1
|
Rowe JM, Cramer BC, Boe SG. Even with exposure to errors, motor imagery cannot update internal models. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2025; 89:104. [PMID: 40434577 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-025-02138-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/23/2025] [Accepted: 05/18/2025] [Indexed: 05/29/2025]
Abstract
Recent evidence suggests that motor imagery is insufficient for updating internal models, essential for predicting and refining overt movement outcomes. The covert nature of motor imagery limits exposure to errors, perhaps preventing the updating of internal models. To explore this, 90 participants were exposed to a prism that shifted vision leftward, completing 20 physical pointing trials followed by either 230 more physical pointing trials [physical practice (PP)], 230 imagined pointing trials [physical practice motor imagery (PP-MI)], 230 unrelated task trials [physical practice control (PP-CTRL)], or no further trials [physical practice none (PP-None)]. We hypothesized that if exposure to errors is needed for motor imagery to update internal models, then PP-MI would exhibit aftereffects, characterized by pointing opposite to the prism shift (i.e., rightwards), similar to PP, but differing from PP-CTRL and PP-None. After prism exposure, all groups showed significant aftereffects (PP: 4.73° ± 2.12°, PP-MI: 2.62 ± 1.61, PP-CTRL: 2.58 ± 1.53, PP-None: 3.11 ± 1.68), however there were no significant differences in the magnitude of aftereffects between PP-MI, and PP-CTRL/PP-None. Our findings demonstrate that motor imagery alone is insufficient for updating internal models, even when participants are initially exposed to errors under a prism shift. This further reinforces that motor imagery is not a direct simulation of overt movement, as proposed by Motor Simulation Theory- the foundation for its use in rehabilitation. Deepening our understanding of how learning occurs through motor imagery is crucial for enhancing its effectiveness in practical applications like rehabilitation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Juliet M Rowe
- Laboratory for Brain Recovery and Function, Dalhousie University, Halifax, NS, Canada
- School of Physiotherapy, Dalhousie University, Rm 407, 4th Floor Forrest Building 5869 University Avenue, PO Box 15000, Halifax, NS, B3H 4R2, Canada
| | - Brooke C Cramer
- Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Dalhousie University, Halifax, NS, Canada
| | - Shaun G Boe
- Laboratory for Brain Recovery and Function, Dalhousie University, Halifax, NS, Canada.
- School of Physiotherapy, Dalhousie University, Rm 407, 4th Floor Forrest Building 5869 University Avenue, PO Box 15000, Halifax, NS, B3H 4R2, Canada.
- School of Health and Human Performance, Dalhousie University, Halifax, NS, Canada.
- Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Dalhousie University, Halifax, NS, Canada.
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Rowe JM, Boe SG. Unlike overt movement, motor imagery cannot update internal models. Brain Cogn 2024; 181:106219. [PMID: 39241457 DOI: 10.1016/j.bandc.2024.106219] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/08/2024] [Revised: 08/31/2024] [Accepted: 09/01/2024] [Indexed: 09/09/2024]
Abstract
In overt movement, internal models make predictions about the sensory consequences of a desired movement, generating the appropriate motor commands to achieve that movement. Using available sensory feedback, internal models are updated to allow for movement adaptation and in-turn better performance. Whether internal models are updated during motor imagery, the mental rehearsal of movement, is not well established. To investigate internal modelling during motor imagery, 66 participants were exposed to a leftwards prism shift while performing actual pointing movements (physical practice; PP), imagined pointing movements (motor imagery; MI), or no pointing movements (control). If motor imagery updates internal models, we hypothesized that aftereffects (pointing in the direction opposite the prism shift) would be observed in MI, like that of PP, and unlike that of control. After prism exposure, the magnitude of aftereffects was significant in PP (4.73° ± 1.56°), but not in MI (0.34° ± 0.96°) and control (0.34° ± 1.04°). Accordingly, PP differed significantly from MI and control. Our results show that motor imagery does not update internal models, suggesting that it is not a direct simulation of overt movement. Furthering our understanding of the mechanisms that underlie learning through motor imagery will lead to more effective applications of motor imagery.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Juliet M Rowe
- Laboratory for Brain Recovery and Function, Dalhousie University, Halifax, NS, Canada; School of Physiotherapy, Dalhousie University, Halifax, NS, Canada
| | - Shaun G Boe
- Laboratory for Brain Recovery and Function, Dalhousie University, Halifax, NS, Canada; School of Physiotherapy, Dalhousie University, Halifax, NS, Canada; School of Health and Human Performance, Dalhousie University, Halifax, NS, Canada; Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Dalhousie University, Halifax, NS, Canada.
