1
|
Forschack N, Oxner M, Müller MM. The consequences of color chromaticity on electrophysiological measures of attentional deployment in visual search. iScience 2025; 28:112252. [PMID: 40241762 PMCID: PMC12002620 DOI: 10.1016/j.isci.2025.112252] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/07/2024] [Revised: 12/17/2024] [Accepted: 03/17/2025] [Indexed: 04/18/2025] Open
Abstract
We investigated to what extent color vividness of visual items influences how humans prioritize information in a search task. For this, color chromaticity was manipulated over two search experiments. While recording the electroencephalogram, participants searched for a shape of certain color among three other shapes, when it emerged from a stream of flickering gray placeholders. Each location was tagged with a specific frequency evoking the steady-state-visual-evoked potential (SSVEP) allowing to track attentional deployment on multiple items. Color vividness boosted SSVEP amplitudes independent of item type, i.e., targets or distractors, while leaving other measures of attentional deployment-event-related potentials and alpha-band amplitudes-mostly unaffected. Interestingly, relative modulation of target and distractor SSVEP amplitudes was comparable between experiments suggesting similar attentional deployment. The results highlight that attentional deployment to search items depends on low-level stimulus features that need to be controlled to allow for inferences about capture or suppression of individual items.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Norman Forschack
- Wilhelm Wundt Department of Psychology, University of Leipzig, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Matt Oxner
- Wilhelm Wundt Department of Psychology, University of Leipzig, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Matthias M. Müller
- Wilhelm Wundt Department of Psychology, University of Leipzig, Leipzig, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Janczyk M, Mackenzie IG, Koob V. A comment on the Revised Diffusion Model for Conflict tasks (RDMC). Psychon Bull Rev 2025; 32:690-704. [PMID: 39448516 PMCID: PMC12000171 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-024-02574-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 08/20/2024] [Indexed: 10/26/2024]
Abstract
In conflict tasks, such as the Simon, Eriksen flanker, or Stroop task, a relevant and an irrelevant feature indicate the same or different responses in congruent and incongruent trials, respectively. The congruency effect refers to faster and less error-prone responses in congruent relative to incongruent trials. Distributional analyses reveal that the congruency effect in the Simon task becomes smaller with increasing RTs, reflected by a negative-going delta function. In contrast, for other tasks, the delta function is typically positive-going, meaning that congruency effects become larger with increasing RTs. The Diffusion Model for Conflict tasks (DMC; Ulrich et al., Cognitive Psychology, 78, 148-174, 2015) accounts for this by explicitly modeling the information accumulated from the relevant and the irrelevant features and attributes negatively- versus positively-sloped delta functions to different peak times of a pulse-like activation resulting from the task-irrelevant feature. Because the underlying function implies negative drift rates, Lee and Sewell (Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 31(5), 1-31, 2024) recently questioned this assumption and suggested their Revised Diffusion Model for Conflict tasks (RDMC). We address three issues regarding RDMC compared to DMC: (1) The pulse-like function is not as implausible as Lee and Sewell suggest. (2) RDMC itself comes with a questionable assumption that different parameters are required for congruent and incongruent trials. (3) Moreover, we present data from a new parameter recovery study, suggesting that RDMC lacks acceptable recovery of several parameters (in particular compared to DMC). In this light, we discuss RDMC as not (yet) a revised version of DMC.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Markus Janczyk
- Psychological Research Methods and Cognitive Psychology, Department of Psychology, University of Bremen, Hochschulring 18, D-28359, Bremen, Germany.
| | | | - Valentin Koob
- Psychological Research Methods and Cognitive Psychology, Department of Psychology, University of Bremen, Hochschulring 18, D-28359, Bremen, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Kerzel D. Electrophysiological evidence for the optimal tuning of attention. Sci Rep 2025; 15:6908. [PMID: 40011619 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-025-90687-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/16/2024] [Accepted: 02/14/2025] [Indexed: 02/28/2025] Open
Abstract
Optimal tuning of attention refers to shifts in goal-driven attention that increase the difference between the representation of the target and nontarget features. Evidence for optimal tuning comes from studies measuring the memory representation of the target and, to a lesser degree, from studies measuring attentional selectivity. In one study on attentional selectivity, cueing effects were found to be greater for cue colors deviating away from the nontarget color compared to cue colors deviating toward the nontarget color, suggesting that participants' search goal was optimally tuned. To address alternative accounts, we measured event-related potentials (ERPs) elicited by different cue colors at posterior electrodes PO7/PO8. We found that ERPs associated with attentional orienting (N1pc) or selection (N2pc) were larger for cue colors deviating away from the nontarget color, which is consistent with the optimal tuning of attention. In contrast, the results are difficult to reconcile with alternative accounts such as rapid disengagement or object updating. Further, we aimed to evaluate contributions from sensory adaptation by analyzing the Ppc component, a lateralized ERP in the P1 time range. Two control conditions, however, suggested that the Ppc was more likely driven by imbalanced saliency than sensory adaptation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dirk Kerzel
- Department of Psychology, Faculté de Psychologie et des Sciences de l'Éducation, Université de Genève, 40 Boulevard du Pont d'Arve, Geneva, Switzerland.
