1
|
Hipólito I, van Es T. Enactive-Dynamic Social Cognition and Active Inference. Front Psychol 2022; 13:855074. [PMID: 35572328 PMCID: PMC9102990 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.855074] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/14/2022] [Accepted: 03/16/2022] [Indexed: 11/17/2022] Open
Abstract
This aim of this paper is two-fold: it critically analyses and rejects accounts blending active inference as theory of mind and enactivism; and it advances an enactivist-dynamic understanding of social cognition that is compatible with active inference. While some social cognition theories seemingly take an enactive perspective on social cognition, they explain it as the attribution of mental states to other people, by assuming representational structures, in line with the classic Theory of Mind (ToM). Holding both enactivism and ToM, we argue, entails contradiction and confusion due to two ToM assumptions widely known to be rejected by enactivism: that (1) social cognition reduces to mental representation and (2) social cognition is a hardwired contentful 'toolkit' or 'starter pack' that fuels the model-like theorising supposed in (1). The paper offers a positive alternative, one that avoids contradictions or confusion. After rejecting ToM-inspired theories of social cognition and clarifying the profile of social cognition under enactivism, that is without assumptions (1) and (2), the last section advances an enactivist-dynamic model of cognition as dynamic, real-time, fluid, contextual social action, where we use the formalisms of dynamical systems theory to explain the origins of socio-cognitive novelty in developmental change and active inference as a tool to demonstrate social understanding as generalised synchronisation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Inês Hipólito
- Berlin School of Mind and Brain, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
- Department of Psychology, Amsterdam Brain and Cognition, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Netherlands
| | - Thomas van Es
- Centre for Philosophical Psychology, Universiteit Antwerpen, Antwerp, Belgium
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Burnside K, Neumann C, Poulin-Dubois D. Infants Generalize Beliefs Across Individuals. Front Psychol 2020; 11:547680. [PMID: 33071864 PMCID: PMC7536113 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.547680] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/31/2020] [Accepted: 08/18/2020] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
It has been argued that infants possess a rich, sophisticated theory of mind (ToM) that is only revealed with tasks based on spontaneous responses. A mature (ToM) implies the understanding that mental states are person specific. Previous studies on infants' understanding of motivational mental states, such as goals and preferences have revealed that, by 9 months of age, infants do not generalize these motivational mental states across agents. However, it remains to be determined if infants also perceive epistemic states as person specific. Therefore, the goal of the present study was to use a switch agent paradigm with the classic false belief violation-of-expectation task. Results revealed that 16-month-old infants attributed true and false beliefs to a naïve agent - they did not perceive beliefs as person specific. These findings indicate that the mechanisms that underlie infants' implicit attribution of beliefs differ from those assumed for explicit reasoning about beliefs.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kimberly Burnside
- Department of Psychology, Concordia University, Montréal, QC, Canada
| | - Cassandra Neumann
- Department of Psychology, Concordia University, Montréal, QC, Canada
| | | |
Collapse
|
3
|
Strasser A. In-between implicit and explicit. PHILOSOPHICAL PSYCHOLOGY 2020. [DOI: 10.1080/09515089.2020.1778163] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Anna Strasser
- Berlin School of Mind and Brain, Humboldt-Universitat zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Investigating real-time social interaction in pairs of adolescents with the Perceptual Crossing Experiment. Behav Res Methods 2020; 52:1929-1938. [PMID: 32077080 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-020-01378-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
The study of real-time social interaction provides ecologically valid insight into social behavior. The objective of the current research is to experimentally assess real-time social contingency detection in an adolescent population, using a shortened version of the Perceptual Crossing Experiment (PCE). Pairs of 148 adolescents aged between 12 and 19 were instructed to find each other in a virtual environment interspersed with other objects by interacting with each other using tactile feedback only. Across six rounds, participants demonstrated increasing accuracy in social contingency detection, which was associated with increasing subjective experience of the mutual interaction. Subjective experience was highest in rounds when both participants were simultaneously accurate in detecting each other's presence. The six-round version yielded comparable social contingency detection outcome measures to a ten-round version of the task. The shortened six-round version of the PCE has therefore enabled us to extend the previous findings on social contingency detection in adults to an adolescent population, enabling implementation in prospective research designs to assess the development of social contingency detection over time.
