1
|
Schulze C, Aka A, Bartels DM, Bucher SF, Embrey JR, Gureckis TM, Häubl G, Ho MK, Krajbich I, Moore AK, Oettingen G, Ongchoco JDK, Oprea R, Reinholtz N, Newell BR. A timeline of cognitive costs in decision-making. Trends Cogn Sci 2025:S1364-6613(25)00083-X. [PMID: 40393899 DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2025.04.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/30/2024] [Revised: 04/02/2025] [Accepted: 04/02/2025] [Indexed: 05/22/2025]
Abstract
Recent research from economics, psychology, cognitive science, computer science, and marketing is increasingly interested in the idea that people face cognitive costs when making decisions. Reviewing and synthesizing this research, we develop a framework of cognitive costs that organizes concepts along a temporal dimension and maps out when costs occur in the decision-making process and how they impact decisions. Our unifying framework broadens the scope of research on cognitive costs to a wider timeline of cognitive processing. We identify implications and recommendations emerging from our framework for intervening on behavior to tackle some of the most pressing issues of our day, from improving health and saving decisions to mitigating the consequences of climate change.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Christin Schulze
- School of Psychology, University of New South Wales, Sydney, NSW, Australia.
| | - Ada Aka
- Stanford Graduate School of Business, Stanford, CA, USA
| | - Daniel M Bartels
- University of Chicago, Booth School of Business, Chicago, IL, USA
| | - Stefan F Bucher
- University of Cambridge, Faculty of Economics, Cambridge, UK; Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Sloan School of Management, Cambridge, MA, USA; Max Planck Institute for Biological Cybernetics, Tübingen, Germany
| | - Jake R Embrey
- School of Psychology, University of New South Wales, Sydney, NSW, Australia; University of Chicago, Booth School of Business, Chicago, IL, USA
| | - Todd M Gureckis
- New York University, Department of Psychology, New York, NY, USA
| | - Gerald Häubl
- University of Alberta, School of Business, Edmonton, AB, Canada
| | - Mark K Ho
- Stevens Institute of Technology, Department of Computer Science, Hoboken, NJ, USA
| | - Ian Krajbich
- University of California Los Angeles, Department of Psychology, Los Angeles, CA, USA
| | - Alexander K Moore
- University of Illinois Chicago, Department of Marketing, Chicago, IL, USA
| | | | - Joan D K Ongchoco
- University of British Columbia, Department of Psychology, Vancouver, BC, Canada
| | - Ryan Oprea
- University of California Santa Barbara, Department of Economics, Santa Barbara, CA, USA
| | - Nicholas Reinholtz
- University of Colorado Boulder, Leeds School of Business, Boulder, CO, USA
| | - Ben R Newell
- School of Psychology, University of New South Wales, Sydney, NSW, Australia; Institute for Climate Risk & Response, University of New South Wales, Sydney, NSW, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Murray S, Amaya S. The strategic allocation theory of vigilance. WILEY INTERDISCIPLINARY REVIEWS. COGNITIVE SCIENCE 2024; 15:e1693. [PMID: 39295156 DOI: 10.1002/wcs.1693] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/26/2024] [Revised: 06/19/2024] [Accepted: 07/20/2024] [Indexed: 09/21/2024]
Abstract
Despite its importance in different occupational and everyday contexts, vigilance, typically defined as the capacity to sustain attention over time, is remarkably limited. What explains these limits? Two theories have been proposed. The Overload Theory states that being vigilant consumes limited information-processing resources; when depleted, task performance degrades. The Underload Theory states that motivation to perform vigilance tasks declines over time, thereby prompting attentional shifts and hindering performance. We highlight some conceptual and empirical problems for both theories and propose an alternative: the Strategic Allocation Theory. For the Strategic Allocation Theory, performance on vigilance tasks optimizes as a function of intrinsic and extrinsic motivations, including metacognitive factors such as the expected value of effort and the expected value of planning. Limited capacities must be deployed across task sets to maximize expected reward. The observed limits of vigilance reflect changes in the perceived value of, among other things, sustaining attention to a task rather than attending to something else. Drawing from recent computational theories of cognitive control and meta-reasoning, we argue that the Strategic Allocation Theory explains more phenomena related to vigilance behavior than other theories, including self-report data. Finally, we outline some of the testable predictions the theory makes across several experimental paradigms. This article is categorized under: Philosophy > Foundations of Cognitive Science Psychology > Attention.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Samuel Murray
- Laboratorio de Juicios y Emociones Morales, Universidad de Los Andes, Bogotá, Colombia
- Department of Philosophy, Providence College, Providence, Rhode Island, USA
- Neuroscience Program, Providence College, Providence, Rhode Island, USA
| | - Santiago Amaya
- Laboratorio de Juicios y Emociones Morales, Universidad de Los Andes, Bogotá, Colombia
- Departamento de Filosofía, Universidad de los Andes, Bogota, Colombia