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Hickey C, Grignolio D, Munasinghe V, Acunzo D. Using N2pc variability to probe functionality: Linear mixed modelling of trial EEG and behaviour. Biol Psychol 2025; 195:108987. [PMID: 39855539 DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2025.108987] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/19/2024] [Revised: 01/07/2025] [Accepted: 01/15/2025] [Indexed: 01/27/2025]
Abstract
This paper has two concurrent goals. On one hand, we hope it will serve as a simple primer in the use of linear mixed modelling (LMM) for inferential statistical analysis of multimodal data. We describe how LMM can be easily adopted for the identification of trial-wise relationships between disparate measures and provide a brief cookbook for assessing the suitability of LMM in your analyses. On the other hand, this paper is an empirical report, probing how trial-wise variance in the N2pc, and specifically its sub-component the NT, can be predicted by manual reaction time (RT) and stimuli parameters. Extant work has identified a link between N2pc and RT that has been interpreted as evidence of a direct and causative relationship. However, results have left open the less-interesting possibility that the measures covary as a function of motivation or arousal. Using LMM, we demonstrate that the relationship only emerges when the NT is elicited by targets, not distractors, suggesting a discrete and functional relationship. In other analyses, we find that the target-elicited NT is sensitive to variance in distractor identity even when the distractor cannot itself elicit consistently lateralized brain activity. The NT thus appears closely linked to attentional target processing, supporting the propagation of target-related information to response preparation and execution. At the same time, we find that this component is sensitive to distractor interference, which leaves open the possibility that NT reflects brain activity responsible for the suppression of irrelevant distractor information.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Clayton Hickey
- Center for Human Brain Health and School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, UK.
| | - Damiano Grignolio
- Center for Human Brain Health and School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, UK
| | - Vinura Munasinghe
- Center for Human Brain Health and School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, UK
| | - David Acunzo
- Center for Human Brain Health and School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, UK
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Li Z, Guo W, Zhao N, Liu Q. The Suppression Mechanisms of Passive Memory in Visual Working Memory: The Evidence from Electroencephalography. J Cogn Neurosci 2025; 37:334-344. [PMID: 39436256 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_02265] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/23/2024]
Abstract
Recent studies of visual working memory (VWM) underscore a structured hierarchy of storage states. Memories that are not immediately relevant to the task at hand but are essential for later use are transferred to a passive state, which operates independently of actively maintaining and manipulating current memories. Note that stimulating passive memory forcefully can reactivate it into an active state, resulting in a competition with active memory. Thus, to remain stable representations for both states within VWM, passive memory might involve sustained suppression during activity-silent maintenance to prevent reactivation from disrupting the current active storage. To investigate this, we analyzed lateralized EEG signals while human participants (both women and men) were engaged in a sequential presentation memory task across two experiments. The results revealed positive contralateral delayed activity components and lateralized alpha enhancement for passive memory, neural indicative of suppression on passive storage. In addition, the suppression effect was independent of the memory load in both the active and the passive states. These findings support the notion of sustained suppression during activity-silent maintenance of passive memory, facilitating the stable maintenance of distinct storage states and advancing our understanding of the dynamic coding framework in VWM.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ziyuan Li
- Sichuan Normal University
- Anyang Normal University
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
6
|
Sapountzis P, Antoniadou A, Gregoriou GG. Diverse neuronal activity patterns contribute to the control of distraction in the prefrontal and parietal cortex. PLoS Biol 2025; 23:e3003008. [PMID: 39869632 PMCID: PMC11801722 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pbio.3003008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/24/2024] [Revised: 02/06/2025] [Accepted: 01/08/2025] [Indexed: 01/29/2025] Open
Abstract
Goal-directed behavior requires the effective suppression of distractions to focus on the task at hand. Although experimental evidence suggests that brain areas in the prefrontal and parietal lobe contribute to the selection of task-relevant and the suppression of task-irrelevant stimuli, how conspicuous distractors are encoded and effectively ignored remains poorly understood. We recorded neuronal responses from 2 regions in the prefrontal and parietal cortex of macaques, the frontal eye field (FEF) and the lateral intraparietal (LIP) area, during a visual search task, in the presence and absence of a salient distractor. We found that in both areas, salient distractors are encoded by both response enhancement and suppression by distinct neuronal populations. In FEF, a larger proportion of units displayed suppression of responses to the salient distractor compared to LIP, with suppression effects in FEF being correlated with search time. Moreover, in FEF but not in LIP, the suppression for the salient distractor compared to non-salient distractors that shared the target color could not be accounted for by an enhancement of target features. These results reveal a distinct contribution of FEF in the suppression of salient distractors. Critically, we found that in both areas, the population level representations of the target and singleton locations were not orthogonal, suggesting a mechanism of interference from salient stimuli.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Panagiotis Sapountzis
- Institute of Applied and Computational Mathematics, Foundation for Research and Technology Hellas, Heraklion, Crete, Greece
- Department of Basic Sciences, Faculty of Medicine, University of Crete, Heraklion, Crete, Greece
| | - Alexandra Antoniadou
- Institute of Applied and Computational Mathematics, Foundation for Research and Technology Hellas, Heraklion, Crete, Greece
- Department of Basic Sciences, Faculty of Medicine, University of Crete, Heraklion, Crete, Greece
| | - Georgia G. Gregoriou
- Institute of Applied and Computational Mathematics, Foundation for Research and Technology Hellas, Heraklion, Crete, Greece
- Department of Basic Sciences, Faculty of Medicine, University of Crete, Heraklion, Crete, Greece
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Werle D, Sablottny L, Ansorge U, Biehl SC, Tuschen-Caffier B, Svaldi J. Attention to food stimuli in binge eating disorder: Electrophysiological evidence. Appetite 2024; 203:107682. [PMID: 39303828 DOI: 10.1016/j.appet.2024.107682] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/01/2024] [Revised: 08/09/2024] [Accepted: 09/16/2024] [Indexed: 09/22/2024]
Abstract
Attentional biases towards food play an important role in the pathology of binge eating disorder (BED). Later stage electrophysiological potentials (P300, late positive potential) present promising markers of motivated attention with high temporal, albeit low spatial resolution. Complementing this, the N2pc is an earlier-latency component providing the possibility of more directly analyzing visuospatial attention. Therefore, we tested a group with BED (N = 60), as well as an overweight (OW; N = 28) and normal weight (NW; N = 30) group without BED in a Go/No-Go paradigm using food and nonfood distractor images. Only the OW group in exclusively the Go trials displayed a stronger spatial attention allocation towards nonfood distractors as evidenced by an increased N2pc amplitude. In the P300's time window, the OW group displayed no attentional bias towards food and the NW group only did so in the absence of a target. Solely the BED group allocated more motivated attention towards food distractors both in Go and No-Go trials. In the following late positive potential (LPP), the OW group exhibited a general attentional bias towards food distractors, while the BED group only did so in the absence of a target. These results are discussed in light of the incentive sensitization theory and a potential early attentional suppression of potent distractors.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dustin Werle
- University of Tuebingen, Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Germany.
