1
|
John L, Santon M, Michiels NK. Scorpionfish adjust skin pattern contrast on different backgrounds. Ecol Evol 2024; 14:e11124. [PMID: 38476704 PMCID: PMC10928359 DOI: 10.1002/ece3.11124] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/15/2023] [Revised: 02/06/2024] [Accepted: 02/23/2024] [Indexed: 03/14/2024] Open
Abstract
The two scorpionfish species Scorpaena maderensis and S. porcus are well camouflaged ambush predators that rapidly change body colouration to adjust to background colour in less than 1 min. We tested whether individuals of both species also adjust body pattern to that of the background. We placed fish on backgrounds of different pattern granularity and quantified the change in fish body pattern over 1 min. We used calibrated image analysis to analyse the patterns from the visual perspective of a prey fish species using a granularity (pattern energy) analysis and an image clustering approach. In our experiment, fish did not change their most contrasting pattern components as defined by the dominant marking size, but changed their average marking size. Moreover, fish responded with a change in pattern in contrast to the different experimental backgrounds, especially when compared to the acclimation phase. These results indicate that scorpionfish have one main pattern that can be adjusted by modulating its internal contrast. A reduction in pattern contrast could thereby improve background matching, while an increase could promote camouflage via disruptive colouration.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Leonie John
- Animal Evolutionary EcologyInstitute of Evolution and Ecology, University of TübingenTübingenGermany
| | - Matteo Santon
- Ecology of Vision Group, School of Biological SciencesUniversity of BristolBristolUK
| | - Nico K. Michiels
- Animal Evolutionary EcologyInstitute of Evolution and Ecology, University of TübingenTübingenGermany
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Blary CLM, Duriez O, Bonadonna F, Mitkus M, Caro SP, Besnard A, Potier S. Low achromatic contrast sensitivity in birds: a common attribute shared by many phylogenetic orders. J Exp Biol 2024; 227:jeb246342. [PMID: 38099472 DOI: 10.1242/jeb.246342] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/21/2023] [Accepted: 12/06/2023] [Indexed: 02/10/2024]
Abstract
Vision is an important sensory modality in birds, which can outperform other vertebrates in some visual abilities. However, sensitivity to achromatic contrasts - the ability to discern luminance difference between two objects or an object and its background - has been shown to be lower in birds compared with other vertebrates. We conducted a comparative study to evaluate the achromatic contrast sensitivity of 32 bird species from 12 orders using the optocollic reflex technique. We then performed an analysis to test for potential variability in contrast sensitivity depending on the corneal diameter to the axial length ratio, a proxy of the retinal image brightness. To account for potential influences of evolutionary relatedness, we included phylogeny in our analyses. We found a low achromatic contrast sensitivity for all avian species studied compared with other vertebrates (except small mammals), with high variability between species. This variability is partly related to phylogeny but appears to be independent of image brightness.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Constance L M Blary
- CEFE, Univ Montpellier, CNRS, EPHE, IRD, 34293 Montpellier, France
- Agence de l'environnement et de la Maîtrise de l'Energie 20, 49004 Angers Cedex 01, France
| | - Olivier Duriez
- CEFE, Univ Montpellier, CNRS, EPHE, IRD, 34293 Montpellier, France
| | | | - Mindaugas Mitkus
- Institute of Biosciences, Life Sciences Center, Vilnius University, 10257 Vilnius, Lithuania
| | - Samuel P Caro
- CEFE, Univ Montpellier, CNRS, EPHE, IRD, 34293 Montpellier, France
| | - Aurélien Besnard
- CEFE, Univ Montpellier, CNRS, EPHE PSL University, IRD, 34293 Montpellier, France
| | - Simon Potier
- Lund Vision Group, Department of Biology, Lund University, Lund 22362, Sweden
- Les Ailes de l'Urga, 27320 Marcilly la Campagne, France
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Tosetto L, Hart NS, Williamson JE. A fish can change its stripes: investigating the role of body colour and pattern in the bluelined goatfish. PeerJ 2024; 12:e16645. [PMID: 38304190 PMCID: PMC10832622 DOI: 10.7717/peerj.16645] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/07/2023] [Accepted: 11/20/2023] [Indexed: 02/03/2024] Open
Abstract
