and the Static condition for these 2 criteria. The Moving condition was considered the best one to increase the illusion of movement (in 70% of the participants). Two participants did not feel any illusion of movement. CONCLUSIONS This study showed the interest of using congruent cues in VR in order to enhance the consistency of the illusion of movement induced by tendon vibration among participants after stroke, regardless of their clinical severity. By stimulating the brain motor areas, this visuo-proprioceptive feedback could be an interesting tool in motor rehabilitation. Record number in Clinical Trials: NCT04130711, registered on October 17th 2019 ( https://clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT04130711?id=NCT04130711&draw=2&rank=1 ).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Salomé Le Franc
- Rehabilitation Medicine Unit, CHU de Rennes, University Hospital of Rennes, 2, rue Henri Le Guilloux, 35000, Rennes, France.
- Hybrid Unity, Inria, University of Rennes, Irisa, 6074 Umr Cnrs, Rennes, France.
| | - Isabelle Bonan
- Rehabilitation Medicine Unit, CHU de Rennes, University Hospital of Rennes, 2, rue Henri Le Guilloux, 35000, Rennes, France
- Empenn Unity U1228, Inserm, Inria, University of Rennes, Irisa, 6074 Umr Cnrs, Rennes, France
| | - Mathis Fleury
- Hybrid Unity, Inria, University of Rennes, Irisa, 6074 Umr Cnrs, Rennes, France
- Empenn Unity U1228, Inserm, Inria, University of Rennes, Irisa, 6074 Umr Cnrs, Rennes, France
| | - Simon Butet
- Rehabilitation Medicine Unit, CHU de Rennes, University Hospital of Rennes, 2, rue Henri Le Guilloux, 35000, Rennes, France
- Empenn Unity U1228, Inserm, Inria, University of Rennes, Irisa, 6074 Umr Cnrs, Rennes, France
| | - Christian Barillot
- Empenn Unity U1228, Inserm, Inria, University of Rennes, Irisa, 6074 Umr Cnrs, Rennes, France
| | - Anatole Lécuyer
- Hybrid Unity, Inria, University of Rennes, Irisa, 6074 Umr Cnrs, Rennes, France
| | - Mélanie Cogné
- Rehabilitation Medicine Unit, CHU de Rennes, University Hospital of Rennes, 2, rue Henri Le Guilloux, 35000, Rennes, France
- Hybrid Unity, Inria, University of Rennes, Irisa, 6074 Umr Cnrs, Rennes, France
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Two senses of human limb position: methods of measurement and roles in proprioception. Exp Brain Res 2021; 239:3157-3174. [PMID: 34482421 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-021-06207-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/19/2021] [Accepted: 08/26/2021] [Indexed: 12/18/2022]
Abstract
The sense of position of the body and its limbs is a proprioceptive sense. Proprioceptors are concerned with monitoring the body's own actions. Position sense is important because it is believed to contribute to our self-awareness. This review discusses recent developments in the debate about the sources of peripheral afferent signals contributing to position sense and describes different methods of measurement of position sense under conditions where vision does not participate. These include pointing to or verbal reporting of the perceived position of a hidden body part, alignment of one body part with the perceived position of another, or using memory-based repositioning tasks. The evidence suggests that there are at least two different mechanisms involved in the generation of position sense, mechanisms using different central processing pathways. The principal sensory receptor responsible for position sense is believed to be the muscle spindle. One criterion for identifying mechanism is whether position sense can be manipulated by controlled changes in spindle discharge rates. Position sense measured in two-limb matching is altered in a predictable way by such changes, while values for pointing and verbal reporting remain unresponsive. It is proposed that in two-limb matching the sensation generated is limb position in postural space. In pointing or verbal reporting, information is provided about limb position in extrapersonal space. Here vision is believed to play a role. The evidence suggests that we are aware, at the same time, of sensations of limb position in postural space as well as in extrapersonal space.
Collapse
|
11
|
Le Franc S, Fleury M, Jeunet C, Butet S, Barillot C, Bonan I, Cogné M, Lécuyer A. Influence of the visuo-proprioceptive illusion of movement and motor imagery of the wrist on EEG cortical excitability among healthy participants. PLoS One 2021; 16:e0256723. [PMID: 34473788 PMCID: PMC8412266 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0256723] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/30/2020] [Accepted: 08/13/2021] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
INTRODUCTION Motor Imagery (MI) is a powerful tool to stimulate sensorimotor brain areas and is currently used in motor rehabilitation after a stroke. The aim of our study was to evaluate whether an illusion of movement induced by visuo-proprioceptive immersion (VPI) including tendon vibration (TV) and Virtual moving hand (VR) combined with MI tasks could be more efficient than VPI alone or MI alone on cortical excitability assessed using Electroencephalography (EEG). METHODS We recorded EEG signals in 20 healthy participants in 3 different conditions: MI tasks involving their non-dominant wrist (MI condition); VPI condition; and VPI with MI tasks (combined condition). Each condition lasted 3 minutes, and was repeated 3 times in randomized order. Our main judgment criterion was the Event-Related De-synchronization (ERD) threshold in sensori-motor areas in each condition in the brain motor area. RESULTS The combined condition induced a greater change in the ERD percentage than the MI condition alone, but no significant difference was found between the combined and the VPI condition (p = 0.07) and between the VPI and MI condition (p = 0.20). CONCLUSION This study demonstrated the interest of using a visuo-proprioceptive immersion with MI rather than MI alone in order to increase excitability in motor areas of the brain. Further studies could test this hypothesis among patients with stroke to provide new perspectives for motor rehabilitation in this population.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Salomé Le Franc