| | - Lynn Sablottny
- University of Freiburg, Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Germany
| | | | - Stefanie C Biehl
- University of Tuebingen, Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Germany
| | | | - Jennifer Svaldi
- University of Tuebingen, Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Ma X, Abrams RA. Bias-free measure of distractor avoidance in visual search. Cognition 2024; 254:106007. [PMID: 39541895 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/19/2024] [Revised: 09/07/2024] [Accepted: 11/05/2024] [Indexed: 11/17/2024]
Abstract
Recent findings suggest that it is possible for people to proactively avoid attentional capture by salient distractors during visual search. The results have important implications for understanding the competing influences of top-down and bottom-up factors in visual attention. Nevertheless, questions remain regarding the extent to which apparently ignored distractors are processed. To assess distractor processing, previous experiments have used a probe method in which stimuli are occasionally superimposed on the search display-requiring participants to abort the search and identify the probe stimuli. It has been recently shown that such probe tasks may be vulnerable to decision-level biases, such as a participant's willingness to report stimuli on to-be-ignored items. We report here results from a new method that is not subject to this limitation. In the new method, the non-target search elements, including the salient distractors, contained features that were either congruent or incongruent with the target. Processing of the non-target elements is inferred from the effects of the compatibility of the shared features on judgments about the target. In four experiments using the technique we show that ignored salient distractors are indeed processed less fully than non-target elements that are not salient, replicating the results of earlier studies using the probe methods. Additionally, the processing of the distractors was found to be reduced at least in part at early perceptual or attentional stages, as assumed by models of attentional suppression. The study confirms the proactive avoidance of capture by salient distractors measured without decision-level biases and provides a new technique for assessing the magnitude of distractor processing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Xiaojin Ma
- Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, Washington University in St. Louis, United States of America; Department of Psychological Science, University of Missouri, Columbia, United States of America.
| | - Richard A Abrams
- Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, Washington University in St. Louis, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Oxner M, Mazza V, Müller MM. Repetition of critical search features modulates EEG lateralized potentials in visual search. Cereb Cortex 2024; 34:bhae450. [PMID: 39568401 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhae450] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/24/2024] [Revised: 10/29/2024] [Accepted: 10/31/2024] [Indexed: 11/22/2024] Open
Abstract
In visual search, the repetition of target and distractor colors enables both successful search and effective distractor handling. Nevertheless, the specific consequences of trial-to-trial feature repetition in different search contexts are poorly understood. Here, we investigated how feature repetition shapes the electrophysiological and behavioral correlates of target processing and distractor handling, testing theoretically informed predictions with single-trial mixed-effects modeling. In two experiments, the colors of a fixed-shape target and singleton distractor changed unpredictably across trials. Targets were color singletons in Experiment 1, allowing efficient search among pop-out items, but were not uniquely colored in Experiment 2, encouraging slower shape-feature search. Interference by the distractor occurred only in pop-out search but was reduced by repetition. This was paralleled by the contralateral electroencephalography (EEG) response: Following a search color change, the target-related N2pc was greatly reduced, and salient distractors elicited an N2pc followed by an enhanced PD. This biphasic response was absent in Experiment 2, where color was less useful to search. Overall, distractor positivities were not sensitive to feature repetition, suggesting that they are unrelated to preparatory suppression. Attention-related lateralization components are not universally elicited by target or distractor feature values but are driven specifically by expected features important to the search task.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Matt Oxner
- Wilhelm-Wundt-Institut für Psychologie, Universität Leipzig, Neumarkt 9-19, 04109 Leipzig, Germany
| | - Veronica Mazza
- Center for Mind/Brain Sciences (CIMeC), University of Trento, C.so Bettini 31, 38068 Rovereto (Tn), Italy
| | - Matthias M Müller
- Wilhelm-Wundt-Institut für Psychologie, Universität Leipzig, Neumarkt 9-19, 04109 Leipzig, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Zhong C, Qu Z, Yang N, Sun M, Wang Y, Ding Y. Susceptibility to Attentional Capture by Target-Matching Distractors Predicts High Visual Working Memory Capacity. Psychol Sci 2024; 35:1203-1216. [PMID: 39418213 DOI: 10.1177/09567976241279520] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/19/2024] Open
Abstract
Recent event-related potential (ERP) studies showed that individuals with low visual working memory (VWM) capacity are more susceptible to salience-driven attentional capture than high-capacity individuals are, with the latter being able to proactively suppress salient but irrelevant distractors. However, it remains unclear whether and how contingent attentional capture by distractors that possess a task-relevant (target) feature is related to VWM capacity. Here, we adopted a central focused-attention task that contained peripheral target-matching distractors to investigate this issue (N = 51 adults). Surprisingly, we revealed that target-matching distractors elicited both a larger N2-posterior-contralateral (N2pc) and a larger post-N2pc distractor positivity (PD) component in high-capacity individuals than in low-capacity ones, meaning that high-capacity individuals are less able to ignore such distractors initially, though they could call on a stronger reactive suppression mechanism afterward. These findings illustrate that high-capacity individuals are more (rather than less or equally) susceptible to contingent attention capture compared with low-capacity ones.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chupeng Zhong
- School of Psychology; Center for Studies of Psychological Application; Guangdong Key Laboratory of Mental Health and Cognitive Science; Ministry of Education Key Laboratory of Brain, Cognition, and Education Sciences; South China Normal University
| | - Zhe Qu
- Department of Psychology, Sun Yat-Sen University
| | - Nan Yang
- School of Psychology; Center for Studies of Psychological Application; Guangdong Key Laboratory of Mental Health and Cognitive Science; Ministry of Education Key Laboratory of Brain, Cognition, and Education Sciences; South China Normal University
| | - Mingze Sun
