1
|
Jinsi O, Henderson MM, Tarr MJ. Early experience with low-pass filtered images facilitates visual category learning in a neural network model. PLoS One 2023; 18:e0280145. [PMID: 36608003 PMCID: PMC9821476 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0280145] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/09/2022] [Accepted: 12/21/2022] [Indexed: 01/07/2023] Open
Abstract
Humans are born with very low contrast sensitivity, meaning that inputs to the infant visual system are both blurry and low contrast. Is this solely a byproduct of maturational processes or is there a functional advantage for beginning life with poor visual acuity? We addressed the impact of poor vision during early learning by exploring whether reduced visual acuity facilitated the acquisition of basic-level categories in a convolutional neural network model (CNN), as well as whether any such benefit transferred to subordinate-level category learning. Using the ecoset dataset to simulate basic-level category learning, we manipulated model training curricula along three dimensions: presence of blurred inputs early in training, rate of blur reduction over time, and grayscale versus color inputs. First, a training regime where blur was initially high and was gradually reduced over time-as in human development-improved basic-level categorization performance in a CNN relative to a regime in which non-blurred inputs were used throughout training. Second, when basic-level models were fine-tuned on a task including both basic-level and subordinate-level categories (using the ImageNet dataset), models initially trained with blurred inputs showed a greater performance benefit as compared to models trained exclusively on non-blurred inputs, suggesting that the benefit of blurring generalized from basic-level to subordinate-level categorization. Third, analogous to the low sensitivity to color that infants experience during the first 4-6 months of development, these advantages were observed only when grayscale images were used as inputs. We conclude that poor visual acuity in human newborns may confer functional advantages, including, as demonstrated here, more rapid and accurate acquisition of visual object categories at multiple levels.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Omisa Jinsi
- Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, United States of America
| | - Margaret M. Henderson
- Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, United States of America
- Neuroscience Institute, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, United States of America
- Department of Machine Learning, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, United States of America
| | - Michael J. Tarr
- Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, United States of America
- Neuroscience Institute, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, United States of America
- Department of Machine Learning, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Ross-Sheehy S, Eschman B, Reynolds EE. Seeing and looking: Evidence for developmental and stimulus-dependent changes in infant scanning efficiency. PLoS One 2022; 17:e0274113. [PMID: 36112722 PMCID: PMC9481018 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0274113] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/26/2021] [Accepted: 08/22/2022] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Though previous work has examined infant attention across a variety of tasks, less is known about the individual saccades and fixations that make up each bout of attention, and how individual differences in saccade and fixation patterns (i.e., scanning efficiency) change with development, scene content and perceptual load. To address this, infants between the ages of 5 and 11 months were assessed longitudinally (Experiment 1) and cross-sectionally (Experiment 2). Scanning efficiency (fixation duration, saccade rate, saccade amplitude, and saccade velocity) was assessed while infants viewed six quasi-naturalistic scenes that varied in content (social or non-social) and scene complexity (3, 6 or 9 people/objects). Results from Experiment 1 revealed moderate to strong stability of individual differences in saccade rate, mean fixation duration, and saccade amplitude, and both experiments revealed 5-month-old infants to make larger, faster, and more frequent saccades than older infants. Scanning efficiency was assessed as the relation between fixation duration and saccade amplitude, and results revealed 11-month-olds to have high scanning efficiency across all scenes. However, scanning efficiency also varied with scene content, such that all infants showing higher scanning efficiency when viewing social scenes, and more complex scenes. These results suggest both developmental and stimulus-dependent changes in scanning efficiency, and further highlight the use of saccade and fixation metrics as a sensitive indicator of cognitive processing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Shannon Ross-Sheehy
- Department of Psychology, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, TN, United States of America
- * E-mail:
| | - Bret Eschman
- Department of Psychology, University of Tennessee at Chattanooga, Chattanooga, TN, United States of America
| | - Esther E. Reynolds
- Department of Psychology, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, TN, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Features of Retinal Neurogenesis as a Key Factor of Age-Related Neurodegeneration: Myth or Reality? Int J Mol Sci 2021; 22:ijms22147373. [PMID: 34298993 PMCID: PMC8303671 DOI: 10.3390/ijms22147373] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/17/2021] [Revised: 07/05/2021] [Accepted: 07/05/2021] [Indexed: 11/16/2022] Open
Abstract
Age-related macular degeneration (AMD) is a complex multifactorial neurodegenerative disease that constitutes the most common cause of irreversible blindness in the elderly in the developed countries. Incomplete knowledge about its pathogenesis prevents the search for effective methods of prevention and treatment of AMD, primarily of its "dry" type which is by far the most common (90% of all AMD cases). In the recent years, AMD has become "younger": late stages of the disease are now detected in relatively young people. It is known that AMD pathogenesis-according to the age-related structural and functional changes in the retina-is linked with inflammation, hypoxia, oxidative stress, mitochondrial dysfunction, and an impairment of neurotrophic support, but the mechanisms that trigger the conversion of normal age-related changes to the pathological process as well as the reason for early AMD development remain unclear. In the adult mammalian retina, de novo neurogenesis is very limited. Therefore, the structural and functional features that arise during its maturation and formation can exert long-term effects on further ontogenesis of this tissue. The aim of this review was to discuss possible contributions of the changes/disturbances in retinal neurogenesis to the early development of AMD.