- Rehabilitation Medicine Unit, University Hospital of Rennes, Rennes, France
- Hybrid Team, Inria, University of Rennes, Irisa, UMR CNRS 6074, Rennes, France
| | - Mathis Fleury
- Hybrid Team, Inria, University of Rennes, Irisa, UMR CNRS 6074, Rennes, France
- Empenn Unit U1228, Inserm, Inria, University of Rennes, Irisa, UMR CNRS 6074, Rennes, France
| | - Camille Jeunet
- CLLE Lab, CNRS, Univ. Toulouse Jean Jaurès, Toulouse, France
| | - Simon Butet
- Rehabilitation Medicine Unit, University Hospital of Rennes, Rennes, France
- Empenn Unit U1228, Inserm, Inria, University of Rennes, Irisa, UMR CNRS 6074, Rennes, France
| | - Christian Barillot
- Empenn Unit U1228, Inserm, Inria, University of Rennes, Irisa, UMR CNRS 6074, Rennes, France
| | - Isabelle Bonan
- Rehabilitation Medicine Unit, University Hospital of Rennes, Rennes, France
- Empenn Unit U1228, Inserm, Inria, University of Rennes, Irisa, UMR CNRS 6074, Rennes, France
| | - Mélanie Cogné
- Rehabilitation Medicine Unit, University Hospital of Rennes, Rennes, France
- Hybrid Team, Inria, University of Rennes, Irisa, UMR CNRS 6074, Rennes, France
| | - Anatole Lécuyer
- Hybrid Team, Inria, University of Rennes, Irisa, UMR CNRS 6074, Rennes, France
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Schneider C, Marquis R, Jöhr J, Lopes da Silva M, Ryvlin P, Serino A, De Lucia M, Diserens K. Disentangling the percepts of illusory movement and sensory stimulation during tendon vibration in the EEG. Neuroimage 2021; 241:118431. [PMID: 34329723 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118431] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/12/2020] [Revised: 07/16/2021] [Accepted: 07/26/2021] [Indexed: 01/10/2023] Open
Abstract
Mechanical vibration of muscle tendons in specific frequencies - termed functional proprioceptive stimulation (FPS) - has the ability to induce the illusion of a movement which is congruent with a lengthening of the vibrated tendon and muscle. The majority of previous reports of the brain correlates of this illusion are based on functional neuroimaging. Contrary to the electroencephalogram (EEG) however, such technologies are not suitable for bedside or ambulant use. While a handful of studies have shown EEG changes during FPS, it remains underinvestigated whether these changes were due to the perceived illusion or the perceived vibration. Here, we aimed at disentangling the neural correlates of the illusory movement from those produced by the vibration sensation by comparing the neural responses to two vibration types, one that did and one that did not elicit an illusion. We recruited 40 naïve participants, 20 for the EEG experiment and 20 for a supporting behavioral study, who received functional tendon co-vibration on the biceps and triceps tendon at their left elbow, pseudo-randomly switching between the illusion and non-illusion trials. Time-frequency decomposition uncovered a strong and lasting event-related desynchronization (ERD) in the mu and beta band in both conditions, suggesting a strong somatosensory response to the vibration. Additionally, the analysis of the evoked potentials revealed a significant difference between the two experimental conditions from 310 to 990ms post stimulus onset. Training classifiers on the frequency-based and voltage-based correlates of illusion perception yielded above chance accuracies for 17 and 13 out of the 20 subjects respectively. Our findings show that FPS-induced illusions produce EEG correlates that are distinct from a vibration-based control and which can be classified reliably in a large number of participants. These results encourage pursuing EEG-based detection of kinesthetic illusions as a tool for clinical use, e.g., to uncover aspects of cognitive perception in unresponsive patients.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Christoph Schneider
- Acute Neurorehabilitation Unit (LRNA), Division of Neurology, Department of Clinical Neurosciences, Centre Hospitalier Universitaire Vaudois (CHUV), Lausanne, Switzerland.
| | - Renaud Marquis
- Acute Neurorehabilitation Unit (LRNA), Division of Neurology, Department of Clinical Neurosciences, Centre Hospitalier Universitaire Vaudois (CHUV), Lausanne, Switzerland
| | - Jane Jöhr
- Acute Neurorehabilitation Unit (LRNA), Division of Neurology, Department of Clinical Neurosciences, Centre Hospitalier Universitaire Vaudois (CHUV), Lausanne, Switzerland; Division of Neurorehabilitation and Neuropsychology, Department of Clinical Neurosciences, Centre Hospitalier Universitaire Vaudois (CHUV), Lausanne, Switzerland
| | - Marina Lopes da Silva
- Acute Neurorehabilitation Unit (LRNA), Division of Neurology, Department of Clinical Neurosciences, Centre Hospitalier Universitaire Vaudois (CHUV), Lausanne, Switzerland
| | - Philippe Ryvlin
- Division of Neurology, Department of Clinical Neurosciences, Centre Hospitalier Universitaire Vaudois (CHUV), Lausanne, Switzerland
| | - Andrea Serino
- MySpace Laboratory, Department of Clinical Neurosciences, Centre Hospitalier Universitaire Vaudois (CHUV), Lausanne, Switzerland
| | - Marzia De Lucia
- Laboratory for Research in Neuroimaging (LREN), Department of Clinical Neurosciences, Centre Hospitalier Universitaire Vaudois (CHUV), Lausanne, Switzerland
| | - Karin Diserens
- Acute Neurorehabilitation Unit (LRNA), Division of Neurology, Department of Clinical Neurosciences, Centre Hospitalier Universitaire Vaudois (CHUV), Lausanne, Switzerland.