- School of Psychology; Center for Studies of Psychological Application; Guangdong Key Laboratory of Mental Health and Cognitive Science; Ministry of Education Key Laboratory of Brain, Cognition, and Education Sciences; South China Normal University
| | - Yajie Wang
- School of Psychology; Center for Studies of Psychological Application; Guangdong Key Laboratory of Mental Health and Cognitive Science; Ministry of Education Key Laboratory of Brain, Cognition, and Education Sciences; South China Normal University
| | - Yulong Ding
- School of Psychology; Center for Studies of Psychological Application; Guangdong Key Laboratory of Mental Health and Cognitive Science; Ministry of Education Key Laboratory of Brain, Cognition, and Education Sciences; South China Normal University
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Chiossi F, Trautmannsheimer I, Ou C, Gruenefeld U, Mayer S. Searching Across Realities: Investigating ERPs and Eye-Tracking Correlates of Visual Search in Mixed Reality. IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON VISUALIZATION AND COMPUTER GRAPHICS 2024; 30:6997-7007. [PMID: 39264778 DOI: 10.1109/tvcg.2024.3456172] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 09/14/2024]
Abstract
Mixed Reality allows us to integrate virtual and physical content into users' environments seamlessly. Yet, how this fusion affects perceptual and cognitive resources and our ability to find virtual or physical objects remains uncertain. Displaying virtual and physical information simultaneously might lead to divided attention and increased visual complexity, impacting users' visual processing, performance, and workload. In a visual search task, we asked participants to locate virtual and physical objects in Augmented Reality and Augmented Virtuality to understand the effects on performance. We evaluated search efficiency and attention allocation for virtual and physical objects using event-related potentials, fixation and saccade metrics, and behavioral measures. We found that users were more efficient in identifying objects in Augmented Virtuality, while virtual objects gained saliency in Augmented Virtuality. This suggests that visual fidelity might increase the perceptual load of the scene. Reduced amplitude in distractor positivity ERP, and fixation patterns supported improved distractor suppression and search efficiency in Augmented Virtuality. We discuss design implications for mixed reality adaptive systems based on physiological inputs for interaction.
Collapse
|
12
|
Trentin C, Olivers C, Slagter HA. Action Planning Renders Objects in Working Memory More Attentionally Salient. J Cogn Neurosci 2024; 36:2166-2183. [PMID: 39136556 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_02235] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 09/10/2024]
Abstract
A rapidly growing body of work suggests that visual working memory (VWM) is fundamentally action oriented. Consistent with this, we recently showed that attention is more strongly biased by VWM representations of objects when we plan to act on those objects in the future. Using EEG and eye tracking, here, we investigated neurophysiological correlates of the interactions between VWM and action. Participants (n = 36) memorized a shape for a subsequent VWM test. At test, a probe was presented along with a secondary object. In the action condition, participants gripped the actual probe if it matched the memorized shape, whereas in the control condition, they gripped the secondary object. Crucially, during the VWM delay, participants engaged in a visual selection task, in which they located a target as fast as possible. The memorized shape could either encircle the target (congruent trials) or a distractor (incongruent trials). Replicating previous findings, we found that eye gaze was biased toward the VWM-matching shape and, importantly, more so when the shape was directly associated with an action plan. Moreover, the ERP results revealed that during the selection task, future action-relevant VWM-matching shapes elicited (1) a stronger Ppc (posterior positivity contralateral), signaling greater attentional saliency; (2) an earlier PD (distractor positivity) component, suggesting faster suppression; (3) a larger inverse (i.e., positive) sustained posterior contralateral negativity in incongruent trials, consistent with stronger suppression of action-associated distractors; and (4) an enhanced response-locked positivity over left motor regions, possibly indicating enhanced inhibition of the response associated with the memorized item during the interim task. Overall, these results suggest that action planning renders objects in VWM more attentionally salient, supporting the notion of selection-for-action in working memory.
Collapse
|
13
|
Taylor ED, Feldmann-Wüstefeld T. Reward-modulated attention deployment is driven by suppression, not attentional capture. Neuroimage 2024; 299:120831. [PMID: 39233126 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2024.120831] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/12/2024] [Revised: 08/30/2024] [Accepted: 09/01/2024] [Indexed: 09/06/2024] Open
Abstract
One driving factor for attention deployment towards a stimulus is its associated value due to previous experience and learning history. Previous visual search studies found that when looking for a target, distractors associated with higher reward produce more interference (e.g., longer response times). The present study investigated the neural mechanism of such value-driven attention deployment. Specifically, we were interested in which of the three attention sub-processes are responsible for the interference that was repeatedly observed behaviorally: enhancement of relevant information, attentional capture by irrelevant information, or suppression of irrelevant information. We replicated earlier findings showing longer response times and lower accuracy when a target competed with a high-reward compared to a low-reward distractor. We also found a spatial gradient of interference: behavioral performance dropped with increasing proximity to the target. This gradient was steeper for high- than low-reward distractors. Event-related potentials of the EEG signal showed the reason for the reward-induced attentional bias: High-reward distractors required more suppression than low-reward distractors as evident in larger Pd components. This effect was only found for distractors near targets, showing the additional filtering needs required for competing stimuli in close proximity. As a result, fewer attentional resources can be distributed to the target when it competes with a high-reward distractor, as evident in a smaller target-N2pc amplitude. The distractor-N2pc, indicative of attentional capture, was neither affected by distance nor reward, showing that attentional capture alone cannot explain interference by stimuli of high value. In sum our results show that the higher need for suppression of high-value stimuli contributes to reward-modulated attention deployment and increased suppression can prevent attentional capture of high-value stimuli.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Emily D Taylor
- School of Psychology, University of Southampton, Southampton, UK
| | - Tobias Feldmann-Wüstefeld
- School of Psychology, University of Southampton, Southampton, UK; Institute of Psychology and Ergonomics, Technische Universität Berlin, Berlin, Germany.
