1
|
Wachowiak M, Dewan A, Bozza T, O'Connell TF, Hong EJ. Recalibrating Olfactory Neuroscience to the Range of Naturally Occurring Odor Concentrations. J Neurosci 2025; 45:e1872242024. [PMID: 40044450 PMCID: PMC11884396 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.1872-24.2024] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/25/2024] [Revised: 11/15/2024] [Accepted: 11/25/2024] [Indexed: 03/09/2025] Open
Abstract
Sensory systems enable organisms to detect and respond to environmental signals relevant for their survival and reproduction. A crucial aspect of any sensory signal is its intensity; understanding how sensory signals guide behavior requires probing sensory system function across the range of stimulus intensities naturally experienced by an organism. In olfaction, defining the range of natural odorant concentrations is difficult. Odors are complex mixtures of airborne chemicals emitting from a source in an irregular pattern that varies across time and space, necessitating specialized methods to obtain an accurate measurement of concentration. Perhaps as a result, experimentalists often choose stimulus concentrations based on empirical considerations rather than with respect to ecological or behavioral context. Here, we attempt to determine naturally relevant concentration ranges for olfactory stimuli by reviewing and integrating data from diverse disciplines. We compare odorant concentrations used in experimental studies in rodents and insects with those reported in different settings including ambient natural environments, the headspace of natural sources, and within the sources themselves. We also compare these values to psychophysical measurements of odorant detection threshold in rodents, where thresholds have been extensively measured. Odorant concentrations in natural regimes rarely exceed a few parts per billion, while most experimental studies investigating olfactory coding and behavior exceed these concentrations by several orders of magnitude. We discuss the implications of this mismatch and the importance of testing odorants in their natural concentration range for understanding neural mechanisms underlying olfactory sensation and odor-guided behaviors.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Matt Wachowiak
- Department of Neurobiology, University of Utah School of Medicine, Salt Lake City, Utah 84112
| | - Adam Dewan
- Department of Psychology and Program in Neuroscience, Florida State University, Tallahassee, Florida 32306
| | - Thomas Bozza
- Department of Neurobiology, Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois 60208
| | - Tom F O'Connell
- Division of Biology & Biological Engineering, California Institute of Technology, Pasadena, California 91125
| | - Elizabeth J Hong
- Division of Biology & Biological Engineering, California Institute of Technology, Pasadena, California 91125
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Srinivasan S, Daste S, Modi MN, Turner GC, Fleischmann A, Navlakha S. Effects of stochastic coding on olfactory discrimination in flies and mice. PLoS Biol 2023; 21:e3002206. [PMID: 37906721 PMCID: PMC10618007 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pbio.3002206] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/21/2022] [Accepted: 08/21/2023] [Indexed: 11/02/2023] Open
Abstract
Sparse coding can improve discrimination of sensory stimuli by reducing overlap between their representations. Two factors, however, can offset sparse coding's benefits: similar sensory stimuli have significant overlap and responses vary across trials. To elucidate the effects of these 2 factors, we analyzed odor responses in the fly and mouse olfactory regions implicated in learning and discrimination-the mushroom body (MB) and the piriform cortex (PCx). We found that neuronal responses fall along a continuum from extremely reliable across trials to extremely variable or stochastic. Computationally, we show that the observed variability arises from noise within central circuits rather than sensory noise. We propose this coding scheme to be advantageous for coarse- and fine-odor discrimination. More reliable cells enable quick discrimination between dissimilar odors. For similar odors, however, these cells overlap and do not provide distinguishing information. By contrast, more unreliable cells are decorrelated for similar odors, providing distinguishing information, though these benefits only accrue with extended training with more trials. Overall, we have uncovered a conserved, stochastic coding scheme in vertebrates and invertebrates, and we identify a candidate mechanism, based on variability in a winner-take-all (WTA) inhibitory circuit, that improves discrimination with training.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Shyam Srinivasan
- Kavli Institute for Brain and Mind, University of California, San Diego, California, United States of America
- Salk Institute for Biological Studies, La Jolla, California, United States of America
| | - Simon Daste
- Department of Neuroscience, Division of Biology and Medicine, Brown University, Providence, Rhode Island, United States of America
- Robert J. and Nancy D. Carney Institute for Brain Science, Brown University, Providence, Rhode Island, United States of America
| | - Mehrab N. Modi
- Janelia Research Campus, Howard Hughes Medical Institute, Ashburn, Virginia, United States of America
| | - Glenn C. Turner
- Janelia Research Campus, Howard Hughes Medical Institute, Ashburn, Virginia, United States of America
| | - Alexander Fleischmann