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Keri MI, Shehata AW, Marasco PD, Hebert JS, Vette AH. A Cost-Effective Inertial Measurement System for Tracking Movement and Triggering Kinesthetic Feedback in Lower-Limb Prosthesis Users. SENSORS (BASEL, SWITZERLAND) 2021; 21:1844. [PMID: 33800790 PMCID: PMC7961441 DOI: 10.3390/s21051844] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/02/2021] [Revised: 02/28/2021] [Accepted: 03/02/2021] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Advances in lower-limb prosthetic technologies have facilitated the restoration of ambulation; however, users of such technologies still experience reduced balance control, also due to the absence of proprioceptive feedback. Recent efforts have demonstrated the ability to restore kinesthetic feedback in upper-limb prosthesis applications; however, technical solutions to trigger the required muscle vibration and provide automated feedback have not been explored for lower-limb prostheses. The study's first objective was therefore to develop a feedback system capable of tracking lower-limb movement and automatically triggering a muscle vibrator to induce the kinesthetic illusion. The second objective was to investigate the developed system's ability to provide kinesthetic feedback in a case participant. A low-cost, wireless feedback system, incorporating two inertial measurement units to trigger a muscle vibrator, was developed and tested in an individual with limb loss above the knee. Our system had a maximum communication delay of 50 ms and showed good tracking of Gaussian and sinusoidal movement profiles for velocities below 180 degrees per second (error < 8 degrees), mimicking stepping and walking, respectively. We demonstrated in the case participant that the developed feedback system can successfully elicit the kinesthetic illusion. Our work contributes to the integration of sensory feedback in lower-limb prostheses, to increase their use and functionality.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- McNiel-Inyani Keri
- Donadeo Innovation Centre for Engineering, Department of Mechanical Engineering, University of Alberta, 9211 116 Street NW, Edmonton, AB T6G 1H9, Canada;
| | - Ahmed W. Shehata
- Division of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Faculty of Medicine and Dentistry, University of Alberta, 5005 Katz Group Centre, Edmonton, AB T6G 2E1, Canada; (A.W.S.); (J.S.H.)
| | - Paul D. Marasco
- Laboratory for Bionic Integration, Department of Biomedical Engineering, Lerner Research Institute, Cleveland Clinic, 9500 Euclid Avenue, ND20, Cleveland, OH 44195, USA;
- Advanced Platform Technology Center, Louis Stokes Cleveland Department of Veterans Affairs Medical Center, 10701 East Boulevard 151 W/APT, Cleveland, OH 44106, USA
| | - Jacqueline S. Hebert
- Division of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Faculty of Medicine and Dentistry, University of Alberta, 5005 Katz Group Centre, Edmonton, AB T6G 2E1, Canada; (A.W.S.); (J.S.H.)
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, 1098 Research Transition Facility, University of Alberta, Edmonton, AB T6G 2V2, Canada
- Glenrose Rehabilitation Hospital, Alberta Health Services, 10230 111 Avenue NW, Edmonton, AB T5G 0B7, Canada
| | - Albert H. Vette
- Donadeo Innovation Centre for Engineering, Department of Mechanical Engineering, University of Alberta, 9211 116 Street NW, Edmonton, AB T6G 1H9, Canada;
- Glenrose Rehabilitation Hospital, Alberta Health Services, 10230 111 Avenue NW, Edmonton, AB T5G 0B7, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Hagimori D, Isoyama N, Yoshimoto S, Sakata N, Kiyokawa K. Tendon vibration changes perceived joint angle independent of voluntary body motion direction in virtual environments. Adv Robot 2020. [DOI: 10.1080/01691864.2020.1852959] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Daiki Hagimori
- Division of Information Science, Nara Institute of Science and Technology, Nara, Japan
| | - Naoya Isoyama
- Division of Information Science, Nara Institute of Science and Technology, Nara, Japan
| | - Shunsuke Yoshimoto
- Graduate School of Frontier Sciences, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan
| | - Nobuchika Sakata
- Division of Information Science, Nara Institute of Science and Technology, Nara, Japan
| | - Kiyoshi Kiyokawa
- Division of Information Science, Nara Institute of Science and Technology, Nara, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Le Franc S, Fleury M, Cogne M, Butet S, Barillot C, Lecuyer A, Bonan I. Influence of virtual reality visual feedback on the illusion of movement induced by tendon vibration of wrist in healthy participants. PLoS One 2020; 15:e0242416. [PMID: 33216756 PMCID: PMC7678999 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0242416] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/12/2020] [Accepted: 11/02/2020] [Indexed: 12/16/2022] Open
Abstract
Introduction Illusion of movement induced by tendon vibration is an effective approach for motor and sensory rehabilitation in case of neurological impairments. The aim of our study was to investigate which modality of visual feedback in Virtual Reality (VR) associated with tendon vibration of the wrist could induce the best illusion of movement. Methods We included 30 healthy participants in the experiment. Tendon vibration inducing illusion of movement (wrist extension, 100Hz) was applied on their wrist during 3 VR visual conditions (10 times each): a moving virtual hand corresponding to the movement that the participants could feel during the tendon vibration (Moving condition), a static virtual hand (Static condition), or no virtual hand at all (Hidden condition). After each trial, the participants had to quantify the intensity of the illusory movement on a Likert scale, the subjective degree of extension of their wrist and afterwards they answered a questionnaire. Results There was a significant difference between the 3 visual feedback conditions concerning the Likert scale ranking and the degree of wrist’s extension (p<0.001). The Moving condition induced a higher intensity of illusion of movement and a higher sensation of wrist’s extension than the Hidden condition (p<0.001 and p<0.001 respectively) than that of the Static condition (p<0.001 and p<0.001 respectively). The Hidden condition also induced a higher intensity of illusion of movement and a higher sensation of wrist’s extension than the Static condition (p<0.01 and p<0.01 respectively). The preferred condition to facilitate movement’s illusion was the Moving condition (63.3%). Conclusions This study demonstrated the importance of carefully selecting a visual feedback to improve the illusion of movement induced by tendon vibration, and the increase of illusion by adding VR visual cues congruent to the illusion of movement. Further work will consist in testing the same hypothesis with stroke patients.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Salomé Le Franc
- Rehabilitation Medicine Unit, University Hospital of Rennes, Rennes, France
- * E-mail:
| | - Mathis Fleury
- Inria, Rennes, France
- Empenn Unity U1228, Inserm, Inria, University of Rennes, Irisa, Umr Cnrs 6074, Rennes, France
| | - Mélanie Cogne