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Chen X, Cao L, Haendel BF. Right visual field advantage in orientation discrimination is influenced by biased suppression. Sci Rep 2024; 14:22687. [PMID: 39349588 PMCID: PMC11442441 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-024-73967-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/20/2024] [Accepted: 09/23/2024] [Indexed: 10/04/2024] Open
Abstract
Visual input is not equally processed over space. In recent years, a right visual field advantage during free walking and standing in orientation discrimination and contrast detection task was reported. The current study investigated the underlying mechanism of the previously reported right visual field advantage. It particularly tested if the advantage is driven by a stronger suppression of distracting input from the left visual field or improved processing of targets from the right visual field. Combing behavioural and electrophysiological measurements in a mobile EEG and augmented reality setup, human participants (n = 30) in a standing and a walking condition performed a line orientation discrimination task with stimulus eccentricity and distractor status being manipulated. The right visual field advantage, as demonstrated in accuracy and reaction time, was influenced by the distractor status. Specifically, the right visual field advantage was only observed when the target had an incongruent line orientation with the distractor. Neural data further showed that the right visual field advantage was paralleled by a strong modulation of neural activity in the right hemisphere (i.e. contralateral to the distractor). A significant positive correlation between this right hemispheric event related potential (ERP) and behavioural measures (accuracy and reaction time) was found exclusively for trials in which a target was presented on the right and an incongruent distractor was presented on the left. The right hemispheric ERP component further predicted the strength of the right visual field advantage. Notably, the lateralised brain activity and the right visual field advantage were both independent of stimulus eccentricity and the movement state of participants. Overall, our findings suggest an important role of spatially biased suppression of left distracting input in the right visual field advantage as found in orientation discrimination.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Xinyu Chen
- Department of Psychology and Behavioural Sciences, Zhejiang University, Hangzhou, 310058, China.
- Department of Psychology (III), Julius-Maximilians-Universität Würzburg, 97070, Würzburg, Germany.
| | - Liyu Cao
- Department of Psychology and Behavioural Sciences, Zhejiang University, Hangzhou, 310058, China
- State Key Laboratory of Brain-Machine Intelligence, Zhejiang University, Hangzhou, 311121, China
| | - Barbara F Haendel
- Department of Psychology and Behavioural Sciences, Zhejiang University, Hangzhou, 310058, China
- Department of Psychology (III), Julius-Maximilians-Universität Würzburg, 97070, Würzburg, Germany
- Neurology department, University Hospital Würzburg, 97080, Würzburg, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Kerzel D, Constant M. Effects of spatial location on distractor interference. J Vis 2024; 24:4. [PMID: 39240585 PMCID: PMC11382967 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.9.4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 09/07/2024] Open
Abstract
When target and distractor stimuli are close together, they activate the same neurons and there is ambiguity as to what the neural activity represents. It has been suggested that the ambiguity is resolved by spatial competition between target and nontarget stimuli. A competitive advantage is conveyed by bottom-up biases (e.g., stimulus saliency) and top-down biases (e.g., the match to a stored representation of the target stimulus). Here, we tested the hypothesis that regions with high perceptual performance may provide a bottom-up bias, resulting in increased distractor interference. Initially, we focused on two known anisotropies. At equal distance from central fixation, perceptual performance is better along the horizontal than the vertical meridian, and in the lower than in the upper visual hemifield. Consistently, interference from distractors on the horizontal meridian was greater than interference from distractors on the vertical meridian. However, distractors in the lower hemifield interfered less than distractors in the upper visual hemifield, which is contrary to the known anisotropy. These results were obtained with targets and distractors on opposite meridians. Further, we observed greater interference from distractors on the meridians compared with distractors on the diagonals, possibly reflecting anisotropies in attentional scanning. Overall, the results are only partially consistent with the hypothesis that distractor interference is larger for distractors on regions with high perceptual performance.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dirk Kerzel
- Faculté de Psychologie et des Sciences de l'Education, Université de Genève, Genève, Switzerland
- https://orcid.org/0000-0002-2466-5221
| | - Martin Constant
- Faculté de Psychologie et des Sciences de l'Education, Université de Genève, Genève, Switzerland
- https://orcid.org/0000-0001-9574-0674
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Jungerius C, Perizonius S, Slagter HA. Reduced contextual uncertainty facilitates learning what to attend to and what to ignore. Atten Percept Psychophys 2024; 86:1861-1871. [PMID: 39254916 PMCID: PMC11410890 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-024-02945-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 06/18/2024] [Indexed: 09/11/2024]
Abstract
Variability in the search environment has been shown to affect the capture of attention by salient distractors, as attentional capture is reduced when context variability is low. However, it remains unclear whether this reduction in capture is caused by contextual learning or other mechanisms, grounded in generic context-structure learning. We set out to test this by training participants (n = 200) over two sessions in a visual search task, conducted online, where they gained experience with a small subset of search displays, which significantly reduced capture of attention by colour singletons. In a third session, we then tested participants on a mix of familiar and novel search displays and examined whether this reduction in capture was specific to familiar displays, indicative of contextual cueing effects, or would generalise to novel displays. We found no capture by the singleton in either the familiar or novel condition. Instead, our findings suggested that reduced statistical volatility reduced capture by allowing the development of generic predictions about task-relevant locations and features of the display. These findings add to the current debate about the determinants of capture by salient distractors by showing that capture is also affected by generic task regularities and by the volatility of the learning environment.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chris Jungerius
- Swammerdam Institute for Life Sciences, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
- Department of Experimental and Applied Psychology, Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
- Institute for Brain and Behavior, Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
| | - Sophie Perizonius
- Department of Experimental and Applied Psychology, Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
- Institute for Brain and Behavior, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| | - Heleen A Slagter
- Department of Experimental and Applied Psychology, Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
