1
|
Fernández-López M, Solaja O, Crepaldi D, Perea M. Top-down feedback normalizes distortion in early visual word recognition: Insights from masked priming. Psychon Bull Rev 2025; 32:920-929. [PMID: 39424754 PMCID: PMC12000148 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-024-02585-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 09/06/2024] [Indexed: 10/21/2024]
Abstract
The identification of visually presented words tolerates distortions in the input format, as Hannagan et al. Plos One, 7, e32121, (2012) demonstrated in a masked priming lexical decision task, showing sizable identity-priming effects with CAPTCHA-like primes. This tolerance to distortion has two potential explanations: bottom-up normalization in the encoding stage (Dehaene et al., Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 335-341, 2005) or top-down lexical feedback (McClelland & Rumelhart, Psychological Review, 88, 375-407, 1981). To disentangle the predictions of these accounts, we conducted two masked identity-priming experiments with printed and CAPTCHA-like distorted primes on high- and low-frequency words. The rationale was that, in the distorted format, high-frequency words would benefit more from top-down feedback than low-frequency words. Results in the lexical decision experiment showed that, for high-frequency words, identity-priming effects were only slightly greater for printed than for CAPTCHA-like primes, whereas this difference was larger for low-frequency words. In contrast, when employing the same-different matching task, which does not require lexical access, the identity-priming effect was greater for printed primes and was unaffected by word frequency. Thus, during lexical access, top-down feedback may help normalize the visual input in the early stages of word recognition, challenging bottom-up models of visual word recognition.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Maria Fernández-López
- Department of Basic Psychology, Universitat de València, Av. Blasco Ibáñez, 21, 46010, València, Spain.
| | - Olga Solaja
- Scuola Internazionale Superiore di Studi Avanzati (SISSA), Trieste, Italy
| | - Davide Crepaldi
- Scuola Internazionale Superiore di Studi Avanzati (SISSA), Trieste, Italy
| | - Manuel Perea
- Department of Methodology and ERI-Lectura, Universitat de València, València, Spain
- CINC, Universidad Nebrija, Madrid, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Civera T, Perea M, Leone-Fernandez B, Vergara-Martínez M. The effect of inter-letter spacing on the N170 during visual word recognition: An event-related potentials experiment. COGNITIVE, AFFECTIVE & BEHAVIORAL NEUROSCIENCE 2024; 24:1096-1108. [PMID: 39313747 PMCID: PMC11525266 DOI: 10.3758/s13415-024-01221-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 08/19/2024] [Indexed: 09/25/2024]
Abstract
Previous behavioral studies have shown that inter-letter spacing affects visual word recognition and reading. While condensed spacing may hinder the early stages of letter encoding because of increased crowding effects, the impact of expanded inter-letter spacing is still unclear. To examine the electrophysiological signature of inter-letter spacing on visual word recognition, we presented words in three different inter-letter spacing conditions (default, condensed [-1.5 points] or expanded [+1.5 points]) in an event-related potentials go/no-go semantic categorization task. Our focus was on the N170, an event-related potentials component associated with the early encoding of orthographic information, which also is sensitive to crowding effects. Results revealed that the N170 amplitude reached the largest values for the condensed condition than for the default and expanded spacing conditions, which did not differ. While increased crowding impacted the early encoding of orthographic information, extra letter spacing (compared with default spacing) did not. This outcome is consistent with the Modified Receptive Field hypothesis, in which letter receptors adapt their size to cope with letter crowding. These findings reveal that reducing the space between letters more than the default spacing impairs the ability to process written words, whereas slightly expanding the space between letters does not provide any additional benefit.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Teresa Civera
- ERI-Lectura, Universitat de València, Av. Blasco Ibáñez, 21, 46010, Valencia, Spain.