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Master SL, Li S, Curtis CE. Trying Harder: How Cognitive Effort Sculpts Neural Representations during Working Memory. J Neurosci 2024; 44:e0060242024. [PMID: 38769009 PMCID: PMC11236589 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.0060-24.2024] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/10/2024] [Revised: 03/26/2024] [Accepted: 03/28/2024] [Indexed: 05/22/2024] Open
Abstract
While the exertion of mental effort improves performance on cognitive tasks, the neural mechanisms by which motivational factors impact cognition remain unknown. Here, we used fMRI to test how changes in cognitive effort, induced by changes in task difficulty, impact neural representations of working memory (WM). Participants (both sexes) were precued whether WM difficulty would be hard or easy. We hypothesized that hard trials demanded more effort as a later decision required finer mnemonic precision. Behaviorally, pupil size was larger and response times were slower on hard compared with easy trials suggesting our manipulation of effort succeeded. Neurally, we observed robust persistent activity during delay periods in the prefrontal cortex (PFC), especially during hard trials. Yet, details of the memoranda could not be decoded from patterns in prefrontal activity. In the patterns of activity in the visual cortex, however, we found strong decoding of memorized targets, where accuracy was higher on hard trials. To potentially link these across-region effects, we hypothesized that effort, carried by persistent activity in the PFC, impacts the quality of WM representations encoded in the visual cortex. Indeed, we found that the amplitude of delay period activity in the frontal cortex predicted decoded accuracy in the visual cortex on a trial-wise basis. These results indicate that effort-related feedback signals sculpt population activity in the visual cortex, improving mnemonic fidelity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sarah L Master
- Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, New York 10003
| | - Shanshan Li
- Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, New York 10003
- Program in Psychology, New York University Abu Dhabi, Abu Dhabi, United Arab Emirates
| | - Clayton E Curtis
- Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, New York 10003
- Center for Neural Science, New York University, New York, New York 10003
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Bolenz F, Pachur T. Older adults select different but not simpler strategies than younger adults in risky choice. PLoS Comput Biol 2024; 20:e1012204. [PMID: 38857295 PMCID: PMC11192436 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1012204] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/09/2023] [Revised: 06/21/2024] [Accepted: 05/28/2024] [Indexed: 06/12/2024] Open
Abstract
Younger and older adults often differ in their risky choices. Theoretical frameworks on human aging point to various cognitive and motivational factors that might underlie these differences. Using a novel computational model based on the framework of resource rationality, we find that the two age groups rely on different strategies. Importantly, older adults did not use simpler strategies than younger adults, they did not select among fewer strategies, they did not make more errors, and they did not put more weight on cognitive costs. Instead, older adults selected strategies that had different risk propensities than those selected by younger adults. Our modeling approach suggests that age differences in risky choice are not necessarily a consequence of cognitive decline; instead, they may reflect motivational differences between age groups.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Florian Bolenz
- Center for Adaptive Rationality, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany
- Science of Intelligence, Research Cluster of Excellence, Berlin, Germany
| | - Thorsten Pachur
- Center for Adaptive Rationality, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany
- Science of Intelligence, Research Cluster of Excellence, Berlin, Germany
- School of Management, Technical University of Munich, Munich, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Master SL, Li S, Curtis CE. Trying harder: how cognitive effort sculpts neural representations during working memory. BIORXIV : THE PREPRINT SERVER FOR BIOLOGY 2023:2023.12.07.570686. [PMID: 38106094 PMCID: PMC10723420 DOI: 10.1101/2023.12.07.570686] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/19/2023]
Abstract
The neural mechanisms by which motivational factors influence cognition remain unknown. Using fMRI, we tested how cognitive effort impacts working memory (WM). Participants were precued whether WM difficulty would be hard or easy. Hard trials demanded more effort as a later decision required finer mnemonic precision. Behaviorally, pupil size was larger and response times were slower on hard trials suggesting our manipulation of effort succeeded. Neurally, we observed robust persistent activity in prefrontal cortex, especially during hard trials. We found strong decoding of location in visual cortex, where accuracy was higher on hard trials. Connecting these across-region effects, we found that the amplitude of delay period activity in frontal cortex predicted decoded accuracy in visual cortex on a trial-wise basis. We conclude that the gain of persistent activity in frontal cortex may be the source of effort-related feedback signals that improve the quality of WM representations stored in visual cortex.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Shanshan Li
- Department of Psychology, New York University