- Institute for Brain and Behavior, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Zhao G, Chen J, Duan Y, Li S, Wang Q, Li D. The proactive and reactive mechanisms of learned spatial suppression. Cereb Cortex 2024; 34:bhae333. [PMID: 39110410 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhae333] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/27/2024] [Revised: 07/08/2024] [Accepted: 07/27/2024] [Indexed: 01/29/2025] Open
Abstract
Selection history refers to the notion that previous allocations of attention or suppression have the potential to elicit lingering and enduring selection biases that are isolated from goal-driven or stimulus-driven attention. However, in the singleton detection mode task, manipulating the selection history of distractors cannot give rise to pure proactive inhibition. Therefore, we employed a combination of a working memory task and a feature search mode task, simultaneously recording cortical activity using EEG, to investigate the mechanisms of suppression guided by selection history. The results from event-related potential and reaction times showed an enhanced inhibitory performance when the distractor was presented at the high-probability location, along with instances where the target appeared at the high-probability location of distractors. These findings demonstrate that a generalized proactive inhibition bias is learned and processed independent of cognitive resources, which is supported by selection history. In contrast, reactive rejection toward the low-probability location was evident through the Pd component under varying cognitive resource conditions. Taken together, our findings indicated that participants learned proactive inhibition when the distractor was at the high-probability location, whereas reactive rejection was involved at low-probability location.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Guang Zhao
- Faculty of Psychology, Tianjin Normal University, 393 Binshuixi Road, Tianjin 300387, China
- Key Research Base of Humanities and Social Sciences of the Ministry of Education, Academy of Psychology and Behavior, Tianjin Normal University, 393 Binshuixi Road, Tianjin 300387, China
- Tianjin Social Science Laboratory of Students' Mental Development and Learning, 393 Binshuixi Road, Tianjin 300387, China
| | - Jiahuan Chen
- Faculty of Psychology, Tianjin Normal University, 393 Binshuixi Road, Tianjin 300387, China
| | - Yuhao Duan
- Faculty of Psychology, Tianjin Normal University, 393 Binshuixi Road, Tianjin 300387, China
| | - Shiyi Li
- Faculty of Psychology, Tianjin Normal University, 393 Binshuixi Road, Tianjin 300387, China
- Key Research Base of Humanities and Social Sciences of the Ministry of Education, Academy of Psychology and Behavior, Tianjin Normal University, 393 Binshuixi Road, Tianjin 300387, China
- Tianjin Social Science Laboratory of Students' Mental Development and Learning, 393 Binshuixi Road, Tianjin 300387, China
| | - Qiang Wang
- Faculty of Psychology, Tianjin Normal University, 393 Binshuixi Road, Tianjin 300387, China
- Key Research Base of Humanities and Social Sciences of the Ministry of Education, Academy of Psychology and Behavior, Tianjin Normal University, 393 Binshuixi Road, Tianjin 300387, China
- Tianjin Social Science Laboratory of Students' Mental Development and Learning, 393 Binshuixi Road, Tianjin 300387, China
| | - Dongwei Li
- Department of Psychology, Faculty of Arts and Sciences, Beijing Normal University at Zhuhai, Zhuhai 519085, China
- Beijing Key Laboratory of Applied Experimental Psychology, National Demonstration Center for Experimental Psychology Education (Beijing Normal University), Faculty of Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing 100875, China
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Mandal A, Liesefeld AM, Liesefeld HR. Tracking the Misallocation and Reallocation of Spatial Attention toward Auditory Stimuli. J Neurosci 2024; 44:e2196232024. [PMID: 38886058 PMCID: PMC11270513 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.2196-23.2024] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/27/2023] [Revised: 05/04/2024] [Accepted: 05/06/2024] [Indexed: 06/20/2024] Open
Abstract
Completely ignoring a salient distractor presented concurrently with a target is difficult, and sometimes attention is involuntarily attracted to the distractor's location (attentional capture). Employing the N2ac component as a marker of attention allocation toward sounds, in this study we investigate the spatiotemporal dynamics of auditory attention across two experiments. Human participants (male and female) performed an auditory search task, where the target was accompanied by a distractor in two-third of the trials. For a distractor more salient than the target (Experiment 1), we observe not only a distractor N2ac (indicating attentional capture) but the full chain of attentional dynamics implied by the notion of attentional capture, namely, (1) the distractor captures attention before the target is attended, (2) allocation of attention to the target is delayed by distractor presence, and (3) the target is attended after the distractor. Conversely, for a distractor less salient than the target (Experiment 2), although responses were delayed, no attentional capture was observed. Together, these findings reveal two types of spatial attentional dynamics in the auditory modality (distraction with and without attentional capture).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ananya Mandal
- General and Experimental Psychology, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Munich, Munich 80802, Germany
- Graduate School for Systemic Neurosciences, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Munich, Planegg 82152, Germany
| | - Anna M Liesefeld
- General and Experimental Psychology, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Munich, Munich 80802, Germany
| | - Heinrich R Liesefeld
- Graduate School for Systemic Neurosciences, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Munich, Planegg 82152, Germany
- Department of Psychology, Universität Bremen, Bremen 28359, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Adams OJ, Gaspelin N. Attentional suppression of dynamic versus static salient distractors. Atten Percept Psychophys 2024; 86:1-14. [PMID: 38839713 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-024-02903-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 04/28/2024] [Indexed: 06/07/2024]
Abstract
Attention must be carefully controlled to avoid distraction by salient stimuli. The signal suppression hypothesis proposes that salient stimuli can be proactively suppressed to prevent distraction. Although this hypothesis has garnered much support, most previous studies have used one class of salient distractors: color singletons. It therefore remains unclear whether other kinds of salient distractors can also be suppressed. The current study directly compared suppression of a variety of salient stimuli using an attentional capture task that was adapted for eye tracking. The working hypothesis was that static salient stimuli (e.g., color singletons) would be easier to suppress than dynamic salient stimuli (e.g., motion singletons). The results showed that participants could ignore a wide variety of salient distractors. Importantly, suppression was weaker and slower to develop for dynamic salient stimuli than static salient stimuli. A final experiment revealed that adding a static salient feature to a dynamic motion distractor greatly improved suppression. Altogether, the results suggest that an underlying inhibitory process is applied to all kinds of salient distractors, but that suppression is more readily applied to static features than dynamic features.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Owen J Adams
- State University of New York at Binghamton, Binghamton, NY, 13902, USA
| | - Nicholas Gaspelin
- Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri, McAlester Hall, 320 S. 6th Street, Columbia, MO, 65211, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Carrasco CD, Bahle B, Simmons AM, Luck SJ. Using multivariate pattern analysis to increase effect sizes for event-related potential analyses. Psychophysiology 2024; 61:e14570. [PMID: 38516957 DOI: 10.1111/psyp.14570] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/08/2023] [Revised: 02/21/2024] [Accepted: 03/09/2024] [Indexed: 03/23/2024]
Abstract
Multivariate pattern analysis (MVPA) approaches can be applied to the topographic distribution of event-related potential (ERP) signals to "decode" subtly different stimulus classes, such as different faces or different orientations. These approaches are extremely sensitive, and it seems possible that they could also be used to increase effect sizes and statistical power in traditional paradigms that ask whether an ERP component differs in amplitude across conditions. To assess this possibility, we leveraged the open-source ERP CORE data set and compared the effect sizes resulting from conventional univariate analyses of mean amplitude with two MVPA approaches (support vector machine decoding and the cross-validated Mahalanobis distance, both of which are easy to compute using open-source software). We assessed these approaches across seven widely studied ERP components (N170, N400, N2pc, P3b, lateral readiness potential, error related negativity, and mismatch negativity). Across all components, we found that multivariate approaches yielded effect sizes that were as large or larger than the effect sizes produced by univariate approaches. These results indicate that researchers could obtain larger effect sizes, and therefore greater statistical power, by using multivariate analysis of topographic voltage patterns instead of traditional univariate analyses in many ERP studies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Brett Bahle
- Center for Mind and Brain, University of California, Davis, California, USA
| | | | - Steven J Luck
- Center for Mind and Brain, University of California, Davis, California, USA
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Lien MC, Ruthruff E, Tolomeo D. Evidence that proactive distractor suppression does not require attentional resources. Psychon Bull Rev 2024; 31:1376-1386. [PMID: 38049572 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-023-02422-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 11/11/2023] [Indexed: 12/06/2023]
Abstract
Does the suppression of irrelevant visual features require attentional resources? McDonald et al. (2023, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 30, 224-234) proposed that suppression processes are unavailable while a person is busy performing another task. They reported the absence of the PD (believed to index suppression) when two tasks were presented close together in time. We looked for converging evidence using established behavior measures of suppression. Following McDonald et al., our participants performed a rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) task followed by a search task. For the RSVP task, participants determined whether the target digit 4 or 6 appeared within a string of other digits. The search display appeared at a lag of 2 or 8 digits after the RSVP target. Participants searched for a yellow target circle amongst nine background circles, which included a uniquely colored distractor for some trials. The main question was whether distractor suppression would occur at Lag 2, while attentional resources were still processing the RSVP target. Suppression was assessed using the capture-probe paradigm. On 30% of trials, probe letters appeared inside the colored circles and participants reported those letters. Probe recall accuracy was lower at locations with distractor colors than those with neutral colors (the baseline), suggesting proactive suppression. Critically, this difference in probe recall accuracy was similar at Lag 2 and Lag 8, suggesting that the ability to proactively suppress distractors remains intact while dual-tasking. We argue that although reactive suppression likely requires attentional resources, proactive suppression-an implicit process-does not.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mei-Ching Lien
- School of Psychological Science, Oregon State University, Corvallis, OR, 97331-5303, USA.
| | - Eric Ruthruff
- Department of Psychology, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM, 87131-1161, USA
| | - Dominick Tolomeo
- Computer Science, Oregon State University, Corvallis, OR, 97331-5303, USA
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Szaszkó B, Habeler M, Forstinger M, Pomper U, Scheftner M, Stolte M, Grüner M, Ansorge U. 10 Hz rhythmic stimulation modulates electrophysiological, but not behavioral markers of suppression. Front Psychol 2024; 15:1376664. [PMID: 38831943 PMCID: PMC11144928 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1376664] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/25/2024] [Accepted: 04/08/2024] [Indexed: 06/05/2024] Open
Abstract
We investigated the role of alpha in the suppression of attention capture by salient but to-be-suppressed (negative and nonpredictive) color cues, expecting a potential boosting effect of alpha-rhythmic entrainment on feature-specific cue suppression. We did so by presenting a rhythmically flickering visual bar of 10 Hz before the cue - either on the cue's side or opposite the cue -while an arrhythmically flickering visual bar was presented on the respective other side. We hypothesized that rhythmic entrainment at cue location could enhance the suppression of the cue. Testing 27 participants ranging from 18 to 39 years of age, we found both behavioral and electrophysiological evidence of suppression: Search times for a target at a negatively cued location were delayed relative to a target away from the cued location (inverse validity effects). In addition, an event-related potential indicative for suppression (the Distractor Positivity, Pd) was observed following rhythmic but not arrhythmic stimulation, indicating that suppression was boosted by the stimulation. This was also echoed in higher spectral power and intertrial phase coherence of EEG at rhythmically versus arrhythmically stimulated electrode sites, albeit only at the second harmonic (20 Hz), but not at the stimulation frequency. In addition, inverse validity effects were not modulated by rhythmic entrainment congruent with the cue side. Hence, we propose that rhythmic visual stimulation in the alpha range could support suppression, though behavioral evidence remains elusive, in contrast to electrophysiological findings.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Bence Szaszkó
- Department of Cognition, Emotion, and Methods in Psychology, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
| | - Martin Habeler
- Department of Cognition, Emotion, and Methods in Psychology, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
| | - Marlene Forstinger
- Department of Cognition, Emotion, and Methods in Psychology, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
| | - Ulrich Pomper
- Department of Cognition, Emotion, and Methods in Psychology, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
| | - Manuel Scheftner
- Department of Cognition, Emotion, and Methods in Psychology, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
| | - Moritz Stolte
- Department of Cognition, Emotion, and Methods in Psychology, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
| | - Markus Grüner
- Department of Cognition, Emotion, and Methods in Psychology, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
| | - Ulrich Ansorge
- Department of Cognition, Emotion, and Methods in Psychology, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