Bluelined goatfish (Upeneichthys lineatus) rapidly change their body colour from a white horizontally banded pattern to a seemingly more conspicuous vertically banded red pattern, often when foraging. Given the apparent conspicuousness of the pattern to a range of observers, it seems unlikely that this colour change is used for camouflage and instead may be used for communication/signalling. Goatfish often drive multispecies associations, and it is possible that goatfish use this colour change as a foraging success signal to facilitate cooperation, increase food acquisition, and reduce predation risk through a 'safety in numbers' strategy. Using a novel approach, we deployed 3D model goatfish in different colour morphs-white without bands, white with black vertical bands, and white with red vertical bands-to determine whether the red colouration is an important component of the signal or if it is only the vertical banding pattern, regardless of colour, that fish respond to as an indicator of foraging success. Use of remote underwater video allowed us to obtain information without the influence of human observers on the communities and behaviours of other fish in response to these different colours exhibited by goatfish. We found that conspecifics were more abundant around the black- and red-banded model fish when compared with the white models. Conspecifics were also more likely to forage around the models than to pass or show attraction, but this was unaffected by model colour. No difference in the abundance and behaviour of associated heterospecifics around the different models was observed, perhaps due to the static nature of the models. Some species did, however, spend more time around the red- and black-banded fish, which suggests the change in colour may indicate benefits in addition to food resources. Overall, the results suggest that the body colour/pattern of U. lineatus is likely a signalling tool but further work is required to explore the benefits to both conspecifics and heterospecifics and to further determine the behavioural functions of rapid colour change in U. lineatus.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Louise Tosetto
- School of Natural Sciences, Macquarie University, Wallumattagul Campus, North Ryde, NSW, Australia
| | - Nathan S. Hart
- School of Natural Sciences, Macquarie University, Wallumattagul Campus, North Ryde, NSW, Australia
| | - Jane E. Williamson
- School of Natural Sciences, Macquarie University, Wallumattagul Campus, North Ryde, NSW, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
van den Berg CP, Endler JA, Papinczak DEJ, Cheney KL. Using colour pattern edge contrast statistics to predict detection speed and success in triggerfish (Rhinecanthus aculeatus). J Exp Biol 2022; 225:285905. [PMID: 36354306 PMCID: PMC9789405 DOI: 10.1242/jeb.244677] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/16/2022] [Accepted: 10/26/2022] [Indexed: 11/12/2022]
Abstract
Edge detection is important for object detection and recognition. However, we do not know whether edge statistics accurately predict the detection of prey by potential predators. This is crucial given the growing availability of image analysis software and their application across non-human visual systems. Here, we investigated whether Boundary Strength Analysis (BSA), Local Edge Intensity Analysis (LEIA) and the Gabor edge disruption ratio (GabRat) could predict the speed and success with which triggerfish (Rhinecanthus aculeatus) detected patterned circular stimuli against a noisy visual background, in both chromatic and achromatic presentations. We found various statistically significant correlations between edge statistics and detection speed depending on treatment and viewing distance; however, individual pattern statistics only explained up to 2% of the variation in detection time, and up to 6% when considering edge statistics simultaneously. We also found changes in fish response over time. While highlighting the importance of spatial acuity and relevant viewing distances in the study of visual signals, our results demonstrate the importance of considering explained variation when interpreting colour pattern statistics in behavioural experiments. We emphasize the need for statistical approaches suitable for investigating task-specific predictive relationships and ecological effects when considering animal behaviour. This is particularly important given the ever-increasing dimensionality and size of datasets in the field of visual ecology.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Cedric P. van den Berg
- Visual Ecology Lab, School of Biological Sciences, The University of Queensland, St Lucia, QLD 4072, Australia,Author for correspondence ()
| | - John A. Endler
- Centre for Integrative Ecology, School of Life and Environmental Sciences, Deakin University, Geelong, VIC 3216, Australia
| | - Daniel E. J. Papinczak