Collapse
|
5
|
Jiménez L, Menéndez S, Hidalgo V. Factor Analysis of EMA-Scale on Adolescent Adjustment From a Developmental Perspective: A Short Form. Front Psychol 2018; 9:2406. [PMID: 30559698 PMCID: PMC6286973 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02406] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/11/2018] [Accepted: 11/15/2018] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Many published instruments for assessing adolescent adjustment can be implemented in the school context. However, most of them fail to include a comprehensive and positive theoretical perspective of adolescent development and, even when they do, priority is often given to the clinical perspective, or problems with ecological validity and cost-effectiveness emerge. The Magallanes Adaptation Scale is a 90-item Likert-instrument designed for Spanish-speaking adolescents in order to screen several adjustment areas from a holistic and positive perspective of development. Although some evidence of its psychometric robustness has been tested, no confirmatory analysis of its structure has been published. This paper analyzes the items and the factor structure (exploratory factor analysis and confirmatory factor analysis, using the split-half method) of the scales. Participants were 948 Spanish adolescents (49.84% girls) aged between 11 and 17 and stratified sampled. Thirty-six items were removed from the item analysis. The results of the exploratory factor analysis revealed five factors, excluding mother's adaptation. Several models were tested during the confirmatory factor analyses, with a 24-item second-order four-factor solution being found to have the best adjustment indicators. The short version proposed in this paper can constitute a helpful tool with screening purposes to help school teachers to assess students' overall development beyond mere academic performance, although further validity research is needed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lucía Jiménez
- Department of Developmental and Educational Psychology, University of Seville, Seville, Spain
| | - Susana Menéndez
- Department of Social, Developmental and Educational Psychology, University of Huelva, Huelva, Spain
| | - Victoria Hidalgo
- Department of Developmental and Educational Psychology, University of Seville, Seville, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Szokolszky A, Read C. Developmental Ecological Psychology and a Coalition of Ecological–Relational Developmental Approaches. ECOLOGICAL PSYCHOLOGY 2018. [DOI: 10.1080/10407413.2018.1410409] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
Affiliation(s)
| | - Catherine Read
- Department of Plant Biology, Rutgers University
- Department of Psychology, Ithaca College
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Kojima H, Froese T, Oka M, Iizuka H, Ikegami T. A Sensorimotor Signature of the Transition to Conscious Social Perception: Co-regulation of Active and Passive Touch. Front Psychol 2017; 8:1778. [PMID: 29085318 PMCID: PMC5649206 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01778] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/31/2016] [Accepted: 09/25/2017] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
It is not yet well understood how we become conscious of the presence of other people as being other subjects in their own right. Developmental and phenomenological approaches are converging on a relational hypothesis: my perception of a "you" is primarily constituted by another subject's attention being directed toward "me." This is particularly the case when my body is being physically explored in an intentional manner. We set out to characterize the sensorimotor signature of the transition to being aware of the other by re-analyzing time series of embodied interactions between pairs of adults (recorded during a "perceptual crossing" experiment). Measures of turn-taking and movement synchrony were used to quantify social coordination, and transfer entropy was used to quantify direction of influence. We found that the transition leading to one's conscious perception of the other's presence was indeed characterized by a significant increase in one's passive reception of the other's tactile stimulations. Unexpectedly, one's clear experience of such passive touch was consistently followed by a switch to active touching of the other, while the other correspondingly became more passive, which suggests that this intersubjective experience was reciprocally co-regulated by both participants.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hiroki Kojima
- Graduate School of Arts and Sciences, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan
| | - Tom Froese
- Institute for Applied Mathematics and Systems Research (IIMAS), National Autonomous University of Mexico (UNAM), Mexico City, Mexico
- Center for Complexity Sciences (C3), National Autonomous University of Mexico (UNAM), Mexico City, Mexico
| | - Mizuki Oka
- Graduate School of Systems and Information Engineering, University of Tsukuba, Ibaraki, Japan
| | - Hiroyuki Iizuka
- Graduate School of Information Science and Technology, Hokkaido University, Sapporo, Japan
| | - Takashi Ikegami
- Graduate School of Arts and Sciences, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Burnel MC, Perrone-Bertolotti M, Durrleman S, Reboul AC, Baciu M. Role of Two Types of Syntactic Embedding in Belief Attribution in Adults with or without Asperger Syndrome. Front Psychol 2017; 8:743. [PMID: 28553246 PMCID: PMC5427150 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00743] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/05/2016] [Accepted: 04/24/2017] [Indexed: 11/30/2022] Open
