1
|
Santon M, Troscianko J, Heatubun CD, How MJ. Stealth and deception: Adaptive motion camouflage in hunting broadclub cuttlefish. SCIENCE ADVANCES 2025; 11:eadr3686. [PMID: 40138425 PMCID: PMC11939058 DOI: 10.1126/sciadv.adr3686] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/27/2024] [Accepted: 02/11/2025] [Indexed: 03/29/2025]
Abstract
Maintaining camouflage while moving is a challenge faced by many predators. Some exploit background motion to hide while hunting, and others may use coloration and behavior to generate motion noise that impairs detection or recognition. Here, we uncover a unique form of motion camouflage, showing that broadclub cuttlefish pass dark stripes downward across their head and arms to disguise their hunting maneuvers. This "passing-stripe" display reduces the probability of response to predatory expanding stimuli by prey crabs in a lab-based experiment, is modulated according to approach speed during a hunt, and generates a motion pattern that is different from that of looming predators. This form of motion camouflage likely functions by overwhelming the threatening motion of the approaching predator with nonthreatening downward motion generated by the rhythmic stripes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Matteo Santon
- School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK
| | - Jolyon Troscianko
- Centre for Ecology and Conservation, University of Exeter, Penryn, UK
| | - Charlie D. Heatubun
- Faculty of Forestry, University of Papua, Manokwari, Indonesia
- Regional Research and Innovation Agency (BRIDA), Manokwari, Indonesia
| | - Martin J. How
- School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Smithers SP, Brett MF, How MJ, Scott-Samuel NE, Roberts NW. Fiddler crabs (Afruca tangeri) detect second-order motion in both intensity and polarization. Commun Biol 2024; 7:1255. [PMID: 39362984 PMCID: PMC11450093 DOI: 10.1038/s42003-024-06953-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/10/2023] [Accepted: 09/24/2024] [Indexed: 10/05/2024] Open
Abstract
Motion vision is vital for a wide range of animal behaviors. Fiddler crabs, for example, rely heavily on motion to detect the movement of avian predators. They are known to detect first-order motion using both intensity (defined by spatiotemporal correlations in luminance) and polarization information (defined separately as spatiotemporal correlations in the degree and/or angle of polarization). However, little is known about their ability to detect second-order motion, another important form of motion information; defined separately by spatiotemporal correlations in higher-order image properties. In this work we used behavioral experiments to test how fiddler crabs (Afruca tangeri) responded to both second-order intensity and polarization stimuli. Fiddler crabs responded to a number of different intensity based second-order stimuli. Furthermore, the crabs also responded to second-order polarization stimuli, a behaviorally relevant stimulus applicable to an unpolarized flying bird when viewed against a polarized sky. The detection of second-order motion in polarization is, to the best of our knowledge, the first demonstration of this ability in any animal. This discovery therefore opens a new dimension in our understanding of how animals use polarization vision for target detection and the broader importance of second-order motion detection for animal behavior.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Samuel P Smithers
- School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol Life Sciences Building, Bristol, UK.
- Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, USA.
| | - Maisie F Brett
- School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol Life Sciences Building, Bristol, UK
| | - Martin J How
- School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol Life Sciences Building, Bristol, UK
| | | | - Nicholas W Roberts
- School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol Life Sciences Building, Bristol, UK.
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Park J, Son M, Park J, Bang SY, Ha J, Moon H, Lee YN, Lee SI, Jablonski PG. Escape behaviors in prey and the evolution of pennaceous plumage in dinosaurs. Sci Rep 2024; 14:549. [PMID: 38272887 PMCID: PMC10811223 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-023-50225-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/26/2023] [Accepted: 12/16/2023] [Indexed: 01/27/2024] Open
Abstract
Numerous non-avian dinosaurs possessed pennaceous feathers on their forelimbs (proto-wings) and tail. Their functions remain unclear. We propose that these pennaceous feathers were used in displays to flush hiding prey through stimulation of sensory-neural escape pathways in prey, allowing the dinosaurs to pursue the flushed prey. We evaluated the escape behavior of grasshoppers to hypothetical visual flush-displays by a robotic dinosaur, and we recorded neurophysiological responses of grasshoppers' escape pathway to computer animations of the hypothetical flush-displays by dinosaurs. We show that the prey of dinosaurs would have fled more often when proto-wings were present, especially distally and with contrasting patterns, and when caudal plumage, especially of a large area, was used during the hypothetical flush-displays. The reinforcing loop between flush and pursue functions could have contributed to the evolution of larger and stiffer feathers for faster running, maneuverability, and stronger flush-displays, promoting foraging based on the flush-pursue strategy. The flush-pursue hypothesis can explain the presence and distribution of the pennaceous feathers, plumage color contrasts, as well as a number of other features observed in early pennaraptorans. This scenario highlights that sensory-neural processes underlying prey's antipredatory reactions may contribute to the origin of major evolutionary innovations in predators.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jinseok Park
- School of Biological Sciences, Seoul National University, Seoul, South Korea
| | - Minyoung Son
- School of Earth and Environmental Sciences, Seoul National University, Seoul, South Korea
- Department of Earth and Environmental Sciences, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, USA
| | - Jeongyeol Park
- Department of Mechanical Engineering, Sungkyunkwan University, Suwon, South Korea
| | - Sang Yun Bang
- School of Biological Sciences, Seoul National University, Seoul, South Korea
| | - Jungmoon Ha
- School of Biological Sciences, Seoul National University, Seoul, South Korea
| | - Hyungpil Moon
- Department of Mechanical Engineering, Sungkyunkwan University, Suwon, South Korea.
| | - Yuong-Nam Lee
- School of Earth and Environmental Sciences, Seoul National University, Seoul, South Korea.
| | - Sang-Im Lee
- Department of New Biology, DGIST, Taegu, South Korea.
| | - Piotr G Jablonski
- School of Biological Sciences, Seoul National University, Seoul, South Korea.