- Program in Psychology, New York University Abu Dhabi
| | - Clayton E. Curtis
- Department of Psychology, New York University
- Center for Neural Science, New York University
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Imamura K, Takano KT, Yoshida Y, Nakashizuka T, Managi S. Effects of information provision on willingness to pay for conservation of alpine plants in Japan. JOURNAL OF ENVIRONMENTAL MANAGEMENT 2023; 342:118175. [PMID: 37301025 DOI: 10.1016/j.jenvman.2023.118175] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/20/2023] [Revised: 04/07/2023] [Accepted: 05/12/2023] [Indexed: 06/12/2023]
Abstract
Information provision is essential for obtaining the cooperation of the general public for the conservation of unfamiliar ecosystems towards a sustainable (e.g. carbon-neutral and nature-positive) society. The purpose of this study is to identify effective ways of raising public awareness for ecosystem conservation. We explored the interaction between the manner of information provision (i.e. the medium through which and how much information is provided) and the personal attributes (e.g. environmental attitude) of the recipients on their willingness to pay (WTP) for conservation using Japanese alpine plants as the subject. Discrete choice experiments using an online survey were conducted with public citizens aged 20-69 years across Japan, and data from 8457 respondents were analysed. The data analysis was performed in two steps: 1) estimating individual WTP and 2) exploring factors affecting WTP. The results demonstrated that individual WTP was 135,798 ± 82,840 (mean ± standard deviation) JPY per person for a lifetime. The WTP increased when information was provided in the form of short texts and graphics for those proactive about nature conservation, but increased more when video information was provided to those reactive about nature conservation. The study shows that ecosystem conservation groups need to adapt the amount and format of information for target audiences (e.g. Generation Z youth, who are more sustainability-oriented and prefer to accomplish more in less time).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kohei Imamura
- Graduate School of Life Sciences, Tohoku University, 6-3 Aoba, Aramaki, Aoba-ku, Sendai, 980-8578, Japan; Global and Local Environment Co-creation Institute, Ibaraki University, 2-2-35 Sakuragawa, Mito City, Ibaraki, 310-0801, Japan.
| | - Kohei Takenaka Takano
- Graduate School of Life Sciences, Tohoku University, 6-3 Aoba, Aramaki, Aoba-ku, Sendai, 980-8578, Japan; Natural Environment Division, Nagano Environmental Conservation Research Institute, 2054-120 Kitago, Nagano-shi, Nagano, 381-0075, Japan.
| | - Yumi Yoshida
- Graduate School of Environmental Studies, Tohoku University, 468-1 Aoba, Aramaki, Aoba-ku, Sendai, 980-8572, Japan; Department of Economics, Faculty of Economics, Rissho University, 4-2-6 Osaki, Shinagawa-ku, Tokyo, 141-8602, Japan
| | - Tohru Nakashizuka
- Graduate School of Life Sciences, Tohoku University, 6-3 Aoba, Aramaki, Aoba-ku, Sendai, 980-8578, Japan; Forestry and Forest Products Research Institute, 1 Matsunosato, Tsukuba, Ibaraki, 305-8687, Japan
| | - Shunsuke Managi
- Urban Institute & Department of Civil Engineering, Kyushu University, 744 Motooka, Nishi-ku, Fukuoka, 819-0395, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Fang J, Schooler L, Shenghua L. Machine learning strategy identification: A paradigm to uncover decision strategies with high fidelity. Behav Res Methods 2023; 55:263-284. [PMID: 35378675 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-022-01828-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 03/09/2022] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
We propose a novel approach, which we call machine learning strategy identification (MLSI), to uncovering hidden decision strategies. In this approach, we first train machine learning models on choice and process data of one set of participants who are instructed to use particular strategies, and then use the trained models to identify the strategies employed by a new set of participants. Unlike most modeling approaches that need many trials to identify a participant's strategy, MLSI can distinguish strategies on a trial-by-trial basis. We examined MLSI's performance in three experiments. In Experiment I, we taught participants three different strategies in a paired-comparison decision task. The best machine learning model identified the strategies used by participants with an accuracy rate above 90%. In Experiment II, we compared MLSI with the multiple-measure maximum likelihood (MM-ML) method that is also capable of integrating multiple types of data in strategy identification, and found that MLSI had higher identification accuracy than MM-ML. In Experiment III, we provided feedback to participants who made decisions freely in a task environment that favors the non-compensatory strategy take-the-best. The trial-by-trial results of MLSI show that during the course of the experiment, most participants explored a range of strategies at the beginning, but eventually learned to use take-the-best. Overall, the results of our study demonstrate that MLSI can identify hidden strategies on a trial-by-trial basis and with a high level of accuracy that rivals the performance of other methods that require multiple trials for strategy identification.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jun Fang
- Department of Psychology, Syracuse University, Syracuse, NY, USA.