- Vienna Cognitive Science Hub, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
- Research Platform Mediatised Lifeworlds, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Pang C, Chen Y, Zhang Y, Nan W, Fu S. Suppression on the basis of template for rejection is reactive: Evidence from human electrophysiology. Atten Percept Psychophys 2024; 86:1148-1162. [PMID: 38491317 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-024-02873-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 02/21/2024] [Indexed: 03/18/2024]
Abstract
According to most theories of attention, the selection of task-relevant visual information can be enhanced by holding them in visual working memory (VWM). However, there has been a long-standing debate concerning whether similar optimization can also be achieved for task-irrelevant information, known as a "template for rejection". The present study aimed to explore this issue by examining the consequence of cue distractors before visual search tasks. For this endeavor, we manipulated the display heterogeneity by using two distractor conditions, salient and non-salient, to explore the extent to which holding the distractor color in VWM might affect attentional selection. We measured the reaction times of participants while their EEG activity was recorded. The results showed that WM-matched distractors did not improve reaction times but rather slowed them down in both tasks. Event-related potential (ERP) results showed that the display heterogeneity had no modulatory effect on the degree of distractor suppression. Even in the salient distractor condition, the WM-matched distractor received no greater suppression. Furthermore, the WM-matched distractor but not the neutral distractor elicited an N2pc before the PD in salient distractor conditions. This suggests that the template for rejection operates reactively since suppression occurs after extra attentional processes to the distractor. Moreover, the presence of WM-matched distractors led to a reduction of P3b, indicating a competition between target processing and WM-matched distractor rejection. Our findings provide insights into the mechanisms underlying the optimization of attentional selection, and have implications for future studies aimed at understanding the role of VWM in cognition.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chao Pang
- Department of Psychology and Center for Brain and Cognitive Sciences, School of Education, Guangzhou University, Guangzhou, 510006, China
| | - Yanzhang Chen
- Philosophy and Social Science Laboratory of Reading and Development in Children and Adolescents (South China Normal University), Ministry of Education, Guangzhou, China
| | - Yue Zhang
- Department of Psychology and Center for Brain and Cognitive Sciences, School of Education, Guangzhou University, Guangzhou, 510006, China
| | - Weizhi Nan
- Department of Psychology and Center for Brain and Cognitive Sciences, School of Education, Guangzhou University, Guangzhou, 510006, China
| | - Shimin Fu
- Department of Psychology and Center for Brain and Cognitive Sciences, School of Education, Guangzhou University, Guangzhou, 510006, China.
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Zhang Y, Zhang H, Fu S. Relative saliency affects attentional capture and suppression of color and face singleton distractors: evidence from event-related potential studies. Cereb Cortex 2024; 34:bhae176. [PMID: 38679483 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhae176] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/18/2023] [Revised: 03/30/2024] [Accepted: 04/09/2024] [Indexed: 05/01/2024] Open
Abstract
Prior research has yet to fully elucidate the impact of varying relative saliency between target and distractor on attentional capture and suppression, along with their underlying neural mechanisms, especially when social (e.g. face) and perceptual (e.g. color) information interchangeably serve as singleton targets or distractors, competing for attention in a search array. Here, we employed an additional singleton paradigm to investigate the effects of relative saliency on attentional capture (as assessed by N2pc) and suppression (as assessed by PD) of color or face singleton distractors in a visual search task by recording event-related potentials. We found that face singleton distractors with higher relative saliency induced stronger attentional processing. Furthermore, enhancing the physical salience of colors using a bold color ring could enhance attentional processing toward color singleton distractors. Reducing the physical salience of facial stimuli by blurring weakened attentional processing toward face singleton distractors; however, blurring enhanced attentional processing toward color singleton distractors because of the change in relative saliency. In conclusion, the attentional processes of singleton distractors are affected by their relative saliency to singleton targets, with higher relative saliency of singleton distractors resulting in stronger attentional capture and suppression; faces, however, exhibit some specificity in attentional capture and suppression due to high social saliency.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yue Zhang
- Department of Psychology and Center for Brain and Cognitive Sciences, School of Education, Guangzhou University, 230 Wai Huan Xi Road, Guangzhou Higher Education Mega Center, Guangzhou 510006, China
| | - Hai Zhang
- Department of Psychology and Center for Brain and Cognitive Sciences, School of Education, Guangzhou University, 230 Wai Huan Xi Road, Guangzhou Higher Education Mega Center, Guangzhou 510006, China
| | - Shimin Fu
- Department of Psychology and Center for Brain and Cognitive Sciences, School of Education, Guangzhou University, 230 Wai Huan Xi Road, Guangzhou Higher Education Mega Center, Guangzhou 510006, China
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Carrasco CD, Bahle B, Simmons AM, Luck SJ. Using Multivariate Pattern Analysis to Increase Effect Sizes for Event-Related Potential Analyses. BIORXIV : THE PREPRINT SERVER FOR BIOLOGY 2024:2023.11.07.566051. [PMID: 37986854 PMCID: PMC10659264 DOI: 10.1101/2023.11.07.566051] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2023]
Abstract
Multivariate pattern analysis approaches can be applied to the topographic distribution of event-related potential (ERP) signals to 'decode' subtly different stimulus classes, such as different faces or different orientations. These approaches are extremely sensitive, and it seems possible that they could also be used to increase effect sizes and statistical power in traditional paradigms that ask whether an ERP component differs in amplitude across conditions. To assess this possibility, we leveraged the open-source ERP CORE dataset and compared the effect sizes resulting from conventional univariate analyses of mean amplitude with two multivariate pattern analysis approaches (support vector machine decoding and the cross-validated Mahalanobis distance, both of which are easy to compute using open-source software). We assessed these approaches across seven widely studied ERP components (N170, N400, N2pc, P3b, lateral readiness potential, error related negativity, and mismatch negativity). Across all components, we found that multivariate approaches yielded effect sizes that were as large or larger than the effect sizes produced by univariate approaches. These results indicate that researchers could obtain larger effect sizes, and therefore greater statistical power, by using multivariate analysis of topographic voltage patterns instead of traditional univariate analyses in many ERP studies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Brett Bahle
- Center for Mind & Brain, University of California, Davis
| | | | - Steven J Luck
- Center for Mind & Brain, University of California, Davis
| |
Collapse
|