- Visual Ecology Lab, School of Biological Sciences, The University of Queensland, St Lucia, QLD 4072, Australia
| | - Karen L. Cheney
- Visual Ecology Lab, School of Biological Sciences, The University of Queensland, St Lucia, QLD 4072, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Rossi GS, Labbé D, Wright PA. Out of water in the dark: Plasticity in visual structures and function in an amphibious fish. JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL ZOOLOGY. PART A, ECOLOGICAL AND INTEGRATIVE PHYSIOLOGY 2022; 337:776-784. [PMID: 35727120 DOI: 10.1002/jez.2636] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/21/2022] [Revised: 05/20/2022] [Accepted: 06/08/2022] [Indexed: 06/15/2023]
Abstract
Many fishes encounter periods of prolonged darkness within their lifetime, yet the consequences for the visual system are poorly understood. We used an amphibious fish (Kryptolebias marmoratus) that occupies dark terrestrial environments during seasonal droughts to test whether exposure to prolonged darkness diminishes visual performance owing to reduced optic tectum (OT) size and/or neurogenesis. We performed a 3-week acclimation with a 2 ×$\times $ 2 factorial design, in which fish were either acclimated to a 12 h:12 h or 0 h:24 h light:dark photoperiod in water or in air. We found that water-exposed fish had poorer visual acuity when acclimated to the dark, while air-acclimated fish had poorer visual acuity regardless of photoperiod. The ability of K. marmoratus to capture aerial prey from water followed a similar trend, suggesting that good vision is important for hunting effectively. Changes in visual acuity did not result from changes in OT size, but air-acclimated fish had 37% fewer proliferating cells in the OT than water-acclimated fish. As K. marmoratus are unable to eat on land, reducing cell proliferation in the OT may serve as a mechanism to reduce maintenance costs associated with the visual system. Overall, we suggest that prolonged darkness and air exposure can impair vision in K. marmoratus, and that changes in visual performance may be mediated, in part, by OT neurogenesis. More broadly, we show that plastic changes to the visual system of fishes can have potential consequences for organismal performance and fitness.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Giulia S Rossi
- Department of Integrative Biology, University of Guelph, Guelph, Ontario, Canada
- Department of Biological Sciences, University of Toronto Scarborough, Scarborough, Ontario, Canada
| | - Daniel Labbé
- Department of Integrative Biology, University of Guelph, Guelph, Ontario, Canada
- School of Earth and Ocean Sciences, University of Victoria, Victoria, British Columbia, Canada
| | - Patricia A Wright
- Department of Integrative Biology, University of Guelph, Guelph, Ontario, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Santon M, Deiss F, Bitton P, Michiels NK. A context analysis of bobbing and fin-flicking in a small marine benthic fish. Ecol Evol 2021; 11:1254-1263. [PMID: 33598128 PMCID: PMC7863667 DOI: 10.1002/ece3.7116] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/24/2020] [Revised: 11/05/2020] [Accepted: 11/11/2020] [Indexed: 11/23/2022] Open
Abstract
Most antipredator strategies increase survival of individuals by signaling to predators, by reducing the chances of being recognized as prey, or by bewildering a predator's perception. In fish, bobbing and fin-flicking are commonly considered as pursuit-deterrent behaviors that signal a predator that it has been detected and thus lost its surprise-attack advantage. Yet, very few studies assessed whether such behavioral traits are restricted to the visual presence of a predator. In this study, we used the yellow black-headed triplefin Tripterygion delaisi to investigate the association between these behaviors and the visual exposure to (a) a black scorpionfish predator (Scorpaena porcus), (b) a stone of a size similar to that of S. porcus, (c) a conspecific, and (d) a harmless heterospecific combtooth blenny (Parablennius sanguinolentus). We used a laboratory-controlled experiment with freshly caught fish designed to test for differences in visual cues only. Distance kept by the focal fish to each stimulus and frequency of bobbing and fin-flicking were recorded. Triplefins kept greater distance from the stimulus compartment when a scorpionfish predator was visible. Bobbing was more frequent in the visual presence of a scorpionfish, but also shown toward the other stimuli. However, fin flicks were equally abundant across all stimuli. Both behaviors decreased in frequency over time suggesting that triplefin become gradually comfortable in a nonchanging new environment. We discuss why bobbing and fin-flicking are not exclusive pursuit-deterrent behaviors in this species, and propose additional nonexclusive functions such as enhancing depth perception by parallax motion (bobbing) or signaling vigilance (fin-flicking).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Matteo Santon