- Rehabilitation Medicine Unit, University Hospital of Rennes, Rennes, France
| | - Simon Butet
- Rehabilitation Medicine Unit, University Hospital of Rennes, Rennes, France
| | - Christian Barillot
- Empenn Unity U1228, Inserm, Inria, University of Rennes, Irisa, Umr Cnrs 6074, Rennes, France
| | | | - Isabelle Bonan
- Rehabilitation Medicine Unit, University Hospital of Rennes, Rennes, France
- Empenn Unity U1228, Inserm, Inria, University of Rennes, Irisa, Umr Cnrs 6074, Rennes, France
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Pinardi M, Ferrari F, D’Alonzo M, Clemente F, Raiano L, Cipriani C, Di Pino G. ‘Doublecheck: a sensory confirmation is required to own a robotic hand, sending a command to feel in charge of it’. Cogn Neurosci 2020; 11:216-228. [DOI: 10.1080/17588928.2020.1793751] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/12/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- M. Pinardi
- NeXT: Neurophysiology and Neuroengineering of Human-Technology Interaction Research Unit, Campus Bio-Medico University, Rome, Italy
| | - F. Ferrari
- The BioRobotics Institute, Scuola Superiore Sant’Anna, Pisa, Italy
- Department of Excellence in Robotics & A.I., Scuola Superiore Sant’Anna, Pisa, Italy
| | - M. D’Alonzo
- NeXT: Neurophysiology and Neuroengineering of Human-Technology Interaction Research Unit, Campus Bio-Medico University, Rome, Italy
| | - F. Clemente
- The BioRobotics Institute, Scuola Superiore Sant’Anna, Pisa, Italy
- Department of Excellence in Robotics & A.I., Scuola Superiore Sant’Anna, Pisa, Italy
| | - L. Raiano
- NeXT: Neurophysiology and Neuroengineering of Human-Technology Interaction Research Unit, Campus Bio-Medico University, Rome, Italy
| | - C. Cipriani
- The BioRobotics Institute, Scuola Superiore Sant’Anna, Pisa, Italy
- Department of Excellence in Robotics & A.I., Scuola Superiore Sant’Anna, Pisa, Italy
| | - G. Di Pino
- NeXT: Neurophysiology and Neuroengineering of Human-Technology Interaction Research Unit, Campus Bio-Medico University, Rome, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Visual feedback from a virtual body modulates motor illusion induced by tendon vibration. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2020; 85:926-938. [DOI: 10.1007/s00426-020-01366-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/27/2019] [Accepted: 05/22/2020] [Indexed: 12/19/2022]
|
18
|
Glazebrook CM, Brown K, Prime SL, Passmore SR, Marotta JJ. Both reaching and grasping are impacted by temporarily induced paresthesia. Somatosens Mot Res 2020; 37:106-116. [PMID: 32312126 DOI: 10.1080/08990220.2020.1750359] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
Abstract
Along with visual feedback, somatosensory feedback provides the nervous system with information regarding movement performance. Somatosensory system damage disrupts the normal feedback process, which can lead to a pins and needles sensation, or paresthaesia, and impaired movement control. The present study assessed the impact of temporarily induced median nerve paresthaesia, in individuals with otherwise intact sensorimotor function, on goal-directed reaching and grasping movements. Healthy, right-handed participants performed reach and grasp movements to five wooden Efron shapes, of which three were selected for analysis. Participants performed the task without online visual feedback and in two somatosensory conditions: 1) normal; and 2) disrupted somatosensory feedback. Disrupted somatosensory feedback was induced temporarily using a Digitimer (DS7AH) constant current stimulator. Participants' movements to shapes 15 or 30 cm to the right of the hand's start position were recorded using a 3 D motion analysis system at 300 Hz (Optotrak 3 D Investigator). Analyses revealed no significant differences for reaction time. Main effects for paresthaesia were observed for temporal and spatial aspects of the both the reach and grasp components of the movements. Although participants scaled their grip aperture to shape size under paresthaesia, the movements were smaller and more variable. Overall participants behaved as though they perceived they were performing larger and faster movements than they actually were. We suggest the presence of temporally induced paresthaesia affected online control by disrupting somatosensory feedback of the reach and grasp movements, ultimately leading to smaller forces and fewer corrective movements.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Cheryl M Glazebrook
- Perceptual Motor Integration Lab, Faculty of Kinesiology and Recreation Management, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg, MB, Canada
| | - Kelsey Brown
- Perceptual Motor Integration Lab, Faculty of Kinesiology and Recreation Management, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg, MB, Canada
| | - Steven L Prime
- Neurocognition and Psychophysics Laboratory, Department of Psychology, University of Saskatchewan, Saskatoon, SK, Canada.,Perception and Action Lab, Department of Psychology, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg, MB, Canada
| | - Steven R Passmore
- Perceptual Motor Behaviour Lab, Faculty of Kinesiology and Recreation Management, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg, MB, Canada
| | - Jonathan J Marotta
- Perception and Action Lab, Department of Psychology, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg, MB, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Beaulieu LD, Schneider C, Massé-Alarie H, Ribot-Ciscar E. A new method to elicit and measure movement illusions in stroke by means of muscle tendon vibration: the Standardized Kinesthetic Illusion Procedure (SKIP). Somatosens Mot Res 2020; 37:28-36. [PMID: 31973656 DOI: 10.1080/08990220.2020.1713739] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
Abstract
Purpose: Muscle tendon vibration (MTV) strongly activates muscle spindles and can evoke kinaesthetic illusions. Although potentially relevant for sensorimotor rehabilitation in stroke, MTV is scarcely used in clinical practice, likely because of the absence of standardised procedures to elicit and characterise movement illusions. This work developed and validated a Standardised Kinaesthetic Illusion Procedure (SKIP) to favour the use of MTV-induced illusions in clinical settings.Materials and methods: SKIP scores were obtained in 15 individuals with chronic stroke and 18 age- and gender-matched healthy counterparts. A further 13 healthy subjects were tested to provide more data with the general population. MTV was applied over the Achilles tendon and SKIP scoring system characterised the clearness and direction of the illusions of ankle dorsiflexion movements.Results: All healthy and stroke participants perceived movement illusions. SKIP scores on the paretic side were significantly lower compared to the non paretic and healthy. Illusions were less clear and sometimes in unexpected directions with the impaired ankle, but still possible to elicit in the presence of sensorimotor deficits.Conclusions: SKIP represents an ancillary and potentially useful clinical method to elicit and characterise illusions of movements induced by MTV. SKIP could be relevant to further assess the processing of proprioceptive afferents in stroke and their potential impact on motor control and recovery. It may be used to guide therapy and improve sensorimotor recovery. Future work is needed to investigate the metrological properties of our method (reliability, responsiveness, etc.), and also the neurophysiological underpinnings of MTV-induced illusions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Louis-David Beaulieu