- Museum and Institute of Zoology, Polish Academy of Sciences, Warsaw, Poland.
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Zeil J. Views from 'crabworld': the spatial distribution of light in a tropical mudflat. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 2023; 209:859-876. [PMID: 37460846 PMCID: PMC10643439 DOI: 10.1007/s00359-023-01653-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/23/2023] [Revised: 06/18/2023] [Accepted: 06/29/2023] [Indexed: 11/14/2023]
Abstract
Natural scene analysis has been extensively used to understand how the invariant structure of the visual environment may have shaped biological image processing strategies. This paper deals with four crucial, but hitherto largely neglected aspects of natural scenes: (1) the viewpoint of specific animals; (2) the fact that image statistics are not independent of the position within the visual field; (3) the influence of the direction of illumination on luminance, spectral and polarization contrast in a scene; and (4) the biologically relevant information content of natural scenes. To address these issues, I recorded the spatial distribution of light in a tropical mudflat with a spectrographic imager equipped with a polarizing filter in an attempt to describe quantitatively the visual environment of fiddler crabs. The environment viewed by the crabs has a distinct structure. Depending on the position of the sun, the luminance, the spectral composition, and the polarization characteristics of horizontal light distribution are not uniform. This is true for both skylight and for reflections from the mudflat surface. The high-contrast feature of the line of horizon dominates the vertical distribution of light and is a discontinuity in terms of luminance, spectral distribution and of image statistics. On a clear day, skylight intensity increases towards the horizon due to multiple scattering, and its spectral composition increasingly resembles that of sunlight. Sky-substratum contrast is highest at short wavelengths. I discuss the consequences of this extreme example of the topography of vision for extracting biologically relevant information from natural scenes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jochen Zeil
- Research School of Biology, Australian National University, P.O. Box 475, Canberra, ACT, 2601, Australia.
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Robledo-Ospina LE, Morehouse N, Escobar F, Tapia-McClung H, Narendra A, Rao D. Visual antipredator effects of web flexing in an orb web spider, with special reference to web decorations. THE SCIENCE OF NATURE - NATURWISSENSCHAFTEN 2023; 110:23. [PMID: 37219696 DOI: 10.1007/s00114-023-01849-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/27/2023] [Revised: 05/05/2023] [Accepted: 05/09/2023] [Indexed: 05/24/2023]
Abstract
Some visual antipredator strategies involve the rapid movement of highly contrasting body patterns to frighten or confuse the predator. Bright body colouration, however, can also be detected by potential predators and used as a cue. Among spiders, Argiope spp. are usually brightly coloured but they are not a common item in the diet of araneophagic wasps. When disturbed, Argiope executes a web-flexing behaviour in which they move rapidly and may be perceived as if they move backwards and towards an observer in front of the web. We studied the mechanisms underlying web-flexing behaviour as a defensive strategy. Using multispectral images and high-speed videos with deep-learning-based tracking techniques, we evaluated body colouration, body pattern, and spider kinematics from the perspective of a potential wasp predator. We show that the spider's abdomen is conspicuous, with a disruptive colouration pattern. We found that the body outline of spiders with web decorations was harder to detect when compared to spiders without decorations. The abdomen was also the body part that moved fastest, and its motion was composed mainly of translational (vertical) vectors in the potential predator's optical flow. In addition, with high contrast colouration, the spider's movement might be perceived as a sudden change in body size (looming effect) as perceived by the predator. These effects alongside the other visual cues may confuse potential wasp predators by breaking the spider body outline and affecting the wasp's flight manoeuvre, thereby deterring the wasp from executing the final attack.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Luis E Robledo-Ospina
- Red de Ecoetología, Instituto de Ecología, A.C., Xalapa, Veracruz, México
- Instituto de Biotecnología y Ecología Aplicada, Universidad Veracruzana, Xalapa, Veracruz, México
| | - Nathan Morehouse
- Department of Biological Sciences, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, OH, USA
| | - Federico Escobar
- Red de Ecoetología, Instituto de Ecología, A.C., Xalapa, Veracruz, México
| | - Horacio Tapia-McClung
- Instituto de Investigaciones en Inteligencia Artificial, Universidad Veracruzana, Xalapa, Veracruz, México
| | - Ajay Narendra
- School of Natural Sciences, Macquarie University, Sydney, NSW, 2109, Australia
| | - Dinesh Rao
- Instituto de Biotecnología y Ecología Aplicada, Universidad Veracruzana, Xalapa, Veracruz, México.
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Tomsic D, Rind C. Animal behavior: Timing escape on angular size or angular velocity? Curr Biol 2023; 33:R108-R110. [PMID: 36750021 DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2022.11.050] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/09/2023]
Abstract
Animals avoid rapidly approaching objects. In many arthropods, angular size is a common visual cue used to decide the time of avoidance. A new study shows that fiddler crabs decide when to escape based on object angular expansion velocity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Daniel Tomsic
- Institute of Physiology, Molecular Biology and Neuroscience, IFIBYNE Building, CONICET-University of Buenos Aires, Ciudad Universitaria 1428, CABA, Argentina.
| | - Claire Rind
- Newcastle University Biosciences Institute, Henry Wellcome Building, Newcastle University, Framlington Place, Newcastle upon Tyne NE2 4HH, UK.
| |
Collapse
|