| | - Lael Schooler
- Department of Psychology, Syracuse University, Syracuse, NY, USA
| | - Luan Shenghua
- CAS Key Laboratory for Behavioral Science, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China
- Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Thomson KS, Oppenheimer DM. The "Effort Elephant" in the Room: What Is Effort, Anyway? PERSPECTIVES ON PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE 2022; 17:1633-1652. [PMID: 35767344 DOI: 10.1177/17456916211064896] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
Despite decades of research in the fields of judgment and decision-making, social psychology, cognitive psychology, human-machine interaction, behavioral economics, and neuroscience, we still do not know what "cognitive effort" is. The definitions in use are often imprecise and sometimes diametrically opposed. Researchers with different assumptions talk past each other, and many aspects of effort conservation remain untested and difficult to measure. In this article, we explain why effort is so difficult to pin down and why it is important that researchers develop consensus on precise definitions. Next, we describe major "hidden" sources of miscommunication: areas in which researchers disagree in their underlying assumptions about the nature of effort without realizing it. We briefly review a number of methods used to both measure and manipulate the effortfulness of thinking and highlight why they often produce contradictory findings. We conclude by reviewing existing perspectives on cognitive effort and integrating them to suggest a common framework for communicating about effort as a limited cognitive resource.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Keela S Thomson
- Department of Social and Decision Science and Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University
| | - Daniel M Oppenheimer
- Department of Social and Decision Science and Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Hansen T, Thomsen TU. How individual differences in knowledge over-/underconfidence impede dietary consumer decision making under time pressure. PERSONALITY AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES 2022. [DOI: 10.1016/j.paid.2022.111701] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
|
10
|
Pachur T. Strategy selection in decisions from givens: Deciding at a glance? Cogn Psychol 2022; 136:101483. [PMID: 35640353 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101483] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/26/2020] [Revised: 04/16/2022] [Accepted: 05/12/2022] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
People deciding between alternatives have at their disposal a toolbox containing both compensatory strategies, which take into account all available attributes of those alternatives, and noncompensatory strategies, which consider only some of the attributes. It is commonly assumed that noncompensatory strategies play only a minor role in decisions from givens, where attribute information is openly presented, because all attributes can be processed automatically "at a glance." Based on a literature review, however, I establish that previous studies on strategy selection in decisions from givens have yielded highly heterogeneous findings, including evidence of widespread use of noncompensatory strategies. Drawing on insights from visual attention research on subitizing, I argue that this heterogeneity might be due to differences across studies in the number of attributes and in whether the same or different symbols are used to represent high/low attribute values across attributes. I tested the impact of these factors in two experiments with decisions from givens in which both the number of attributes shown for each alternative and the coding of attribute values was manipulated. An analysis of participants' strategy use with a Bayesian multimethod approach (taking into account both decisions and response-time patterns) showed that a noncompensatory strategy was more frequently selected in conditions with a higher number of attributes; the type of attribute coding scheme did not affect strategy selection. Using a compensatory strategy in the conditions with eight (vs. four) attributes was associated with rather long response times and a high rate of strategy execution errors. The results suggest that decisions from givens can incur cognitive costs that prohibit reliance on automatic compensatory decision making and that can favor the adaptive selection of a noncompensatory strategy.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Thorsten Pachur
- Center for Adaptive Rationality, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany; Science of Intelligence, Research Cluster of Excellence, Germany; School of Management, Technical University of Munich, Germany.
