1
|
Hafed ZM. Superior colliculus peri-saccadic field potentials are dominated by a visual sensory preference for the upper visual field. iScience 2025; 28:112021. [PMID: 40104053 PMCID: PMC11914513 DOI: 10.1016/j.isci.2025.112021] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/08/2024] [Revised: 01/04/2025] [Accepted: 02/11/2025] [Indexed: 03/20/2025] Open
Abstract
The primate superior colliculus (SC) plays important sensory, cognitive, and motor processing roles. Among its properties, the SC has clear visual field asymmetries: visual responses are stronger in the upper visual field representation, whereas saccade-related motor bursts are weaker. Here, I asked whether peri-saccadic SC network activity can still reflect the SC's visual sensitivity asymmetry, thus supporting recent evidence of sensory-related signals embedded within the SC's motor bursts. I analyzed collicular peri-saccadic local field potential (LFP) modulations and found them to be much stronger in the upper visual field, despite the weaker motor bursts. This effect persisted even with saccades toward a blank, suggesting an importance of visual field location. I also found that engaging working memory during saccade preparation differentially modulated the SC's LFP's, again with a dichotomous upper/lower visual field asymmetry. I conclude that the SC network possesses a clear sensory signal at the time of saccade generation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ziad M Hafed
- Werner Reichardt Centre for Integrative Neuroscience, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
- Hertie Institute for Clinical Brain Research, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Bharmauria V, Seo S, Crawford JD. Neural integration of egocentric and allocentric visual cues in the gaze system. J Neurophysiol 2025; 133:109-120. [PMID: 39584726 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00498.2024] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/24/2024] [Revised: 11/14/2024] [Accepted: 11/16/2024] [Indexed: 11/26/2024] Open
Abstract
A fundamental question in neuroscience is how the brain integrates egocentric (body-centered) and allocentric (landmark-centered) visual cues, but for many years this question was ignored in sensorimotor studies. This changed in recent behavioral experiments, but the underlying physiology of ego/allocentric integration remained largely unstudied. The specific goal of this review is to explain how prefrontal neurons integrate eye-centered and landmark-centered visual codes for optimal gaze behavior. First, we briefly review the whole brain/behavioral mechanisms for ego/allocentric integration in the human and summarize egocentric coding mechanisms in the primate gaze system. We then focus in more depth on cellular mechanisms for ego/allocentric coding in the frontal and supplementary eye fields. We first explain how prefrontal visual responses integrate eye-centered target and landmark codes to produce a transformation toward landmark-centered coordinates. Next, we describe what happens when a landmark shifts during the delay between seeing and acquiring a remembered target, initially resulting in independently coexisting ego/allocentric memory codes. We then describe how these codes are reintegrated in the motor burst for the gaze shift. Deep network simulations suggest that these properties emerge spontaneously for optimal gaze behavior. Finally, we synthesize these observations and relate them to normal brain function through a simplified conceptual model. Together, these results show that integration of visuospatial features continues well beyond visual cortex and suggest a general cellular mechanism for goal-directed visual behavior.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Vishal Bharmauria
- The Tampa Human Neurophysiology Lab & Department of Neurosurgery and Brain Repair, Morsani College of Medicine, University of South Florida, Tampa, Florida, United States
- York Centre for Vision Research and Centre for Integrative and Applied Neuroscience, York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
| | - Serah Seo
- York Centre for Vision Research and Centre for Integrative and Applied Neuroscience, York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
- Faculty of Medicine, University of Ottawa, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada
| | - J Douglas Crawford
- York Centre for Vision Research and Centre for Integrative and Applied Neuroscience, York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
- Departments of Psychology, Biology, Kinesiology & Health Sciences, York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Seo S, Bharmauria V, Schütz A, Yan X, Wang H, Crawford JD. Multiunit Frontal Eye Field Activity Codes the Visuomotor Transformation, But Not Gaze Prediction or Retrospective Target Memory, in a Delayed Saccade Task. eNeuro 2024; 11:ENEURO.0413-23.2024. [PMID: 39054056 PMCID: PMC11373882 DOI: 10.1523/eneuro.0413-23.2024] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/13/2023] [Revised: 07/16/2024] [Accepted: 07/18/2024] [Indexed: 07/27/2024] Open
Abstract
Single-unit (SU) activity-action potentials isolated from one neuron-has traditionally been employed to relate neuronal activity to behavior. However, recent investigations have shown that multiunit (MU) activity-ensemble neural activity recorded within the vicinity of one microelectrode-may also contain accurate estimations of task-related neural population dynamics. Here, using an established model-fitting approach, we compared the spatial codes of SU response fields with corresponding MU response fields recorded from the frontal eye fields (FEFs) in head-unrestrained monkeys (Macaca mulatta) during a memory-guided saccade task. Overall, both SU and MU populations showed a simple visuomotor transformation: the visual response coded target-in-eye coordinates, transitioning progressively during the delay toward a future gaze-in-eye code in the saccade motor response. However, the SU population showed additional secondary codes, including a predictive gaze code in the visual response and retention of a target code in the motor response. Further, when SUs were separated into regular/fast spiking neurons, these cell types showed different spatial code progressions during the late delay period, only converging toward gaze coding during the final saccade motor response. Finally, reconstructing MU populations (by summing SU data within the same sites) failed to replicate either the SU or MU pattern. These results confirm the theoretical and practical potential of MU activity recordings as a biomarker for fundamental sensorimotor transformations (e.g., target-to-gaze coding in the oculomotor system), while also highlighting the importance of SU activity for coding more subtle (e.g., predictive/memory) aspects of sensorimotor behavior.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Serah Seo