Collapse
|
4
|
Silvestre D, Guy J, Hanck J, Cornish K, Bertone A. Different luminance- and texture-defined contrast sensitivity profiles for school-aged children. Sci Rep 2020; 10:13039. [PMID: 32747677 PMCID: PMC7400652 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-020-69802-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/10/2020] [Accepted: 07/09/2020] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
Abstract
Our current understanding of how the visual brain develops is based largely on the study of luminance-defined information processing. This approach, however, is somewhat limiting, since everyday scenes are composed of complex images, consisting of information characterized by physical attributes relating to both luminance and texture. Few studies have explored how contrast sensitivity to texture-defined information develops, particularly throughout the school-aged years. The current study investigated how contrast sensitivity to luminance- (luminance-modulated noise) and texture-defined (contrast-modulated noise) static gratings develops in school-aged children. Contrast sensitivity functions identified distinct profiles for luminance- and texture-defined gratings across spatial frequencies (SFs) and age. Sensitivity to luminance-defined gratings reached maturity in childhood by the ages of 9–10 years for all SFs (0.5, 1, 2, 4 and 8 cycles/degree or cpd). Sensitivity to texture-defined gratings reached maturity at 5–6 years for low SFs and 7–8 years for high SFs (i.e., 4 cpd). These results establish that the processing of luminance- and texture-defined information develop differently as a function of SF and age.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Daphné Silvestre
- Perceptual Neuroscience Lab (PNLab) for Autism and Development, Department of Education and Counselling Psychology, McGill University, 3700 McTavish Street, Montreal, QC, H3A 1Y2, Canada
| | - Jacalyn Guy
- Perceptual Neuroscience Lab (PNLab) for Autism and Development, Department of Education and Counselling Psychology, McGill University, 3700 McTavish Street, Montreal, QC, H3A 1Y2, Canada.,MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
| | - Julie Hanck
- Integrated Program in Neuroscience, McGill University, Montreal, Canada
| | - Kim Cornish
- Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, Monash University, Melbourne, Australia
| | - Armando Bertone
- Perceptual Neuroscience Lab (PNLab) for Autism and Development, Department of Education and Counselling Psychology, McGill University, 3700 McTavish Street, Montreal, QC, H3A 1Y2, Canada.
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Near vision in individuals with Down syndrome: a vision screening study. Eye (Lond) 2019; 33:1254-1260. [PMID: 30914784 DOI: 10.1038/s41433-019-0402-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/08/2018] [Revised: 02/11/2019] [Accepted: 02/12/2019] [Indexed: 12/31/2022] Open
Abstract
OBJECTIVES Children with Down syndrome are known to have reduced focusing ability for near vision (hypoaccommodation). Through a vision screening study we investigated the correlation between hypoaccommodation and near visual acuity in individuals with Down syndrome. METHODS A cross-sectional vision screening study was conducted on individuals with Down Syndrome. The screening was done in 4 city schools and 1 screening was conducted as a part of the Special Olympics Bharat program. In addition to the conventional vision screening tests, Nott dynamic retinoscopy was also performed. Both adults and children (age < 18 years) were included. RESULTS A total of 55 participants (33 children: age 6 to 17 years, 22 adults: age 18 to 41 years) with Down syndrome were screened. Twenty-two participants had visual impairment. Accommodative accuracy was assessed in 29 children and 13 adults. Accommodative lag ( ≥1.00D) was present in 12 children (41.37%) and 7 adults (53.84%). No correlation was found between the lag of accommodation and near visual acuity (ρSpearman = 0.15, p = 0.54). LogMAR near visual acuity was inversely correlated (ρSpearman = -0.841, p < 0.001) to the near viewing distance. CONCLUSION Near visual acuity by itself is not a sensitive indicator of accommodative dysfunction. In addition, a closer viewing distance may not indicate adequate amplitude of accommodation. These findings strongly suggest the need for including dynamic retinoscopy in the clinical practice while examining individuals with Down syndrome.