- Department of Neuroscience, Division of Biology and Medicine, Brown University, Providence, Rhode Island, United States of America
- Robert J. and Nancy D. Carney Institute for Brain Science, Brown University, Providence, Rhode Island, United States of America
| | - Saket Navlakha
- Simons Center for Quantitative Biology, Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory, Cold Spring Harbor, New York, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Gardner D. Where are the cores in this thing? …and what are they computing? (with apologies to Larry Abbott). J Comput Neurosci 2022; 50:133-138. [PMID: 35122188 PMCID: PMC9034984 DOI: 10.1007/s10827-021-00809-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/24/2021] [Revised: 11/24/2021] [Accepted: 12/09/2021] [Indexed: 12/04/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Daniel Gardner
- Departments of Physiology & Biophysics, Neurology, and Neuroscience, Weill Cornell Medical College, New York, NY, 10065, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Shen Y, Wang J, Navlakha S. A Correspondence Between Normalization Strategies in Artificial and Biological Neural Networks. Neural Comput 2021; 33:3179-3203. [PMID: 34474484 PMCID: PMC8662716 DOI: 10.1162/neco_a_01439] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/15/2021] [Accepted: 06/14/2021] [Indexed: 12/24/2022]
Abstract
A fundamental challenge at the interface of machine learning and neuroscience is to uncover computational principles that are shared between artificial and biological neural networks. In deep learning, normalization methods such as batch normalization, weight normalization, and their many variants help to stabilize hidden unit activity and accelerate network training, and these methods have been called one of the most important recent innovations for optimizing deep networks. In the brain, homeostatic plasticity represents a set of mechanisms that also stabilize and normalize network activity to lie within certain ranges, and these mechanisms are critical for maintaining normal brain function. In this article, we discuss parallels between artificial and biological normalization methods at four spatial scales: normalization of a single neuron's activity, normalization of synaptic weights of a neuron, normalization of a layer of neurons, and normalization of a network of neurons. We argue that both types of methods are functionally equivalent-that is, both push activation patterns of hidden units toward a homeostatic state, where all neurons are equally used-and we argue that such representations can improve coding capacity, discrimination, and regularization. As a proof of concept, we develop an algorithm, inspired by a neural normalization technique called synaptic scaling, and show that this algorithm performs competitively against existing normalization methods on several data sets. Overall, we hope this bidirectional connection will inspire neuroscientists and machine learners in three ways: to uncover new normalization algorithms based on established neurobiological principles; to help quantify the trade-offs of different homeostatic plasticity mechanisms used in the brain; and to offer insights about how stability may not hinder, but may actually promote, plasticity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yang Shen
- Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory, Simons Center for Quantitative Biology, Cold Spring Harbor, NY 11724, U.S.A.
| | - Julia Wang
- Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory, Simons Center for Quantitative Biology, Cold Spring Harbor, NY 11724, U.S.A.
| | - Saket Navlakha
- Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory, Simons Center for Quantitative Biology, Cold Spring Harbor, NY 11724, U.S.A.
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Gallistel C. The physical basis of memory. Cognition 2021; 213:104533. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104533] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/31/2020] [Revised: 12/01/2020] [Accepted: 12/01/2020] [Indexed: 12/31/2022]
|
6
|
Gjorgjieva J, Meister M, Sompolinsky H. Functional diversity among sensory neurons from efficient coding principles. PLoS Comput Biol 2019; 15:e1007476. [PMID: 31725714 PMCID: PMC6890262 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1007476] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/24/2018] [Revised: 12/03/2019] [Accepted: 10/10/2019] [Indexed: 01/10/2023] Open
Abstract
In many sensory systems the neural signal is coded by the coordinated response of heterogeneous populations of neurons. What computational benefit does this diversity confer on information processing? We derive an efficient coding framework assuming that neurons have evolved to communicate signals optimally given natural stimulus statistics and metabolic constraints. Incorporating nonlinearities and realistic noise, we study optimal population coding of the same sensory variable using two measures: maximizing the mutual information between stimuli and responses, and minimizing the error incurred by the optimal linear decoder of responses. Our theory is applied to a commonly observed splitting of sensory neurons into ON and OFF that signal stimulus increases or decreases, and to populations of monotonically increasing responses of the same type, ON. Depending on the optimality measure, we make different predictions about how to optimally split a population into ON and OFF, and how to allocate the firing thresholds of individual neurons given realistic stimulus distributions and noise, which accord with certain biases observed experimentally.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Markus Meister