- Centre for Vision Research and Centre for Integrative and Applied Neuroscience, York University, Toronto, Ontario M3J 1P3, Canada
| | - Vishal Bharmauria
- Centre for Vision Research and Centre for Integrative and Applied Neuroscience, York University, Toronto, Ontario M3J 1P3, Canada
- Department of Neurosurgery and Brain Repair, Morsani College of Medicine, University of South Florida, Tampa, Florida 33606
| | - Adrian Schütz
- Department of Neurophysics, Philipps-Universität Marburg, 35032 Marburg, Germany
- Center for Mind, Brain, and Behavior - CMBB, Philipps-Universität Marburg, 35032 Marburg, and Justus-Liebig-Universität Giessen, Giessen, Germany
| | - Xiaogang Yan
- Centre for Vision Research and Centre for Integrative and Applied Neuroscience, York University, Toronto, Ontario M3J 1P3, Canada
| | - Hongying Wang
- Centre for Vision Research and Centre for Integrative and Applied Neuroscience, York University, Toronto, Ontario M3J 1P3, Canada
| | - J Douglas Crawford
- Centre for Vision Research and Centre for Integrative and Applied Neuroscience, York University, Toronto, Ontario M3J 1P3, Canada
- Departments of Psychology, Biology, Kinesiology & Health Sciences, York University, Toronto, Ontario M3J 1P3, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Johnston R, Smith MA. Brain-wide arousal signals are segregated from movement planning in the superior colliculus. BIORXIV : THE PREPRINT SERVER FOR BIOLOGY 2024:2024.04.26.591284. [PMID: 38746466 PMCID: PMC11092505 DOI: 10.1101/2024.04.26.591284] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/16/2024]
Abstract
The superior colliculus (SC) is traditionally considered a brain region that functions as an interface between processing visual inputs and generating eye movement outputs. Although its role as a primary reflex center is thought to be conserved across vertebrate species, evidence suggests that the SC has evolved to support higher-order cognitive functions including spatial attention. When it comes to oculomotor areas such as the SC, it is critical that high precision fixation and eye movements are maintained even in the presence of signals related to ongoing changes in cognition and brain state, both of which have the potential to interfere with eye position encoding and movement generation. In this study, we recorded spiking responses of neuronal populations in the SC while monkeys performed a memory-guided saccade task and found that the activity of some of the neurons fluctuated over tens of minutes. By leveraging the statistical power afforded by high-dimensional neuronal recordings, we were able to identify a low-dimensional pattern of activity that was correlated with the subjects' arousal levels. Importantly, we found that the spiking responses of deep-layer SC neurons were less correlated with this brain-wide arousal signal, and that neural activity associated with changes in pupil size and saccade tuning did not overlap in population activity space with movement initiation signals. Taken together, these findings provide a framework for understanding how signals related to cognition and arousal can be embedded in the population activity of oculomotor structures without compromising the fidelity of the motor output.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Richard Johnston
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, USA
- Neuroscience Institute, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, USA
- Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, USA
| | - Matthew A. Smith
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, USA
- Neuroscience Institute, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, USA
- Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, USA
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Pi JS, Fakharian MA, Hage P, Sedaghat-Nejad E, Muller SZ, Shadmehr R. The olivary input to the cerebellum dissociates sensory events from movement plans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2024; 121:e2318849121. [PMID: 38630714 PMCID: PMC11047103 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.2318849121] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/27/2023] [Accepted: 03/19/2024] [Indexed: 04/19/2024] Open
Abstract
Neurons in the inferior olive are thought to anatomically organize the Purkinje cells (P-cells) of the cerebellum into computational modules, but what is computed by each module? Here, we designed a saccade task in marmosets that dissociated sensory events from motor events and then recorded the complex and simple spikes of hundreds of P-cells. We found that when a visual target was presented at a random location, the olive reported the direction of that sensory event to one group of P-cells, but not to a second group. However, just before movement onset, it reported the direction of the planned movement to both groups, even if that movement was not toward the target. At the end of the movement if the subject experienced an error but chose to withhold the corrective movement, only the first group received information about the sensory prediction error. We organized the P-cells based on the information content of their olivary input and found that in the group that received sensory information, the simple spikes were suppressed during fixation, then produced a burst before saccade onset in a direction consistent with assisting the movement. In the second group, the simple spikes were not suppressed during fixation but burst near saccade deceleration in a direction consistent with stopping the movement. Thus, the olive differentiated the P-cells based on whether they would receive sensory or motor information, and this defined their contributions to control of movements as well as holding still.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jay S. Pi
- Laboratory for Computational Motor Control, Department of Biomedical Engineering, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MA21205
| | - Mohammad Amin Fakharian
- Laboratory for Computational Motor Control, Department of Biomedical Engineering, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MA21205
| | - Paul Hage
- Laboratory for Computational Motor Control, Department of Biomedical Engineering, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MA21205
| | - Ehsan Sedaghat-Nejad
- Laboratory for Computational Motor Control, Department of Biomedical Engineering, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MA21205
| | - Salomon Z. Muller
- Zuckerman Mind Brain Behavior Institute, Department of Neuroscience, Columbia University, New York, NY10027
| | - Reza Shadmehr
- Laboratory for Computational Motor Control, Department of Biomedical Engineering, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MA21205
| |
Collapse
|