Abstract
The role of syntax in belief attribution (BA) is not completely understood in healthy adults and understudied in adults with autism spectrum disorder. Embedded syntax could be useful either for the development of Theory of Mind (ToM) (Emergence account) or more generally over the lifespan (Reasoning account). Two hypotheses have been explored, one suggesting that embedding itself (Relatives and Complement sentences and Metarepresentation account) is important for ToM and another one considering that the embedding of a false proposition into a true one (Complement sentences and Misrepresentation account) is important. The goals of this study were to evaluate (1) the role of syntax in ToM (Emergence vs. Reasoning account), (2) the type of syntax implied in ToM (Metarepresentation vs. Misrepresentation account), and (3) the verbally mediated strategies which compensate for ToM deficits in adults with Asperger Syndrome (AS). Fifty NeuroTypical (NT) adults and 22 adults with AS were involved in a forced-choice task including ±ToM tasks (BA and a control task, physical causation, PC) under four Interference conditions (silence, syllable repetition, relative sentences repetition, and complement sentences repetition). The non-significant ±ToM × Interference interaction effect in the NT group did not support the Reasoning account and thus suggests that syntax is useful only for ToM development (i.e., Emergence account). Results also indicated that repeating complement clauses put NT participants in a dual task whereas repeating relative clauses did not, suggesting that repeating relatives is easier for NT than repeating complements. This could be an argument in favor of the Misrepresentation account. However, this result should be interpreted with caution because our results did not support the Reasoning account. Moreover, AS participants (but not NT participants) were more disrupted by ±ToM tasks when asked to repeat complement sentences compared to relative clause sentences. This result is in favor of the Misrepresentation account and indirectly suggests verbally mediated strategies for ToM in AS. To summarize, our results are in favor of the Emergence account in NT and of Reasoning and Misrepresentation accounts in adults with AS. Overall, this suggests that adults with AS use complement syntax to compensate for ToM deficits.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Morgane Clémentine Burnel
- Université Grenoble Alpes, CNRS, LPNC UMR 5105Grenoble, France
- Université de Lyon, CNRS, Institute for Cognitive Sciences – Marc Jeannerod (UMR 5304)Bron, France
| | | | - Stephanie Durrleman
- Department of Psycholinguistics, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, University of GenevaGeneva, Switzerland
| | - Anne C. Reboul
- Université de Lyon, CNRS, Institute for Cognitive Sciences – Marc Jeannerod (UMR 5304)Bron, France
| | - Monica Baciu
- Université Grenoble Alpes, CNRS, LPNC UMR 5105Grenoble, France
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Brizio A, Gabbatore I, Tirassa M, Bosco FM. "No more a child, not yet an adult": studying social cognition in adolescence. Front Psychol 2015; 6:1011. [PMID: 26347664 PMCID: PMC4543799 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 62] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/29/2015] [Accepted: 08/06/2015] [Indexed: 01/06/2023] Open
Abstract
There are several reasons why adolescence is interesting. It is in this phase that an individual finds herself fully facing the external world: basically equipped with the kind of social cognition that s/he has acquired at home, at school and through the media during childhood, s/he has now to meet a host of other, diverse views of what "reasonable," "appropriate," or "expected" courses of thought and emotions are, in the wild with friends and peers, romantic or sexual partners, teachers and employers, and the society at large. Furthermore, she is also expected, both at home and in the external world, to have a wholly new degree of control over such courses. While the idea that the development of social cognition still progresses after infancy (and possibly throughout the life span) is clearly gaining consensus in the field, the literature building on it is still scarce. One of the reasons for this probably is that most tests used to study it focus on its basic component, namely theory of mind, and have been mostly devised for us with children; therefore, they are not suitable to deal with the hugely increasing complexity of social and mental life during adolescence and adulthood. Starting from a review of the literature available, we will argue that the development of social cognition should be viewed as a largely yet-to-be-understood mix of biological and cultural factors. While it is widely agreed upon that the very initial manifestations of social life in the newborn are largely driven by an innate engine with which all humans are equally endowed, it is also evident that each culture, and each individual within it, develops specific adult versions of social cognition.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Adelina Brizio
- Department of Psychology and Center for Cognitive Science, University of Turin , Turin, Italy ; Faculty of Communication Science, Università della Svizzera Italiana , Lugano, Switzerland
| | - Ilaria Gabbatore
- Faculty of Humanities, Child Language Research Center, University of Oulu , Oulu, Finland
| | - Maurizio Tirassa
- Department of Psychology and Center for Cognitive Science, University of Turin , Turin, Italy ; Department of Psychology and Neuroscience Institute of Turin , Turin, Italy