- Division of Biology, California Institute of Technology, Pasadena, California, United States of America
| | - Haim Sompolinsky
- Center for Brain Science, Harvard University, Cambridge, Massachusetts, United States of America
- The Edmond and Lily Safra Center for Brain Sciences, Hebrew University, Jerusalem, Israel
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Louis M. Order in Odors: A Power Law Structures the Encoding of Stimulus Identity and Intensity. Neuron 2019; 101:768-770. [PMID: 30844394 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.02.022] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
Odorant molecules are detected through the combinatorial activation of ensembles of olfactory sensory neurons. By capitalizing on the numerical simplicity of the Drosophila larval brain, Si et al. (2019) uncover principles constraining the representation of the quality and intensity of olfactory stimuli.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Matthieu Louis
- Neuroscience Research Institute and Department of Molecular, Cellular, and Developmental Biology, University of California, Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, CA 93106, USA; Department of Physics, University of California, Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, CA 93106, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Kadakia N, Emonet T. Front-end Weber-Fechner gain control enhances the fidelity of combinatorial odor coding. eLife 2019; 8:e45293. [PMID: 31251174 PMCID: PMC6609331 DOI: 10.7554/elife.45293] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/23/2019] [Accepted: 06/26/2019] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
We showed previously (Gorur-Shandilya et al., 2017) that Drosophila olfactory receptor neurons (ORNs) expressing the co-receptor Orco scale their gain inversely with mean odor intensity according to Weber-Fechner's law. Here, we show that this front-end adaptation promotes the reconstruction of odor identity from dynamic odor signals, even in the presence of confounding background odors and rapid intensity fluctuations. These enhancements are further aided by known downstream transformations in the antennal lobe and mushroom body. Our results, which are applicable to various odor classification and reconstruction schemes, stem from the fact that this adaptation mechanism is not intrinsic to the identity of the receptor involved. Instead, a feedback mechanism adjusts receptor sensitivity based on the activity of the receptor-Orco complex, according to Weber-Fechner's law. Thus, a common scaling of the gain across Orco-expressing ORNs may be a key feature of ORN adaptation that helps preserve combinatorial odor codes in naturalistic landscapes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nirag Kadakia
- Department of Molecular, Cellular and Developmental BiologyYale UniversityNew HavenUnited States
| | - Thierry Emonet
- Department of Molecular, Cellular and Developmental BiologyYale UniversityNew HavenUnited States
- Department of PhysicsYale UniversityNew HavenUnited States
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Kepple DR, Giaffar H, Rinberg D, Koulakov AA. Deconstructing Odorant Identity via Primacy in Dual Networks. Neural Comput 2019; 31:710-737. [PMID: 30764743 PMCID: PMC7449618 DOI: 10.1162/neco_a_01175] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/10/2023]
Abstract
In the olfactory system, odor percepts retain their identity despite substantial variations in concentration, timing, and background. We study a novel strategy for encoding intensity-invariant stimulus identity that is based on representing relative rather than absolute values of stimulus features. For example, in what is known as the primacy coding model, odorant identities are represented by the conditions that some odorant receptors are activated more strongly than others. Because, in this scheme, odorant identity depends only on the relative amplitudes of olfactory receptor responses, identity is invariant to changes in both intensity and monotonic nonlinear transformations of its neuronal responses. Here we show that sparse vectors representing odorant mixtures can be recovered in a compressed sensing framework via elastic net loss minimization. In the primacy model, this minimization is performed under the constraint that some receptors respond to a given odorant more strongly than others. Using duality transformation, we show that this constrained optimization problem can be solved by a neural network whose Lyapunov function represents the dual Lagrangian and whose neural responses represent the Lagrange coefficients of primacy and other constraints. The connectivity in such a dual network resembles known features of connectivity in olfactory circuits. We thus propose that networks in the piriform cortex implement dual computations to compute odorant identity with the sparse activities of individual neurons representing Lagrange coefficients. More generally, we propose that sparse neuronal firing rates may represent Lagrange multipliers, which we call the dual brain hypothesis. We show such a formulation is well suited to solve problems with multiple interacting relative constraints.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Daniel R Kepple
- Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory, Cold Spring Harbor, NY 11724, U.S.A.
| | - Hamza Giaffar
- Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory, Cold Spring Harbor, NY 11724, U.S.A.
| | - Dmitry Rinberg
- Neuroscience Institute, New York University School of Medicine, New York, NY 10016, U.S.A.