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Gentile AE, Rinella S, Desogus E, Verrelli CM, Iosa M, Perciavalle V, Ruggieri M, Polizzi A. Motor imagery for paediatric neurorehabilitation: how much do we know? Perspectives from a systematic review. Front Hum Neurosci 2024; 18:1245707. [PMID: 38571523 PMCID: PMC10987782 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2024.1245707] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/23/2023] [Accepted: 02/28/2024] [Indexed: 04/05/2024] Open
Abstract
Background Motor Imagery (MI) is a cognitive process consisting in mental simulation of body movements without executing physical actions: its clinical use has been investigated prevalently in adults with neurological disorders. Objectives Review of the best-available evidence on the use and efficacy of MI interventions for neurorehabilitation purposes in common and rare childhood neurological disorders. Methods systematic literature search conducted according to PRISMA by using the Scopus, PsycArticles, Cinahl, PUBMED, Web of Science (Clarivate), EMBASE, PsychINFO, and COCHRANE databases, with levels of evidence scored by OCEBM and PEDro Scales. Results Twenty-two original studies were retrieved and included for the analysis; MI was the unique or complementary rehabilitative treatment in 476 individuals (aged 5 to 18 years) with 10 different neurological conditions including, cerebral palsies, stroke, coordination disorders, intellectual disabilities, brain and/or spinal cord injuries, autism, pain syndromes, and hyperactivity. The sample size ranged from single case reports to cohorts and control groups. Treatment lasted 2 days to 6 months with 1 to 24 sessions. MI tasks were conventional, graded or ad-hoc. MI measurement tools included movement assessment batteries, mental chronometry tests, scales, and questionnaires, EEG, and EMG. Overall, the use of MI was stated as effective in 19/22, and uncertain in the remnant studies. Conclusion MI could be a reliable supportive/add-on (home-based) rehabilitative tool for pediatric neurorehabilitation; its clinical use, in children, is highly dependent on the complexity of MI mechanisms, which are related to the underlying neurodevelopmental disorder.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Amalia Egle Gentile
- National Centre for Rare Diseases, Istituto Superiore di Sanità (ISS), Rome, Italy
| | - Sergio Rinella
- Department of Educational Science, Chair of Pediatrics, University of Catania, Catania, Italy
| | - Eleonora Desogus
- National Centre for Rare Diseases, Istituto Superiore di Sanità (ISS), Rome, Italy
| | | | - Marco Iosa
- Department of Psychology, Faculty of Medicine and Psychology, Sapienza University of Rome, Rome, Italy
- Santa Lucia Foundation (IRCCS), Rome, Italy
| | | | - Martino Ruggieri
- Unit of Clinical Pediatrics, Department of Clinical and Experimental Medicine, University of Catania, Catania, Italy
| | - Agata Polizzi
- Department of Educational Science, Chair of Pediatrics, University of Catania, Catania, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Fleury L, Dreyer L, El Makkaoui R, Leroy E, Rossetti Y, Collet C. Inter-Task Transfer of Prism Adaptation through Motor Imagery. Brain Sci 2023; 13:brainsci13010114. [PMID: 36672095 PMCID: PMC9857236 DOI: 10.3390/brainsci13010114] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/30/2022] [Revised: 12/30/2022] [Accepted: 01/04/2023] [Indexed: 01/10/2023] Open
Abstract
Prism adaptation (PA) is a useful method to investigate short-term sensorimotor plasticity. Following active exposure to prisms, individuals show consistent after-effects, probing that they have adapted to the perturbation. Whether after-effects are transferable to another task or remain specific to the task performed under exposure, represents a crucial interest to understand the adaptive processes at work. Motor imagery (MI, i.e., the mental representation of an action without any concomitant execution) offers an original opportunity to investigate the role of cognitive aspects of motor command preparation disregarding actual sensory and motor information related to its execution. The aim of the study was to test whether prism adaptation through MI led to transferable after-effects. Forty-four healthy volunteers were exposed to a rightward prismatic deviation while performing actual (Active group) versus imagined (MI group) pointing movements, or while being inactive (inactive group). Upon prisms removal, in the MI group, only participants with the highest MI abilities (MI+ group) showed consistent after-effects on pointing and, crucially, a significant transfer to throwing. This was not observed in participants with lower MI abilities and in the inactive group. However, a direct comparison of pointing after-effects and transfer to throwing between MI+ and the control inactive group did not show any significant difference. Although this interpretation requires caution, these findings suggest that exposure to intersensory conflict might be responsible for sensory realignment during prism adaptation which could be transferred to another task. This study paves the way for further investigations into MI's potential to develop robust sensorimotor adaptation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lisa Fleury