| | - Francesca M Bosco
- Department of Psychology and Center for Cognitive Science, University of Turin , Turin, Italy ; Department of Psychology and Neuroscience Institute of Turin , Turin, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
|
11
|
Embodied social interaction constitutes social cognition in pairs of humans: a minimalist virtual reality experiment. Sci Rep 2014; 4:3672. [PMID: 24419102 PMCID: PMC3890942 DOI: 10.1038/srep03672] [Citation(s) in RCA: 54] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/25/2013] [Accepted: 12/11/2013] [Indexed: 11/16/2022] Open
Abstract
Scientists have traditionally limited the mechanisms of social cognition to one brain, but recent approaches claim that interaction also realizes cognitive work. Experiments under constrained virtual settings revealed that interaction dynamics implicitly guide social cognition. Here we show that embodied social interaction can be constitutive of agency detection and of experiencing another's presence. Pairs of participants moved their “avatars” along an invisible virtual line and could make haptic contact with three identical objects, two of which embodied the other's motions, but only one, the other's avatar, also embodied the other's contact sensor and thereby enabled responsive interaction. Co-regulated interactions were significantly correlated with identifications of the other's avatar and reports of the clearest awareness of the other's presence. These results challenge folk psychological notions about the boundaries of mind, but make sense from evolutionary and developmental perspectives: an extendible mind can offload cognitive work into its environment.
Collapse
|
12
|
|
13
|
Lam YG. Re-examining the cognitive phenotype in autism: a study with young Chinese children. RESEARCH IN DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES 2013; 34:4591-4598. [PMID: 24171826 DOI: 10.1016/j.ridd.2013.09.039] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/08/2013] [Revised: 09/21/2013] [Accepted: 09/23/2013] [Indexed: 06/02/2023]
Abstract
Deficits consistently found in autism include an impaired "theory of mind", weak central coherence, and deficits in executive function. The current study examined whether this traditional cluster of symptoms existed in a group of Chinese-speaking children with autism. Sixteen high-functioning, non-retarded children with autism were matched to 16 typically developing (TD) children on gender, non-verbal IQ and age. Non-verbal IQ's of all participants were measured using the Raven Progressive Matrices. Each participant was tested individually on measures of "theory of mind", central coherence and executive function. Results indicated that most, but not all, participants with autism performed significantly poorer on two standard measures of first-order "theory of mind," although there was no significant difference on two other measures of that domain. As expected, they performed significantly worse on executive function tasks. However, the hypothesis of weak central coherence in autism was not substantiated. There was no evidence that these three cognitive impairments co-existed in individuals with autism. More likely, each of these deficits appears singly or in pair instead of forming a cluster.
Collapse
|
14
|
Hamlin JK. Failed attempts to help and harm: Intention versus outcome in preverbal infants’ social evaluations. Cognition 2013; 128:451-74. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.04.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 149] [Impact Index Per Article: 12.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/15/2012] [Revised: 04/15/2013] [Accepted: 04/16/2013] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
|
15
|
Social cognition in the we-mode. Trends Cogn Sci 2013; 17:160-5. [DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.02.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 163] [Impact Index Per Article: 13.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/03/2012] [Revised: 02/11/2013] [Accepted: 02/20/2013] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
|
16
|
|
17
|
Apperly IA. What is “theory of mind”? Concepts, cognitive processes and individual differences. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2012; 65:825-39. [DOI: 10.1080/17470218.2012.676055] [Citation(s) in RCA: 118] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/28/2022]
Abstract
Research on “theory of mind” has traditionally focused on a narrow participant group (preschool children) using a narrow range of experimental tasks (most notably, false-belief tasks). Recent work has greatly expanded the age range of human participants tested to include human infants, older children, and adults, has devised new tasks, and has adopted methods from cognitive psychology and neuroscience. However, theoretical work has not kept pace with these changes, with the result that studies using one kind of method or participant group often inherit assumptions about the nature of theory of mind from other research, with little regard for whether these assumptions are appropriate. I argue that three distinct approaches to thinking about theory of mind are already implicit in research practice, and that future work, whether with infants, children, or adults, will benefit from articulating these approaches more clearly and following their different implications for what theory of mind is and how it should be studied.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ian A. Apperly
- School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, Edgbaston, Birmingham, UK
| |
Collapse
|