Collapse
|
6
|
Buss AT, Ross-Sheehy S, Reynolds GD. Visual working memory in early development: a developmental cognitive neuroscience perspective. J Neurophysiol 2018; 120:1472-1483. [PMID: 29897858 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00087.2018] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
In this article, we review the literature on the development of visual working memory (VWM). We focus on two major periods of development, infancy and early childhood. First, we discuss the innovative methods that have been devised to understand how the development of selective attention and perception provide the foundation of VWM abilities. We detail the behavioral and neural data associated with the development of VWM during infancy. Next, we discuss various signatures of development in VWM during early childhood in the context of spatial and featural memory processes. We focus on the developmental transition to more adult-like VWM properties. Finally, we discuss computational frameworks that have explained the complex patterns of behavior observed in VWM tasks from infancy to adulthood and attempt to explain links between measures of infant VWM and childhood VWM.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Aaron T Buss
- Department of Psychology, University of Tennessee , Knoxville, Tennessee
| | | | - Greg D Reynolds
- Department of Psychology, University of Tennessee , Knoxville, Tennessee
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Rider AT, Coutrot A, Pellicano E, Dakin SC, Mareschal I. Semantic content outweighs low-level saliency in determining children's and adults' fixation of movies. J Exp Child Psychol 2018; 166:293-309. [PMID: 28972928 PMCID: PMC5710995 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2017.09.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/11/2016] [Revised: 08/21/2017] [Accepted: 09/05/2017] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
Abstract
To make sense of the visual world, we need to move our eyes to focus regions of interest on the high-resolution fovea. Eye movements, therefore, give us a way to infer mechanisms of visual processing and attention allocation. Here, we examined age-related differences in visual processing by recording eye movements from 37 children (aged 6-14years) and 10 adults while viewing three 5-min dynamic video clips taken from child-friendly movies. The data were analyzed in two complementary ways: (a) gaze based and (b) content based. First, similarity of scanpaths within and across age groups was examined using three different measures of variance (dispersion, clusters, and distance from center). Second, content-based models of fixation were compared to determine which of these provided the best account of our dynamic data. We found that the variance in eye movements decreased as a function of age, suggesting common attentional orienting. Comparison of the different models revealed that a model that relies on faces generally performed better than the other models tested, even for the youngest age group (<10years). However, the best predictor of a given participant's eye movements was the average of all other participants' eye movements both within the same age group and in different age groups. These findings have implications for understanding how children attend to visual information and highlight similarities in viewing strategies across development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Andrew T Rider
- UCL Institute of Ophthalmology, University College London, London WC1E 6BT, UK.