| | - Manuel Perea
- ERI-Lectura, Universitat de València, Av. Blasco Ibáñez, 21, 46010, Valencia, Spain
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
3
|
Perea M, Romero-Ortells I, Labusch M, Fernández-López M, Marcet A. Examining Letter Detector Tolerance through Offset Letter Halves: Evidence from Lexical Decision. J Cogn 2023; 6:56. [PMID: 37780981 PMCID: PMC10540705 DOI: 10.5334/joc.322] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/12/2023] [Accepted: 09/14/2023] [Indexed: 10/03/2023] Open
Abstract
Neurobiological models of reading assume that the specialized detectors at the letter level (e.g., the arrays of detectors for the letter 'n') possess a certain degree of tolerance (e.g., Local Combination Detectors model, Dehaene et al. 2005). In this study, we designed two lexical decision experiments that examined the limits of tolerance of letter detectors by introducing a novel manipulation involving shifting letter halves (e.g., in Experiment 1; in Experiment 2) relative to intact items. This manipulation alters the transition between upper and lower parts of the letters, adding junctions that do not exist in the intact letter forms. We included high- and low-frequency words in the stimulus list to investigate whether letter distortion affects processing beyond the letter level, reasoning that interactive effects would signal top-down lexical feedback. In Experiment 1, which employed a subtle letter shift, we observed a minimal cost of letter distortion that did not interact with word frequency. Experiment 2, employing a larger letter shift, revealed an overall greater reading cost that affected differentially high- and low-frequency words. Overall, these findings offer insights into the limits of resilience in letter detectors to distortion during word recognition and introduce a novel manipulation of letter distortion.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Manuel Perea
- Department of Methodology and ERI-Lectura, Universitat de València, Valencia, Spain
- Centro de Investigación Nebrija en Cognición (CINC), Universidad Nebrija, Madrid, Spain
| | - Inka Romero-Ortells
- Centro de Investigación Nebrija en Cognición (CINC), Universidad Nebrija, Madrid, Spain
| | - Melanie Labusch
- Department of Methodology and ERI-Lectura, Universitat de València, Valencia, Spain
- Centro de Investigación Nebrija en Cognición (CINC), Universidad Nebrija, Madrid, Spain
| | - María Fernández-López
- Department of Basic Psychology, Methodology, and ERI-Lectura, Universitat de València, Valencia, Spain
- Department of Psychology and Sociology, School of Social and Human Sciences, Universidad de Zaragoza, Teruel, Spain
| | - Ana Marcet
- Grupo de Investigación en Enseñanza de Lenguas (GIEL), Department of Language and Literature Teaching, Universitat de València, Valencia, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Fernández-López M, Perea M, Marcet A. Breaking the boundaries: the power of ligatures in visual-word recognition. Front Psychol 2023; 14:1166192. [PMID: 37384168 PMCID: PMC10294432 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1166192] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/14/2023] [Accepted: 04/25/2023] [Indexed: 06/30/2023] Open
Abstract
Introduction Current neurobiological-inspired models of visual-word recognition propose that letter detectors in the word recognition system can tolerate some variations in the visual form of the letters. However, it is unclear whether this tolerance extends to novel ligatures, which combine two letters into a single glyph. Methods To investigate this, the present study utilized a masked priming experiment with a lexical decision task to examine whether primes containing novel ligatures are effective in activating their corresponding base word, relative to omitted-letter primes, in the initial stages of word processing. For each target word (e.g., VIRTUAL), were created an identity prime (virtual), a prime containing a novel ligature of two of the letters (e.g., virtual; "ir" in a single glyph), and an omitted-letter prime where one letter was removed (e.g., vrtual [omitted-vowel] in Experiment 1; vitual [omitted-consonant] in Experiment 2). Results Results showed that the presence of a novel ligature in the prime resulted in faster lexical decision times compared to a prime with an omitted vowel (Experiment 1), but not with an omitted consonant (Experiment 2). Furthermore, the performance with the primes containing the novel ligature was not different from that of the identity primes. Discussion These results suggest that the word recognition system can quickly enable separate letter detectors for novel ligatures. These findings have important implications for our understanding of the front-end of visual-word recognition.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- María Fernández-López
- Department of Methodology of Behavioral Sciences and ERI-Lectura, Universitat de València, Valencia, Spain
| | - Manuel Perea
- Department of Methodology of Behavioral Sciences and ERI-Lectura, Universitat de València, Valencia, Spain
- Center of Research in Cognition, Universidad Antonio de Nebrija, Madrid, Spain
| | - Ana Marcet
- Grupo de Investigación en Enseñanza de Lenguas (GIEL), Department of Language and Literature Teaching, Universitat de València, Valencia, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Zhang K, Gu F, Yu H. Early lexical processing of Chinese one-character words and Mongolian words: A comparative study using event-related potentials. Front Psychol 2023; 13:1061990. [PMID: 36733864 PMCID: PMC9887120 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1061990] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/05/2022] [Accepted: 12/28/2022] [Indexed: 01/18/2023] Open
Abstract
Logographic language and alphabetic language differ significantly in orthography. Investigating the commonality and particularity of visual word recognition between the two distinct writing systems is informative for understating the neural mechanisms underlying visual word recognition. In the present study, we compared the chronometry of early lexical processing and the brain regions involved in early lexical processing between Chinese (logographic language) and Mongolian (alphabetic language) by recording event-related potentials (ERPs) using both implicit and explicit reading tasks. Familiar Chinese one-character words (lexical) and unknown Chinese one-character words (non-lexical) were pseudorandomly presented to native Chinese readers in Experiment 1. Mongolian words (lexical) and pseudowords (non-lexical) were pseudorandomly presented to native Mongolian readers in Experiment 2. In the color decision task, participants were asked to decide the color (black or blue) of each stimulus. In the lexical recognition task, participants were asked to report whether they could recognize each stimulus. The results showed that in both experiments and both tasks, ERPs to lexical items differed significantly from those to non-lexical items in the parietooccipital scalp region approximately 250 ms after stimulus onset, reflecting the early lexical processing, which likely originated from the ventral occipitotemporal cortex as revealed by source analysis. These results indicated that although Chinese and Mongolian differed markedly in orthographic features, the neural mechanisms underlying early lexical processing are similar between the two languages.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kai Zhang