- Biomechanical and Neurophysiological Research Lab in neuro-musculo-skelettal Rehabilitation (BioNR Lab, Université du Québec à Chicoutimi, Chicoutimi, Canada
| | - Cyril Schneider
- Noninvasive Stimulation Laboratory, Research Center - Neuroscience Division and Department Rehabilitation, CHU de Québec-Université Laval, Quebec City, Canada
| | - Hugo Massé-Alarie
- Centre interdisciplinaire de recherche en réadaptation et intégration sociale, Université Laval, Quebec City, Canada
| | - Edith Ribot-Ciscar
- Laboratoire de Neurosciences Sensorielles et Cognitives, Aix Marseille Univ, CNRS, LNSC, Marseille, France
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Shehata AW, Keri MI, Gomez M, Marasco PD, Vette AH, Hebert JS. Skin Stretch Enhances Illusory Movement in Persons with Lower-Limb Amputation. IEEE Int Conf Rehabil Robot 2020; 2019:1233-1238. [PMID: 31374798 DOI: 10.1109/icorr.2019.8779477] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
Abstract
Performance of lower limb prostheses is related not only to the mechanical design and the control scheme, but also to the feedback provided to the user. Proprioceptive feedback, which is the sense of position and movement of one's body parts, can improve the utility as well as facilitate the embodiment of the prosthetic device. Recent studies have shown that proprioceptive kinesthetic (movement) sense can be elicited when non-invasively vibrating a muscle tendon proximal to the targeted joint. However, consistency and quality of the elicited sensation depend on several parameters and muscle tendons after lower limb amputation may not always be accessible. In this study, we developed an experimental protocol to quantitatively and qualitatively assess the elicited proprioceptive kinesthetic illusion when non-invasively vibrating a muscle belly. Furthermore, we explored ways to improve consistency and strength of the illusion by integrating another non-invasive feedback method, namely cutaneous information manipulation via skin stretch. Our preliminary results from tests conducted with a person with transtibial (below knee) amputation show that stretching skin while vibrating a muscle belly on the residual limb provided a stronger and more consistent kinesthetic illusion (90%) than only vibrating the muscle (50%). In addition, we found that stretching skin enhances the range (1.5 times) and speed (3.5 times) of the illusory movement triggered by muscle vibration. These findings may enable the development of mechanisms for controlling feedback parameters in closing the control loop for various walking routines, which may improve performance of lower limb prostheses.
Collapse
|
21
|
Dempsey-Jones H, Kritikos A. Handedness modulates proprioceptive drift in the rubber hand illusion. Exp Brain Res 2018; 237:351-361. [PMID: 30411222 PMCID: PMC6373180 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-018-5391-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/26/2017] [Accepted: 10/03/2018] [Indexed: 12/03/2022]
Abstract
Preference for use of either the left or right hand (‘handedness’) has been linked with modulations of perception and sensory processing—both of space and the body. Here we ask whether multisensory integration of bodily information also varies as a function of handedness. We created a spatial disparity between visual and somatosensory hand position information using the rubber hand illusion, and use the magnitude of illusory shifts in hand position (proprioceptive ‘drift’) as a tool to probe the weighted integration of multisensory information. First, we found drift was significantly reduced when the illusion was performed on the dominant vs. non-dominant hand. We suggest increased manual dexterity of the dominant hand causes greater representational stability and thus an increased resistance to bias by the illusion induction. Second, drift was generally greatest when the hand was in its habitual action space (i.e., near the shoulder of origin), compared to when it laterally displaced towards, or across the midline. This linear effect, however, was only significant for the dominant hand—in both left- and right-handed groups. Thus, our results reveal patterns of habitual hand action modulate drift both within a hand (drift varies with proximity to action space), and between hands (differences in drift between the dominant and non-dominant hands). In contrast, we were unable to find conclusive evidence to support, or contradict, an overall difference between left- and right-handers in susceptibility to RHI drift (i.e., total drift, collapsed across hand positions). In sum, our results provide evidence that patterns of daily activity—and the subsequent patterns of sensory input—shape multisensory integration across space.
Collapse
|
22
|
Ferrari F, Clemente F, Cipriani C. The preload force affects the perception threshold of muscle vibration-induced movement illusions. Exp Brain Res 2018; 237:111-120. [PMID: 30341466 PMCID: PMC6514251 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-018-5402-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/27/2018] [Accepted: 10/12/2018] [Indexed: 11/12/2022]
Abstract
The control and the execution of motor tasks are largely influenced by proprioceptive feedback, i.e. the information about the position and movement of the body. In 1972, it was discovered that a vibratory stimulation applied non-invasively to a muscle or a tendon induces a movement illusion consistent with the elongation of the vibrated muscle/tendon. Although this phenomenon was reported by several studies, it is still unclear how to reliably reproduce it because of the many different features of the stimulation altering the sensation (e.g. frequency, duration, location). By performing a psychophysical test, we analysed the effects of the stimulation point and the preload force on the minimum stimulation amplitude needed to elicit an illusion of movement. In particular, we stimulated two groups of healthy subjects on three target regions of the biceps brachii muscle (the distal tendon, the muscle belly and one of the proximal tendons) applying three preload force ranges (0.5–0.75N, 1–2N and 3–4N). Our results showed that the minimum stimulation amplitude eliciting a sensation is affected by the preload force. On the contrary, it did not change significantly among the three stimulated regions. Nevertheless, the reported vividness of the illusion of movement changed across the stimulated points decreasing while moving from the distal to the proximal tendons. Overall, these outcomes contribute to the scientific debate on the features that modulate the vibration-induced movement illusion proposing ways to increase the reliability of the procedure in basic and applied research studies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Francesca Ferrari
- The BioRobotics Institute, Scuola Superiore Sant'Anna, Viale Rinaldo Piaggio, 34, 56025, Pontedera, PI, Italy.