| | | |
Collapse
|
10
|
Groschner LN, Miesenböck G. Mechanisms of Sensory Discrimination: Insights from Drosophila Olfaction. Annu Rev Biophys 2019; 48:209-229. [PMID: 30786228 DOI: 10.1146/annurev-biophys-052118-115655] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
Abstract
All an animal can do to infer the state of its environment is to observe the sensory-evoked activity of its own neurons. These inferences about the presence, quality, or similarity of objects are probabilistic and inform behavioral decisions that are often made in close to real time. Neural systems employ several strategies to facilitate sensory discrimination: Biophysical mechanisms separate the neuronal response distributions in coding space, compress their variances, and combine information from sequential observations. We review how these strategies are implemented in the olfactory system of the fruit fly. The emerging principles of odor discrimination likely apply to other neural circuits of similar architecture.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lukas N Groschner
- Centre for Neural Circuits and Behavior, University of Oxford, Oxford OX1 3SR, United Kingdom;
| | - Gero Miesenböck
- Centre for Neural Circuits and Behavior, University of Oxford, Oxford OX1 3SR, United Kingdom;
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Si G, Kanwal JK, Hu Y, Tabone CJ, Baron J, Berck M, Vignoud G, Samuel ADT. Structured Odorant Response Patterns across a Complete Olfactory Receptor Neuron Population. Neuron 2019; 101:950-962.e7. [PMID: 30683545 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.12.030] [Citation(s) in RCA: 33] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/17/2017] [Revised: 10/29/2018] [Accepted: 12/20/2018] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
Odor perception allows animals to distinguish odors, recognize the same odor across concentrations, and determine concentration changes. How the activity patterns of primary olfactory receptor neurons (ORNs), at the individual and population levels, facilitate distinguishing these functions remains poorly understood. Here, we interrogate the complete ORN population of the Drosophila larva across a broadly sampled panel of odorants at varying concentrations. We find that the activity of each ORN scales with the concentration of any odorant via a fixed dose-response function with a variable sensitivity. Sensitivities across odorants and ORNs follow a power-law distribution. Much of receptor sensitivity to odorants is accounted for by a single geometrical property of molecular structure. Similarity in the shape of temporal response filters across odorants and ORNs extend these relationships to fluctuating environments. These results uncover shared individual- and population-level patterns that together lend structure to support odor perceptions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Guangwei Si
- Department of Physics, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA; Center for Brain Science, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA
| | - Jessleen K Kanwal
- Center for Brain Science, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA; Program in Neuroscience, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA
| | - Yu Hu
- Center for Brain Science, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA
| | - Christopher J Tabone
- Department of Physics, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA; Center for Brain Science, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA
| | - Jacob Baron
- Department of Physics, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA; Center for Brain Science, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA
| | - Matthew Berck
- Department of Physics, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA; Center for Brain Science, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA
| | - Gaetan Vignoud
- Department of Physics, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA; Center for Brain Science, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA
| | - Aravinthan D T Samuel
- Department of Physics, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA; Center for Brain Science, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Reddy G, Zak JD, Vergassola M, Murthy VN. Antagonism in olfactory receptor neurons and its implications for the perception of odor mixtures. eLife 2018; 7:34958. [PMID: 29687778 PMCID: PMC5915184 DOI: 10.7554/elife.34958] [Citation(s) in RCA: 58] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/10/2018] [Accepted: 03/30/2018] [Indexed: 11/16/2022] Open
Abstract
Natural environments feature mixtures of odorants of diverse quantities, qualities and complexities. Olfactory receptor neurons (ORNs) are the first layer in the sensory pathway and transmit the olfactory signal to higher regions of the brain. Yet, the response of ORNs to mixtures is strongly non-additive, and exhibits antagonistic interactions among odorants. Here, we model the processing of mixtures by mammalian ORNs, focusing on the role of inhibitory mechanisms. We show how antagonism leads to an effective ‘normalization’ of the ensemble ORN response, that is, the distribution of responses of the ORN population induced by any mixture is largely independent of the number of components in the mixture. This property arises from a novel mechanism involving the distinct statistical properties of receptor binding and activation, without any recurrent neuronal circuitry. Normalization allows our encoding model to outperform non-interacting models in odor discrimination tasks, leads to experimentally testable predictions and explains several psychophysical experiments in humans. When ordering in a coffee shop, you probably recognize and enjoy the aroma of freshly roasted coffee beans. But as well as coffee, you can also smell the croissants behind the counter and maybe even the perfume or cologne of the person next to you. Each of these scents consists of a