- Department of Chinese Language and Literature, Northwest Minzu University, Lanzhou, China,Key Laboratory of China’s Ethnic Languages and Intelligent Processing of Gansu Province, Northwest Minzu University, Lanzhou, China
| | - Feng Gu
- Neurocognitive Laboratory for Linguistics and Semiotics, College of Literature and Journalism, Sichuan University, Chengdu, China
| | - Hongzhi Yu
- Department of Chinese Language and Literature, Northwest Minzu University, Lanzhou, China,Key Laboratory of China’s Ethnic Languages and Information Technology of Ministry of Education, Northwest Minzu University, Lanzhou, China,*Correspondence: Hongzhi Yu, ✉
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Duñabeitia JA, Perea M, Labusch M. Rëâdīńg wõrdš wîth ōrńåmêńtš: is there a cost? Front Psychol 2023; 14:1168471. [PMID: 37179852 PMCID: PMC10172505 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1168471] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/17/2023] [Accepted: 04/14/2023] [Indexed: 05/15/2023] Open
Abstract
Introduction Recent research has reported that adding non-existent diacritical marks to a word produces a minimal reading cost compared to the intact word. Here we examined whether this minimal reading cost is due to: (1) the resilience of letter detectors to the perceptual noise (i.e., the cost should be small and comparable for words and nonwords) or (2) top-down lexical processes that normalize the percept for words (i.e., the cost would be larger for nonwords). Methods We designed a letter detection experiment in which a target stimulus (either a word or a nonword) was presented intact or with extra non-existent diacritics [e.g., amigo (friend) vs. ãmîgô; agimo vs. ãgîmô]. Participants had to decide which of two letters was in the stimulus (e.g., A vs. U). Results Although the task involved lexical processing, with responses being faster and more accurate for words compared to nonwords, we found only a minimal advantage in error rates for intact stimuli versus those with non-existent diacritics. This advantage was similar for both words and nonwords. Discussion The letter detectors in the word recognition system appear to be resilient to non-existent diacritics without the need for feedback from higher levels of processing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jon Andoni Duñabeitia
- Centro de Investigación Nebrija en Cognición (CINC), Universidad Antonio de Nebrija, Madrid, Spain
- Department of Languages and Culture, The Arctic University of Norway, Tromsø, Norway
- *Correspondence: Jon Andoni Duñabeitia,
| | - Manuel Perea
- Centro de Investigación Nebrija en Cognición (CINC), Universidad Antonio de Nebrija, Madrid, Spain
- Departamento de Metodología and ERI-Lectura, Universitat de València, Valencia, Spain
| | - Melanie Labusch
- Centro de Investigación Nebrija en Cognición (CINC), Universidad Antonio de Nebrija, Madrid, Spain
- Departamento de Metodología and ERI-Lectura, Universitat de València, Valencia, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Perea M, Baciero A, Labusch M, Fernández‐López M, Marcet A. Are brand names special words? Letter visual-similarity affects the identification of brand names, but not common words. Br J Psychol 2022; 113:835-852. [PMID: 35107840 PMCID: PMC9545185 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12557] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/17/2021] [Revised: 01/11/2022] [Accepted: 01/13/2022] [Indexed: 12/04/2022]
Abstract
Brand names are often considered a special type of words of special relevance to examine the role of visual codes during reading: unlike common words, brand names are typically presented with the same letter-case configuration (e.g., IKEA, adidas). Recently, Pathak et al. (European Journal of Marketing, 2019, 53, 2109) found an effect of visual similarity for misspelled brand names when the participants had to decide whether the brand name was spelled correctly or not (e.g., tacebook [baseword: facebook] was responded more slowly and less accurately than xacebook). This finding is at odds with both orthographically based visual-word recognition models and prior experiments using misspelled common words (e.g., viotin [baseword: violin] is identified as fast as viocin). To solve this puzzle, we designed two experiments in which the participants had to decide whether the presented item was written correctly. In Experiment 1, following a procedure similar to Pathak et al. (European Journal of Marketing, 2019, 53, 2109), we examined the effect of visual similarity on misspelled brand names with/without graphical information (e.g., anazon vs. atazon [baseword: amazon]). Experiment 2 was parallel to Experiment 1, but we focused on misspelled common words (e.g., anarillo vs. atarillo; baseword: amarillo [yellow in Spanish]). Results showed a sizeable effect of visual similarity on misspelled brand names - regardless of their graphical information, but not on misspelled common words. These findings suggest that visual codes play a greater role when identifying brand names than common words. We examined how models of visual-word recognition can account for this dissociation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Manuel Perea
- Universitat de ValènciaValenciaSpain
- Universidad Antonio de NebrijaMadridSpain
| | - Ana Baciero
- Universidad Antonio de NebrijaMadridSpain
- Bournemouth UniversityBournemouthUK
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
8
|
Fernández-López M, Perea M, Vergara-Martínez M. On the time course of the tolerance of letter detectors to rotations: A masked priming ERP investigation. Neuropsychologia 2022; 172:108259. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2022.108259] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/04/2022] [Revised: 05/04/2022] [Accepted: 05/06/2022] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
|