| | - Francesco Clemente
- The BioRobotics Institute, Scuola Superiore Sant'Anna, Viale Rinaldo Piaggio, 34, 56025, Pontedera, PI, Italy
| | - Christian Cipriani
- The BioRobotics Institute, Scuola Superiore Sant'Anna, Viale Rinaldo Piaggio, 34, 56025, Pontedera, PI, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Leib R, Rubin I, Nisky I. Force feedback delay affects perception of stiffness but not action, and the effect depends on the hand used but not on the handedness. J Neurophysiol 2018; 120:781-794. [PMID: 29766763 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00822.2017] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Interaction with an object often requires the estimation of its mechanical properties. We examined whether the hand that is used to interact with the object and their handedness affected people's estimation of these properties using stiffness estimation as a test case. We recorded participants' responses on a stiffness discrimination of a virtual elastic force field and the grip force applied on the robotic device during the interaction. In half of the trials, the robotic device delayed the participants' force feedback. Consistent with previous studies, delayed force feedback biased the perceived stiffness of the force field. Interestingly, in both left-handed and right-handed participants, for the delayed force field, there was even less perceived stiffness when participants used their left hand than their right hand. This result supports the idea that haptic processing is affected by laterality in the brain, not by handedness. Consistent with previous studies, participants adjusted their applied grip force according to the correct size and timing of the load force regardless of the hand that was used, the handedness, or the delay. This suggests that in all of these conditions, participants were able to form an accurate internal representation of the anticipated trajectory of the load force (size and timing) and that this representation was used for accurate control of grip force independently of the perceptual bias. Thus these results provide additional evidence for the dissociation between action and perception in the processing of delayed information. NEW & NOTEWORTHY Introducing delay to force feedback during interaction with an elastic force field biases the perceived stiffness of the force field. We show that this bias depends on the hand that was used for probing but not on handedness. At the same time, both left-handed and right-handed participants adjusted their applied grip force while using either their left or right hands in anticipation of the correct magnitude and timing despite the delay in load force.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Raz Leib
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beersheba, Israel.,Zlotowski Center for Neuroscience, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beersheba, Israel
| | - Inbar Rubin
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beersheba, Israel
| | - Ilana Nisky
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beersheba, Israel.,Zlotowski Center for Neuroscience, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beersheba, Israel
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Effects of wrist tendon vibration and eye movements on manual aiming. Exp Brain Res 2018; 236:847-857. [PMID: 29353311 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-018-5180-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/18/2017] [Accepted: 01/12/2018] [Indexed: 12/19/2022]
Abstract
In the present study, we investigated whether visual information mediates a proprioceptive illusion effect induced by muscle tendon vibration in manual aiming. Visual information was gradually degraded from a situation in which the targets were present and participants (n = 20; 22.3 ± 2.7 years) were permitted to make saccadic eye movements to designated target positions, to a condition in which the targets were not visible and participants were required to perform cyclical aiming while fixating a point between the two target positions. Local tendon vibration applied to the right wrist extensor muscles induced an illusory reduction of 15% in hand movement amplitude. This effect was greater in the fixation than in the saccade condition. Both anticipatory control and proprioceptive feedback are proposed to contribute to the observed effects. The primary saccade amplitude was also reduced by almost 4% when muscle tendon vibration was locally applied to the wrist. These results confirm a tight link between eye movements and manual perception and action. Moreover, the impact of the proprioceptive illusion on the ocular system indicates that the interaction between systems is bidirectional.
Collapse
|
25
|
Barsotti M, Leonardis D, Vanello N, Bergamasco M, Frisoli A. Effects of Continuous Kinaesthetic Feedback Based on Tendon Vibration on Motor Imagery BCI Performance. IEEE Trans Neural Syst Rehabil Eng 2018; 26:105-114. [DOI: 10.1109/tnsre.2017.2739244] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
|
26
|
Caola B, Montalti M, Zanini A, Leadbetter A, Martini M. The Bodily Illusion in Adverse Conditions: Virtual Arm Ownership During Visuomotor Mismatch. Perception 2018; 47:301006618758211. [PMID: 29471714 DOI: 10.1177/0301006618758211] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/28/2024]
Abstract
Classically, body ownership illusions are triggered by cross-modal synchronous stimulations, and hampered by multisensory inconsistencies. Nonetheless, the boundaries of such illusions have been proven to be highly plastic. In this immersive virtual reality study, we explored whether it is possible to induce a sense of body ownership over a virtual body part during visuomotor inconsistencies, with or without the aid of concomitant visuo-tactile stimulations. From a first-person perspective, participants watched a virtual tube moving or an avatar's arm moving, with or without concomitant synchronous visuo-tactile stimulations on their hand. Three different virtual arm/tube speeds were also investigated, while all participants kept their real arms still. The subjective reports show that synchronous visuo-tactile stimulations effectively counteract the effect of visuomotor inconsistencies, but at slow arm movements, a feeling of body ownership might be successfully induced even without concomitant multisensory correspondences. Possible therapeutical implications of these findings are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Barbara Caola
- School of Psychology, 117128 University of East London , London, UK
| | - Martina Montalti
- School of Psychology, 117128 University of East London , London, UK
| | | | | | - Matteo Martini
- School of Psychology, 117128 University of East London , London, UK