collection of chemicals, or odorants. To distinguish between the aroma of coffee and that of croissants, your brain must group the odorants appropriately and then keep the groups separate from each other. This is not a trivial task. Odorants bind to proteins called odorant receptors found on the surface of cells in the nose called olfactory receptor neurons. But each odorant does not have its own dedicated receptor. Instead, a single odorant will bind to multiple types of odorant receptors, and thus, each olfactory receptor neuron may respond to multiple odorants. So how does the brain encode mixtures of odorants in a way that allows us to distinguish one aroma from another? Reddy, Zak et al. have developed a computational model to explain how this process works. The model assumes that an odorant triggers a response in an olfactory receptor neuron via two steps. First, the odorant binds to an odorant receptor. Second, the bound odorant activates the receptor. But the odorant that binds most strongly to a receptor will not necessarily be the odorant that is best at activating that receptor. This allows a phenomenon called competitive antagonism to occur. This is when one odorant in a mixture binds more strongly to a receptor than the other odorants, but only weakly activates that receptor. In so doing, the strongly bound odorant prevents the other odorants from binding to and activating the receptor. This helps tame the dominating influence of background odors, which might otherwise saturate the responses of individual olfactory receptor neurons. Reddy, Zak et al. show that processes such as competitive antagonism enable olfactory receptor neurons to encode all of the odors within a mixture. The model can explain various phenomena observed in experiments and it adds to our understanding of how the brain generates our sense of smell. The model may also be relevant to other biological systems that must filter weak signals from a dominant background. These include the immune system, which must distinguish a small set of foreign proteins from the much larger number of proteins that make up our bodies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gautam Reddy
- Department of Physics, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, United States
| | - Joseph D Zak
- Department of Molecular Cellular Biology, Harvard University, Cambridge, United States.,Center for Brain Science, Harvard University, Cambridge, United States
| | - Massimo Vergassola
- Department of Physics, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, United States
| | - Venkatesh N Murthy
- Department of Molecular Cellular Biology, Harvard University, Cambridge, United States.,Center for Brain Science, Harvard University, Cambridge, United States
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Dasgupta S, Stevens CF, Navlakha S. A neural algorithm for a fundamental computing problem. Science 2017; 358:793-796. [DOI: 10.1126/science.aam9868] [Citation(s) in RCA: 100] [Impact Index Per Article: 12.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/14/2017] [Accepted: 09/25/2017] [Indexed: 12/18/2022]
Abstract
Similarity search—for example, identifying similar images in a database or similar documents on the web—is a fundamental computing problem faced by large-scale information retrieval systems. We discovered that the fruit fly olfactory circuit solves this problem with a variant of a computer science algorithm (called locality-sensitive hashing). The fly circuit assigns similar neural activity patterns to similar odors, so that behaviors learned from one odor can be applied when a similar odor is experienced. The fly algorithm, however, uses three computational strategies that depart from traditional approaches. These strategies can be translated to improve the performance of computational similarity searches. This perspective helps illuminate the logic supporting an important sensory function and provides a conceptually new algorithm for solving a fundamental computational problem.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sanjoy Dasgupta
- Department of Computer Science and Engineering, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA
| | - Charles F. Stevens
- Molecular Neurobiology Laboratory, The Salk Institute for Biological Studies, La Jolla, CA, USA
- Kavli Institute for Brain and Mind, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA
| | - Saket Navlakha
- Integrative Biology Laboratory, The Salk Institute for Biological Studies, La Jolla, CA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Abstract
The olfactory system removes correlations in natural odors using a network of inhibitory neurons in the olfactory bulb. It has been proposed that this network integrates the response from all olfactory receptors and inhibits them equally. However, how such global inhibition influences the neural representations of odors is unclear. Here, we study a simple statistical model of the processing in the olfactory bulb, which leads to concentration-invariant, sparse representations of the odor composition. We show that the inhibition strength can be tuned to obtain sparse representations that are still useful to discriminate odors that vary in relative concentration, size, and composition. The model reveals two generic consequences of global inhibition: (i) odors with many molecular species are more difficult to discriminate and (ii) receptor arrays with heterogeneous sensitivities perform badly. Comparing these predictions to experiments will help us to understand the role of global inhibition in shaping normalized odor representations in the olfactory bulb.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- David Zwicker
- School of Engineering and Applied Sciences, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA 02138, United States of America
- Kavli Institute for Bionano Science and Technology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA 02138, United States of America
- * E-mail:
| |
Collapse
|