- INSERM UMR-S U1028, CNRS UMS 5292, Trajectoires Lyon Neuroscience Research Center (CRNL), 69500 Bron, France. Claude Bernard University of Lyon 1, 69100 Villeurbanne, France
- Inter-University Laboratory of Human Movement Biology (EA 7424), Claude Bernard University of Lyon 1, 69100 Villeurbanne, France
- Defitech Chair of Clinical Neuroengineering, Neuro-X Institute (INX) and Brain Mind Institute (BMI), École Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne (EPFL) Valais, 1950 Sion, Switzerland
- Correspondence:
| | - Léa Dreyer
- Department of Psychology, University of Lyon 2, 69007 Lyon, France
| | - Rola El Makkaoui
- Department of Psychology, University of Lyon 2, 69007 Lyon, France
| | - Elise Leroy
- Department of Psychology, University of Lyon 2, 69007 Lyon, France
| | - Yves Rossetti
- INSERM UMR-S U1028, CNRS UMS 5292, Trajectoires Lyon Neuroscience Research Center (CRNL), 69500 Bron, France. Claude Bernard University of Lyon 1, 69100 Villeurbanne, France
- “Mouvement et Handicap” Platform, Neurological Hospital, Hospices Civils de Lyon, 69500 Bron, France
| | - Christian Collet
- Inter-University Laboratory of Human Movement Biology (EA 7424), Claude Bernard University of Lyon 1, 69100 Villeurbanne, France
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Sawai S, Fujikawa S, Ushio R, Tamura K, Ohsumi C, Yamamoto R, Murata S, Nakano H. Repetitive Peripheral Magnetic Stimulation Combined with Motor Imagery Changes Resting-State EEG Activity: A Randomized Controlled Trial. Brain Sci 2022; 12:1548. [PMID: 36421872 PMCID: PMC9688706 DOI: 10.3390/brainsci12111548] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/14/2022] [Revised: 11/12/2022] [Accepted: 11/14/2022] [Indexed: 01/03/2025] Open
Abstract
Repetitive peripheral magnetic stimulation is a novel non-invasive technique for applying repetitive magnetic stimulation to the peripheral nerves and muscles. Contrarily, a person imagines that he/she is exercising during motor imagery. Resting-state electroencephalography can evaluate the ability of motor imagery; however, the effects of motor imagery and repetitive peripheral magnetic stimulation on resting-state electroencephalography are unknown. We examined the effects of motor imagery and repetitive peripheral magnetic stimulation on the vividness of motor imagery and resting-state electroencephalography. The participants were divided into a motor imagery group and motor imagery and repetitive peripheral magnetic stimulation group. They performed 60 motor imagery tasks involving wrist dorsiflexion movement. In the motor imagery and repetitive peripheral magnetic stimulation group, we applied repetitive peripheral magnetic stimulation to the extensor carpi radialis longus muscle during motor imagery. We measured the vividness of motor imagery and resting-state electroencephalography before and after the task. Both groups displayed a significant increase in the vividness of motor imagery. The motor imagery and repetitive peripheral magnetic stimulation group exhibited increased β activity in the anterior cingulate cortex by source localization for electroencephalography. Hence, combined motor imagery and repetitive peripheral magnetic stimulation changes the resting-state electroencephalography activity and may promote motor imagery.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Shun Sawai
- Graduate School of Health Sciences, Kyoto Tachibana University, Kyoto 607-8175, Japan
- Department of Rehabilitation, Kyoto Kuno Hospital, Kyoto 605-0981, Japan
| | - Shoya Fujikawa
- Department of Physical Therapy, Faculty of Health Sciences, Kyoto Tachibana University, Kyoto 607-8175, Japan
| | - Ryu Ushio
- Department of Physical Therapy, Faculty of Health Sciences, Kyoto Tachibana University, Kyoto 607-8175, Japan
| | - Kosuke Tamura
- Department of Physical Therapy, Faculty of Health Sciences, Kyoto Tachibana University, Kyoto 607-8175, Japan
| | - Chihiro Ohsumi
- Department of Physical Therapy, Faculty of Health Sciences, Kyoto Tachibana University, Kyoto 607-8175, Japan
| | - Ryosuke Yamamoto
- Department of Rehabilitation, Tesseikai Neurosurgical Hospital, Osaka 575-8511, Japan
| | - Shin Murata
- Graduate School of Health Sciences, Kyoto Tachibana University, Kyoto 607-8175, Japan
- Department of Physical Therapy, Faculty of Health Sciences, Kyoto Tachibana University, Kyoto 607-8175, Japan
| | - Hideki Nakano
- Graduate School of Health Sciences, Kyoto Tachibana University, Kyoto 607-8175, Japan
- Department of Physical Therapy, Faculty of Health Sciences, Kyoto Tachibana University, Kyoto 607-8175, Japan
| |
Collapse
|