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Tidoni E, Abu-Alqumsan M, Leonardis D, Kapeller C, Fusco G, Guger C, Hintermuller C, Peer A, Frisoli A, Tecchia F, Bergamasco M, Aglioti SM. Local and Remote Cooperation With Virtual and Robotic Agents: A P300 BCI Study in Healthy and People Living With Spinal Cord Injury. IEEE Trans Neural Syst Rehabil Eng 2017; 25:1622-1632. [DOI: 10.1109/tnsre.2016.2626391] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
|
28
|
Taylor MW, Taylor JL, Seizova-Cajic T. Muscle Vibration-Induced Illusions: Review of Contributing Factors, Taxonomy of Illusions and User’s Guide. Multisens Res 2017. [DOI: 10.1163/22134808-00002544] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
Limb muscle vibration creates an illusory limb movement in the direction corresponding to lengthening of the vibrated muscle. Neck muscle vibration results in illusory motion of visual and auditory stimuli. Attributed to the activation of muscle spindles, these and related effects are of great interest as a tool in research on proprioception, for rehabilitation of sensorimotor function and for multisensory immersive virtual environments. However, these illusions are not easy to elicit in a consistent manner. We review factors that influence them, propose their classification in a scheme that links this area of research to perception theory, and provide practical suggestions to researchers. Local factors that determine the illusory effect of vibration include properties of the vibration stimulus such as its frequency, amplitude and duration, and properties of the vibrated muscle, such as contraction and fatigue. Contextual (gestalt) factors concern the relationship of the vibrated body part to the rest of the body and the environment. Tactile and visual cues play an important role, and so does movement, imagined or real. The best-known vibration illusions concern one’s own body and can be classified as ‘first-order’ due to a direct link between activity in muscle spindles and the percept. More complex illusions involve other sensory modalities and external objects, and provide important clues regarding the hidden role of proprioception, our ‘silent’ sense. Our taxonomy makes explicit this and other distinctions between different illusory effects. We include User’s Guide with tips for anyone wishing to conduct a vibration study.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mitchell W. Taylor
- Faculty of Health Sciences, University of Sydney, 75 East St, Lidcombe 2141 NSW, Sydney, Australia
| | - Janet L. Taylor
- Neuroscience Research Australia, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia
| | - Tatjana Seizova-Cajic
- Faculty of Health Sciences, University of Sydney, 75 East St, Lidcombe 2141 NSW, Sydney, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
29
|
|
30
|
Fusco G, Tidoni E, Barone N, Pilati C, Aglioti SM. Illusion of arm movement evoked by tendon vibration in patients with spinal cord injury. Restor Neurol Neurosci 2016; 34:815-26. [DOI: 10.3233/rnn-160660] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Gabriele Fusco
- Department of Psychology, University of Rome “Sapienza”, Rome, Italy
- IRCCS Fondazione Santa Lucia, IRCCS, Rome, Italy
| | - Emmanuele Tidoni
- Department of Psychology, University of Rome “Sapienza”, Rome, Italy
- IRCCS Fondazione Santa Lucia, IRCCS, Rome, Italy
| | - Nicola Barone
- Hospital A.Alesini C.T.O., Unipolar Unit, Rome, Italy
| | | | - Salvatore Maria Aglioti
- Department of Psychology, University of Rome “Sapienza”, Rome, Italy
- IRCCS Fondazione Santa Lucia, IRCCS, Rome, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
31
|
First Person Perspective of Seated Participants Over a Walking Virtual Body Leads to Illusory Agency Over the Walking. Sci Rep 2016; 6:28879. [PMID: 27364767 PMCID: PMC4929480 DOI: 10.1038/srep28879] [Citation(s) in RCA: 71] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/13/2015] [Accepted: 06/08/2016] [Indexed: 11/08/2022] Open
Abstract
Agency, the attribution of authorship to an action of our body, requires the intention to carry out the action, and subsequently a match between its predicted and actual sensory consequences. However, illusory agency can be generated through priming of the action together with perception of bodily action, even when there has been no actual corresponding action. Here we show that participants can have the illusion of agency over the walking of a virtual body even though in reality they are seated and only allowed head movements. The experiment (n = 28) had two factors: Perspective (1PP or 3PP) and Head Sway (Sway or NoSway). Participants in 1PP saw a life-sized virtual body spatially coincident with their own from a first person perspective, or the virtual body from third person perspective (3PP). In the Sway condition the viewpoint included a walking animation, but not in NoSway. The results show strong illusions of body ownership, agency and walking, in the 1PP compared to the 3PP condition, and an enhanced level of arousal while the walking was up a virtual hill. Sway reduced the level of agency. We conclude with a discussion of the results in the light of current theories of agency.
Collapse
|
32
|
Abstract
The tendon vibration illusion has been extensively used to manipulate the perceived position of one's own body part. However, findings from previous research do not seem conclusive sregarding the perceptual effect of the concurrent stimulation of both agonist and antagonist tendons over one joint. On the basis of recent data, it has been suggested that this paired stimulation generates an inconsistent signal about the limb position, which leads to a perceived shrinkage of the limb. However, this interesting effect has never been replicated. The aim of the present study was to clarify the effect of a simultaneous and equal vibration of the biceps and triceps tendons on the perceived location of the hand. Experiment 1 replicated and extended the previous findings. We compared a dual tendon stimulation condition with single tendon stimulation conditions and with a control condition (no vibration) on both 'upward-downward' and 'towards-away from the elbow' planes. Our results show a mislocalisation towards the elbow of the position of the vibrated arm during dual vibration, in line with previous results; however, this did not clarify whether the effect was due to arm representation contraction (i.e., a 'telescoping' effect). Therefore, in Experiment 2 we investigated explicitly and implicitly the perceived arm length during the same conditions. Our results clearly suggest that in all the vibration conditions there was a mislocalisation of the entire arm (including the elbow), but no evidence of a contraction of the perceived arm length.