- Animal Evolutionary EcologyDepartment of BiologyFaculty of ScienceInstitute of Evolution and EcologyUniversity of TübingenTübingenGermany
| | - Felix Deiss
- Animal Evolutionary EcologyDepartment of BiologyFaculty of ScienceInstitute of Evolution and EcologyUniversity of TübingenTübingenGermany
| | - Pierre‐Paul Bitton
- Animal Evolutionary EcologyDepartment of BiologyFaculty of ScienceInstitute of Evolution and EcologyUniversity of TübingenTübingenGermany
- Department of PsychologyMemorial University of NewfoundlandSt. John'sNLCanada
| | - Nico K. Michiels
- Animal Evolutionary EcologyDepartment of BiologyFaculty of ScienceInstitute of Evolution and EcologyUniversity of TübingenTübingenGermany
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Neiße N, Santon M, Bitton PP, Michiels NK. Small benthic fish strike at prey over distances that fall within theoretical predictions for active sensing using light. JOURNAL OF FISH BIOLOGY 2020; 97:1201-1208. [PMID: 33448392 DOI: 10.1111/jfb.14502] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/13/2020] [Revised: 07/30/2020] [Accepted: 08/14/2020] [Indexed: 06/12/2023]
Abstract
Small, benthic, cryptic fishes represent a species-rich guild on marine substrates. Most of them are micropredators that feed on crustaceans that are often smaller than 1 mm. Typical examples are seahorses and pipefishes (Syngnathidae), most gobies (Gobiidae), dragonets (Callionymidae) and triplefins (Tripterygiidae). Previous work on the yellow black-headed triplefin Tripterygion delaisi demonstrated that it actively redirects downwelling sunlight sideways using its iris and can use this to locally illuminate objects of interest. We call this form of active sensing using light "diurnal active photolocation". Visual modelling predicted that light redirection can be sufficient to induce a perceivable change in luminance in the eyes of one of its prey species, a cryptic gammarid crustacean (Cheirocratus sp.), over distances of 1-2 cm. Empirical validation, however, was not possible because measurements of predation distances have not been quantified for free-ranging, small, benthic fishes before. Here, we present interaction distances measured from videos of T. delaisi approaching and striking at prey in the field. Out of 160 recordings, we were able to quantify 78 prey approaching distances and 100 striking distances. Approaching distances ranged from 2.1 to 4.1 cm (interquartile range, IQR) and involved one to five approaching steps before the actual strike occurred. The distance over which the final strike took place varied from 0.7 to 1.6 cm (IQR). Both approaching and striking distances increased with fish body size. We conclude that most approaching sequences started too far away to be explained by prey detection through light redirection. Striking distances, however, fell well with the distances predicted by the model. We conclude that if diurnal active photolocation plays a role in prey detection, it is during the final decision whether to strike or not.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Niklas Neiße
- Department of Biology, Faculty of Science, Animal Evolutionary Ecology, Institute of Evolution and Ecology, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
| | - Matteo Santon
- Department of Biology, Faculty of Science, Animal Evolutionary Ecology, Institute of Evolution and Ecology, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
| | - Pierre-Paul Bitton
- Department of Biology, Faculty of Science, Animal Evolutionary Ecology, Institute of Evolution and Ecology, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
- Department of Psychology, Memorial University of Newfoundland, St John's, Newfoundland and Labrador, Canada
| | - Nico K Michiels
- Department of Biology, Faculty of Science, Animal Evolutionary Ecology, Institute of Evolution and Ecology, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Santon M, Bitton PP, Dehm J, Fritsch R, Harant UK, Anthes N, Michiels NK. Redirection of ambient light improves predator detection in a diurnal fish. Proc Biol Sci 2020; 287:20192292. [PMID: 31964304 PMCID: PMC7015323 DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2019.2292] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Cases where animals use controlled illumination to improve vision are rare and thus far limited to chemiluminescence, which only functions in darkness. This constraint was recently relaxed by studies on Tripterygion delaisi, a small