| | - Antoine Coutrot
- Centre for Mathematics and Physics in Life Sciences and Experimental Biology (CoMPLEX), University College London, London WC1E 6BT, UK
| | - Elizabeth Pellicano
- Centre for Research in Autism and Education (CRAE), Department of Psychology and Human Development, UCL Institute of Education, University College London, London WC1H 0AL, UK; School of Psychology, University of Western Australia, Crawley, Perth, Western Australia 6009, Australia
| | - Steven C Dakin
- UCL Institute of Ophthalmology, University College London, London WC1E 6BT, UK; School of Optometry and Vision Science, University of Auckland, Auckland 1010, New Zealand
| | | |
Collapse
|
8
|
Császár-Nagy N, Bókkon I. Mother-newborn separation at birth in hospitals: A possible risk for neurodevelopmental disorders? Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2018; 84:337-351. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.08.013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 39] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/04/2017] [Revised: 06/23/2017] [Accepted: 08/20/2017] [Indexed: 12/11/2022]
|
9
|
Pei F, Baldassi S, Tsai JJ, Gerhard HE, Norcia AM. Development of contrast normalization mechanisms during childhood and adolescence. Vision Res 2017; 133:12-20. [PMID: 27826013 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2016.03.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/15/2015] [Revised: 03/21/2016] [Accepted: 03/28/2016] [Indexed: 01/15/2023]
Abstract
Contrast sensitivity is regulated by neural mechanisms that flexibly adjust responsiveness to optimize stimulus encoding across different environments. Here we studied the developmental status of gain control mechanisms in school-age children (5-17years) and adults using a visual masking paradigm. A variable contrast, spatially random 2-D noise test pattern was masked by the presence of a superimposed independent noise pattern presented at 0, 12 and 40% contrast. Frequency-tagged steady state visual evoked potentials were used to separately record responses to the test (5.14Hz) and the mask (7.2Hz). By incrementally increasing the test contrast we measured contrast response functions for each mask contrast. The unmasked contrast response functions were largely similar in shape across age, but peak amplitude was higher in the children. Masking shifted the contrast response function rightward on the contrast axis in both the adults and older children, elevating contrast thresholds by a similar factor across age. However, in younger children, masking resulted in a change in the slope of the contrast response function. These findings suggest that immaturity in the contrast normalization process persists until approximately 11years of age.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Francesca Pei
- Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, United States; Stanford Autism Center at Packard Children's Hospital, Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, United States.
| | - Stefano Baldassi
- Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, United States; Department of Neuroscience, Psychology, Pharmacology and Child Health, University of Florence, Italy
| | - Jeffrey J Tsai
- Department of Neurology, University of Washington, Seattle, WA, United States
| | - Holly E Gerhard
- Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, United States
| | - Anthony M Norcia
- Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, United States
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Abstract
Sensory systems continuously mold themselves to the widely varying contexts in which they must operate. Studies of these adaptations have played a long and central role in vision science. In part this is because the specific adaptations remain a powerful tool for dissecting vision, by exposing the mechanisms that are adapting. That is, "if it adapts, it's there." Many insights about vision have come from using adaptation in this way, as a method. A second important trend has been the realization that the processes of adaptation are themselves essential to how vision works, and thus are likely to operate at all levels. That is, "if it's there, it adapts." This has focused interest on the mechanisms of adaptation as the target rather than the probe. Together both approaches have led to an emerging insight of adaptation as a fundamental and ubiquitous coding strategy impacting all aspects of how we see.
Collapse
|
11
|
Wan MJ, VanderVeen DK. Eye disorders in newborn infants (excluding retinopathy of prematurity). Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed 2015; 100:F264-9. [PMID: 25395469 DOI: 10.1136/archdischild-2014-306215] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/16/2014] [Accepted: 10/22/2014] [Indexed: 01/01/2023]
Abstract
A screening eye examination is an essential part of the newborn assessment. The detection of many ocular disorders in newborn infants can be achieved through careful observation of the infant's visual behaviour and the use of a direct ophthalmoscope to assess the ocular structures and check the red reflex. Early diagnosis and subspecialty referral can have a critical impact on the prognosis for many ocular conditions, including potentially blinding but treatable conditions such as congenital cataracts, life-threatening malignancies such as retinoblastoma and harbingers of disease elsewhere such as sporadic aniridia and its association with the development of Wilms tumour.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michael J Wan
- Department of Ophthalmology, Boston Children's Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts, USA
| | - Deborah K VanderVeen
- Department of Ophthalmology, Boston Children's Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts, USA