Collapse
|
33
|
Tsay AJ, Giummarra MJ, Allen TJ, Proske U. The sensory origins of human position sense. J Physiol 2016; 594:1037-49. [PMID: 26537335 PMCID: PMC4753260 DOI: 10.1113/jp271498] [Citation(s) in RCA: 31] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/27/2015] [Accepted: 10/19/2015] [Indexed: 11/08/2022] Open
Abstract
KEY POINTS Position sense at the human forearm can be measured in blindfolded subjects by matching positions of the arms or by a subject pointing to the perceived position of an unseen arm. Effects on position sense tested were: elbow muscle conditioning with a voluntary contraction, muscle vibration, loading the arm and elbow skin stretch. Conditioning contractions and vibration produced errors in a matching task, consistent with the action of muscle spindles as position sensors. Position errors in a pointing task were not consistent with the action of muscle spindles. Loading the arm or skin stretch had no effect in either matching or pointing tasks. It is proposed that there are two kinds of position sense: (i) indicating positions of different body parts relative to one another, using signals from muscle spindles; and (ii) indicating position of the body in extrapersonal space, using signals from exteroceptors, vision, touch and hearing. ABSTRACT Human limb position sense can be measured in two ways: in a blindfolded matching task, position of one limb is indicated with the other limb. Alternatively, position of a limb, hidden from view, is indicated with a pointer, moved by pressing a lever. These experiments examined the sensory basis of position sense measured in these two ways. Position errors were measured in 14 subjects after elbow flexors or extensors had been conditioned with a half-maximum voluntary contraction. In agreement with previous studies, in the matching trials, position errors were distributed according to a pattern consistent with the action of muscle spindles as the position sensors. In the pointing trials, all errors lay in the direction of extension of the true position of the hidden arm and their distribution was inconsistent with influences arising in muscle spindles. Vibration of elbow muscles produced an illusion of muscle lengthening during a matching task, while during the pointing task no illusion was present. Finally, the matching-pointing error difference was preserved, even when one arm was loaded with a weight or skin over the elbow was stretched. It is proposed that there are two kinds of position sense. One is signalled by muscle spindles, indicating position of one part of the body relative to another. A second provides information about the position of the body in extrapersonal space and here we hypothesise that exteroceptors, including vision, touch and hearing, acting via a central map of the body, provide the spatial information.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- A. J. Tsay
- School of Psychological Sciences and Monash Institute of Cognitive and Clinical NeurosciencesMonash UniversityClaytonVic3800Australia
- Caulfield Pain Management and Research CentreCaulfield HospitalCaulfieldVic3162Australia
| | - M. J. Giummarra
- School of Psychological Sciences and Monash Institute of Cognitive and Clinical NeurosciencesMonash UniversityClaytonVic3800Australia
- Caulfield Pain Management and Research CentreCaulfield HospitalCaulfieldVic3162Australia
| | - T. J. Allen
- Accident Research CentreMonash Injury Research InstituteClaytonVic3800Australia
| | - U. Proske
- Department of PhysiologyMonash UniversityClaytonVic3800Australia
| |
Collapse
|
34
|
Tieri G, Tidoni E, Pavone EF, Aglioti SM. Body visual discontinuity affects feeling of ownership and skin conductance responses. Sci Rep 2015; 5:17139. [PMID: 26602036 PMCID: PMC4658534 DOI: 10.1038/srep17139] [Citation(s) in RCA: 53] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/18/2015] [Accepted: 10/26/2015] [Indexed: 11/17/2022] Open
Abstract
When we look at our hands we are immediately aware that they belong to us and we rarely doubt about the integrity, continuity and sense of ownership of our bodies. Here we explored whether the mere manipulation of the visual appearance of a virtual limb could influence the subjective feeling of ownership and the physiological responses (Skin Conductance Responses, SCRs) associated to a threatening stimulus approaching the virtual hand. Participants observed in first person perspective a virtual body having the right hand-forearm (i) connected by a normal wrist (Full-Limb) or a thin rigid wire connection (Wire) or (ii) disconnected because of a missing wrist (m-Wrist) or a missing wrist plus a plexiglass panel positioned between the hand and the forearm (Plexiglass). While the analysis of subjective ratings revealed that only the observation of natural full connected virtual limb elicited high levels of ownership, high amplitudes of SCRs were found also during observation of the non-natural, rigid wire connection condition. This result suggests that the conscious embodiment of an artificial limb requires a natural looking visual body appearance while implicit reactivity to threat may require physical body continuity, even non-naturally looking, that allows the implementation of protective reactions to threat.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gaetano Tieri
- Fondazione Santa Lucia, IRCCS, Rome, Italy
- Braintrends ltd, Applied Neuroscience, Rome, Italy
| | - Emmanuele Tidoni
- Fondazione Santa Lucia, IRCCS, Rome, Italy
- Department of Psychology, University of Rome “La Sapienza”, Rome, Italy
| | | | - Salvatore Maria Aglioti
- Fondazione Santa Lucia, IRCCS, Rome, Italy
- Department of Psychology, University of Rome “La Sapienza”, Rome, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
35
|
Tidoni E, Tieri G, Aglioti SM. Re-establishing the disrupted sensorimotor loop in deafferented and deefferented people: The case of spinal cord injuries. Neuropsychologia 2015; 79:301-9. [PMID: 26115603 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.06.029] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/03/2015] [Revised: 06/15/2015] [Accepted: 06/21/2015] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
Acting efficiently in the world depends on the activity of motor and somatosensory systems, the integration of which is necessary for the proper functioning of the sensorimotor loop (SL). Profound alterations of SL functioning follow spinal cord injury (SCI), a condition that brings about a disconnection of the body from the brain. Such disconnection creates a substantial deprivation of somatosensorial inputs and motor outputs. Consequent somatic deficits and motor paralysis affect the body below the lesion level. A complete restoration of normal functions of the SL cannot be expected until basic neuroscience has found a way to re-establish the interrupted neural connectivity. Meanwhile, studies should focus on the development of technical solutions for dealing with the disruption of the sensorimotor loop. This review discusses the structural and functional adaptive reorganization of the brain after SCI, and the maladaptive mechanisms that impact on the processing of body related information, which alter motor imagery strategies and EEG signals. Studies that show how residual functions (e.g. face tactile sensitivity) may help people to restore a normal body image are also reviewed. Finally, data on how brain and residual body signals may be used to improve brain computer interface systems is discussed in relation to the issue of how such systems may help SCI people to re-enter the world and interact with objects and other individuals.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- E Tidoni
- Department of Psychology, University of Rome "La Sapienza", Rome, Italy; Fondazione Santa Lucia, IRCCS, Rome, Italy.
| | - G Tieri
- Fondazione Santa Lucia, IRCCS, Rome, Italy; Braintrends Ltd, Applied Neuroscience, Rome, Italy
| | - S M Aglioti
- Department of Psychology, University of Rome "La Sapienza", Rome, Italy; Fondazione Santa Lucia, IRCCS, Rome, Italy.
| |
Collapse
|