triplefin that redirects sunlight instead. By reflecting light sideways with its iris, it has been suggested to induce and detect eyeshine in nearby micro-prey. Here, we test whether 'diurnal active photolocation' also improves T. delaisi's ability to detect the cryptobenthic sit-and-wait predator Scorpaena porcus, a scorpionfish with strong daytime retroreflective eyeshine. Three independent experiments revealed that triplefins in which light redirection was artificially suppressed approached scorpionfish significantly closer than two control treatments before moving away to a safer distance. Visual modelling confirmed that ocular light redirection by a triplefin is sufficiently strong to generate a luminance increase in scorpionfish eyeshine that can be perceived by the triplefin over 6-8 cm under average conditions. These distances coincide well with the closest approaches observed. We conclude that light redirection by small, diurnal fish significantly contributes to their ability to visually detect cryptic predators, strongly widening the conditions under which active sensing with light is feasible. We discuss the consequences for fish eye evolution.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Matteo Santon
- Animal Evolutionary Ecology, Institute of Evolution and Ecology, Department of Biology, Faculty of Science, University of Tübingen, Auf der Morgenstelle 28, 72076 Tübingen, Germany
| | - Pierre-Paul Bitton
- Animal Evolutionary Ecology, Institute of Evolution and Ecology, Department of Biology, Faculty of Science, University of Tübingen, Auf der Morgenstelle 28, 72076 Tübingen, Germany.,Department of Psychology, Memorial University of Newfoundland, 232 Elizabeth Avenue, St John's, NL Canada, A1B 3X9
| | - Jasha Dehm
- Animal Evolutionary Ecology, Institute of Evolution and Ecology, Department of Biology, Faculty of Science, University of Tübingen, Auf der Morgenstelle 28, 72076 Tübingen, Germany.,School of Marine Studies, Faculty of Science, Technology and Environment, University of the South Pacific, Laucala Bay Rd, Suva, Fiji
| | - Roland Fritsch
- Animal Evolutionary Ecology, Institute of Evolution and Ecology, Department of Biology, Faculty of Science, University of Tübingen, Auf der Morgenstelle 28, 72076 Tübingen, Germany
| | - Ulrike K Harant
- Animal Evolutionary Ecology, Institute of Evolution and Ecology, Department of Biology, Faculty of Science, University of Tübingen, Auf der Morgenstelle 28, 72076 Tübingen, Germany
| | - Nils Anthes
- Animal Evolutionary Ecology, Institute of Evolution and Ecology, Department of Biology, Faculty of Science, University of Tübingen, Auf der Morgenstelle 28, 72076 Tübingen, Germany
| | - Nico K Michiels
- Animal Evolutionary Ecology, Institute of Evolution and Ecology, Department of Biology, Faculty of Science, University of Tübingen, Auf der Morgenstelle 28, 72076 Tübingen, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Visual modelling supports the potential for prey detection by means of diurnal active photolocation in a small cryptobenthic fish. Sci Rep 2019; 9:8089. [PMID: 31147614 PMCID: PMC6542814 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-019-44529-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/21/2018] [Accepted: 05/17/2019] [Indexed: 11/16/2022] Open
Abstract
Active sensing has been well documented in animals that use echolocation and electrolocation. Active photolocation, or active sensing using light, has received much less attention, and only in bioluminescent nocturnal species. However, evidence has suggested the diurnal triplefin Tripterygion delaisi uses controlled iris radiance, termed ocular sparks, for prey detection. While this form of diurnal active photolocation was behaviourally described, a study exploring the physical process would provide compelling support for this mechanism. In this paper, we investigate the conditions under which diurnal active photolocation could assist T. delaisi in detecting potential prey. In the field, we sampled gammarids (genus Cheirocratus) and characterized the spectral properties of their eyes, which possess strong directional reflectors. In the laboratory, we quantified ocular sparks size and their angle-dependent radiance. Combined with environmental light measurements and known properties of the visual system of T. delaisi, we modeled diurnal active photolocation under various scenarios. Our results corroborate that diurnal active photolocation should help T. delaisi detect gammarids at distances relevant to foraging, 4.5 cm under favourable conditions and up to 2.5 cm under average conditions. To determine the prevalence of diurnal active photolocation for micro-prey, we encourage further theoretical and empirical work.
Collapse
|