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Millroth P. Toward a richer understanding of human cognition: Unleashing the full potential of the concurrent information-processing paradigm. NEW IDEAS IN PSYCHOLOGY 2021. [DOI: 10.1016/j.newideapsych.2021.100873] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
|
12
|
Li M, Peng H, Ji L. Roles of cognitive load and self‐relevance during emotional information searching on decision‐making. JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DECISION MAKING 2020. [DOI: 10.1002/bdm.2169] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Meijia Li
- Institute of Developmental Psychology Beijing Normal University Beijing China
| | - Huamao Peng
- Institute of Developmental Psychology Beijing Normal University Beijing China
| | - Lingling Ji
- Institute of Developmental Psychology Beijing Normal University Beijing China
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Musalem A, Montoya R, Meißner M, Huber J. Components of attentional effort for repeated tasks. JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DECISION MAKING 2020. [DOI: 10.1002/bdm.2188] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/10/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Andres Musalem
- Industrial Engineering University of Chile Faculty of Physical and Mathematical Sciences Santiago Chile
- Instituto Sistemas Complejos de Ingeniería (ISCI) Santiago Chile
| | - Ricardo Montoya
- Industrial Engineering University of Chile Faculty of Physical and Mathematical Sciences Santiago Chile
- Instituto Sistemas Complejos de Ingeniería (ISCI) Santiago Chile
| | - Martin Meißner
- Department of Business & Economics Zeppelin University Friedrichshafen Germany
| | - Joel Huber
- Fuqua School of Business Duke University Durham North Carolina USA
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
A Game Theoretic Model of Choosing a Valuable Good via a Short List Heuristic. MATHEMATICS 2020. [DOI: 10.3390/math8020199] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
The Internet gives access to a huge amount of data at the click of a mouse. This is very helpful when consumers are making decisions about which product to buy. However, the final decision to purchase is still generally made by humans who have limited memory and perception. The short list heuristic is often used when there are many offers on the market. Searchers first find information about offers via the Internet and on this basis choose a relatively small number of offers to view in real life. Although such rules are often used in practice, little research has been carried out on determining, for example, what the size of the short list should be depending on the parameters of the problem or modelling how the short list heuristic can be implemented when there are multiple decision makers. This article presents a game theoretic model of such a search procedure with two players. These two players can be interpreted, for example, as a couple searching for a flat or a second-hand car. The model indicates that under such a search procedure the roles of searchers should only be divided when the preferences of the players are coherent or there is a high level of goodwill between them. In other cases, dividing the roles leads to a high level of conflict.
Collapse
|
15
|
How to model the neurocognitive dynamics of decision making: A methodological primer with ACT-R. Behav Res Methods 2019; 52:857-880. [PMID: 31396864 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-019-01286-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Higher cognitive functions are the product of a dynamic interplay of perceptual, mnemonic, and other cognitive processes. Modeling the interplay of these processes and generating predictions about both behavioral and neural data can be achieved with cognitive architectures. However, such architectures are still used relatively rarely, likely because working with them comes with high entry-level barriers. To lower these barriers, we provide a methodological primer for modeling higher cognitive functions and their constituent cognitive subprocesses with arguably the most developed cognitive architecture today-ACT-R. We showcase a principled method of generating individual response time predictions, and demonstrate how neural data can be used to refine ACT-R models. To illustrate our approach, we develop a fully specified neurocognitive model of a prominent strategy for memory-based decisions-the take-the-best heuristic-modeling decision making as a dynamic interplay of perceptual, motor, and memory processes. This implementation allows us to predict the dynamics of behavior and the temporal and spatial patterns of brain activity. Moreover, we show that comparing the predictions for brain activity to empirical BOLD data allows us to differentiate competing ACT-R implementations of take the best.
Collapse
|
16
|
Analytis PP, Wu CM, Gelastopoulos A. Make‐or‐Break: Chasing Risky Goals or Settling for Safe Rewards? Cogn Sci 2019; 43:e12743. [DOI: 10.1111/cogs.12743] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/31/2018] [Revised: 04/14/2019] [Accepted: 04/23/2019] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Affiliation(s)
| | - Charley M. Wu
- Center for Adaptive Rationality Max Planck Institute for Human Development
| | | |
Collapse
|
17
|
Marewski JN, Bröder A, Glöckner A. Some Metatheoretical Reflections on Adaptive Decision Making and the Strategy Selection Problem. JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DECISION MAKING 2018. [DOI: 10.1002/bdm.2075] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Julian N. Marewski
- Faculty of Business and Economics; University of Lausanne; Lausanne Switzerland
| | - Arndt Bröder
- School of Social Sciences; University of Mannheim; Mannheim Germany
| | - Andreas Glöckner
- Institute for Psychology; University of Hagen; Hagen Germany
- Max Planck Institute for Research on Collective Goods; Bonn Germany
| |
Collapse
|