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Brown AM, Lindsey DT, Cammenga JG, Giannone PJ, Stenger MR. The contrast sensitivity of the newborn human infant. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 2015; 56:625-32. [PMID: 25564453 DOI: 10.1167/iovs.14-14757] [Citation(s) in RCA: 43] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
PURPOSE To measure the binocular contrast sensitivity (CS) of newborn infants using a fixation-and-following card procedure. METHODS The CS of 119 healthy newborn infants was measured using stimuli printed on cards under the descending method of limits (93 infants) and randomized/masked designs (26 infants). One experienced and one novice adult observer tested the infants using vertical square-wave gratings (0.06 and 0.10 cyc/deg; 20/10,000 and 20/6000 nominal Snellen equivalent); the experienced observer also tested using horizontal gratings (0.10 cyc/deg) and using the Method of Constant Stimuli while being kept unaware of the stimulus values. RESULTS The CS of the newborn infant was 2.0 (contrast threshold = 0.497; 95% confidence interval: 0.475-0.524) for vertically oriented gratings and 1.74 (threshold = 0.575; 95% confidence interval: 0.523-0.633) for horizontally oriented gratings (P < 0.0006). The standard deviation of infant CS was comparable to that obtained by others on adults using the Pelli-Robson chart. The two observers showed similar practice effects. Randomization of stimulus order and masking of the adult observer had no effect on CS. CONCLUSIONS The CS of individual newborn human infants can be measured using a fixation-and-following card procedure.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Angela M Brown
- College of Optometry, The Ohio State University, Columbus, Ohio, United States
| | - Delwin T Lindsey
- College of Optometry, The Ohio State University, Columbus, Ohio, United States Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University, Mansfield, Columbus, Ohio, United States
| | - Joanna G Cammenga
- College of Optometry, The Ohio State University, Columbus, Ohio, United States
| | - Peter J Giannone
- College of Medicine, The Ohio State University, Columbus, Ohio, United States Kentucky Children's Hospital, University of Kentucky College of Medicine, Lexington, Kentucky, United States
| | - Michael R Stenger
- College of Medicine, The Ohio State University, Columbus, Ohio, United States Nationwide Children's Hospital, Columbus, Ohio, United States
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
La vision du tout jeune enfant. ENFANCE 2014. [DOI: 10.4074/s0013754514001037] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
|
14
|
Ma L, Yan SF, Huang YM, Lu XR, Qian F, Pang HL, Xu XR, Zou ZY, Dong PC, Xiao X, Wang X, Sun TT, Dou HL, Lin XM. Effect of Lutein and Zeaxanthin on Macular Pigment and Visual Function in Patients with Early Age-related Macular Degeneration. Ophthalmology 2012; 119:2290-7. [DOI: 10.1016/j.ophtha.2012.06.014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 91] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/08/2011] [Revised: 06/09/2012] [Accepted: 06/11/2012] [Indexed: 10/28/2022] Open
|
15
|
Madan A, Norcia AM, Hou C, Pettet MW, Good WV. Effect of Grade I and II intraventricular hemorrhage on visuocortical function in very low birth weight infants. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2012; 25:143-54. [PMID: 22371027 DOI: 10.1163/187847612x626381] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
The neurological outcome for infants with Grade I/II intraventricular hemorrhage (IVH) is debated. The aim of this study was to determine whether very low birth weight infants (VLBW, <1500 g) with Grade I/II (IVH) have altered visuocortical activity compared with infants with no IVH. We assessed the quantitative swept parameter visual evoked potential (sVEP) responses evoked by three different visual stimuli. Data from 52 VLBW infants were compared with data from 13 infants with Grade I or II IVH, enrolled at 5-7 months corrected age. Acuity thresholds and suprathreshold response amplitudes were compared. Grating acuity (GA), contrast sensitivity (CS) and vernier acuity (VA) were each worse in the Grade I/II IVH compared with the no IVH groups (8.24 cpd in IVH group vs. 13.07 cpd in no IVH group for GA; 1.44% vs. 1.18% for CS and 1.55 arcmin vs. 0.58 arcmin for VA). The slopes of the response amplitude for CS and VA were significantly lower in IVH infants. The spatial frequency tuning function was shifted downward on the spatial frequency axis, without a change in slope. These results indicate that Grade I/II IVH are associated with deleterious effects on cortical vision development and function.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ashima Madan
- Stanford University School of Medline, Stanford, CA 94305, USA
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
16
|
Abstract
Visual coding is a highly dynamic process and continuously adapting to the current viewing context. The perceptual changes that result from adaptation to recently viewed stimuli remain a powerful and popular tool for analyzing sensory mechanisms and plasticity. Over the last decade, the footprints of this adaptation have been tracked to both higher and lower levels of the visual pathway and over a wider range of timescales, revealing that visual processing is much more adaptable than previously thought. This work has also revealed that the pattern of aftereffects is similar across many stimulus dimensions, pointing to common coding principles in which adaptation plays a central role. However, why visual coding adapts has yet to be fully answered.
Collapse
|
17
|
Infant and Child Vision Research: Present Status and Future Directions. Optom Vis Sci 2009; 86:559-60. [DOI: 10.1097/opx.0b013e3181aa06d5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/26/2022] Open
|