1
|
Romero RD, de Souza FSJ. Evolution of Pineal Nonvisual Opsins in Lizards and the Tuatara and Identification of Lepidopsin: A New Opsin Gene. Genome Biol Evol 2025; 17:evaf058. [PMID: 40312047 PMCID: PMC12043008 DOI: 10.1093/gbe/evaf058] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 03/21/2025] [Indexed: 05/03/2025] Open
Abstract
Many lizards (Squamata), as well as the tuatara (Rhynchocephalia), are distinguished among vertebrate groups for the presence of the parietal eye, or "third eye", a structure derived from the pineal complex containing a simplified retina with photoreceptor cells. The parietal eye expresses nonvisual opsins that differ from the visual opsin repertoire of the lateral eyes. These are pinopsin (OPNP), parapinopsin (OPNPP), and parietopsin (OPNPT), all being evolutionary close to visual opsins. Here, we searched over 60 lepidosaurian genomes for pineal nonvisual opsins to check for the evolutionary trajectory of these genes in reptiles. Unexpectedly, we identified a novel opsin gene, which we termed "lepidopsin" (OPNLEP), that is present solely in the genomes of the tuatara and most lizard groups but absent from other vertebrates. Remnants of the gene are found in the coelacanth and some ray-finned fishes, implying that OPNLEP is an ancient opsin that has been repeatedly lost during vertebrate evolution. We found that the tuatara and most lizards of the Iguania, Anguimorpha, Scincoidea, and Lacertidae clades, which possess a parietal eye, harbor all pineal opsin genes. Lizards missing the parietal eye, like geckos, teiids, and a fossorial amphisbaenian, lack most or all pineal nonvisual opsins. In summary, our survey of pineal nonvisual opsins reveals (i) the persistence of a previously unknown ancient opsin gene-OPNLEP-in lepidosaurians; (ii) losses of nonvisual opsins in specific lizard clades; and (iii) a correlation between the presence of a parietal eye and the genomic repertoire of pineal nonvisual opsins.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ricardo D Romero
- Instituto de Fisiología, Biología Molecular y Neurociencias (IFIBYNE-UBA-CONICET), Buenos Aires, Argentina
- Departamento de Fisiología, Biología Molecular y Celular, Facultad de Ciencias Exactas y Naturales, Universidad de Buenos Aires (FBMC-FCEyN-UBA), Buenos Aires, Argentina
| | - Flávio S J de Souza
- Instituto de Fisiología, Biología Molecular y Neurociencias (IFIBYNE-UBA-CONICET), Buenos Aires, Argentina
- Departamento de Fisiología, Biología Molecular y Celular, Facultad de Ciencias Exactas y Naturales, Universidad de Buenos Aires (FBMC-FCEyN-UBA), Buenos Aires, Argentina
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Abstract
More than a century of research, of which JEB has published a substantial selection, has highlighted the rich diversity of animal eyes. From these studies have emerged numerous examples of visual systems that depart from our own familiar blueprint, a single pair of lateral cephalic eyes. It is now clear that such departures are common, widespread and highly diverse, reflecting a variety of different eye types, visual abilities and architectures. Many of these examples have been described as 'distributed' visual systems, but this includes several fundamentally different systems. Here, I re-examine this term, suggest a new framework within which to evaluate visual system distribution in both spatial and functional senses, and propose a roadmap for future work. The various architectures covered by this term reflect three broad strategies that offer different opportunities and require different approaches for study: the duplication of functionally identical eyes, the expression of multiple, functionally distinct eye types in parallel and the use of dispersed photoreceptors to mediate visual behaviour without eyes. Within this context, I explore some of the possible implications of visual system architecture for how visual information is collected and integrated, which has remained conceptually challenging in systems with a large degree of spatial and/or functional distribution. I highlight two areas that should be prioritised in future investigations: the whole-organism approach to behaviour and signal integration, and the evolution of visual system architecture across Metazoa. Recent advances have been made in both areas, through well-designed ethological experiments and the deployment of molecular tools.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lauren Sumner-Rooney
- Museum für Naturkunde, Leibniz Institute for Biodiversity and Evolution, Invalidenstrasse 43, 10115 Berlin, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Brodrick E, Jékely G. Photobehaviours guided by simple photoreceptor systems. Anim Cogn 2023; 26:1817-1835. [PMID: 37650997 PMCID: PMC10770211 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-023-01818-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/15/2023] [Revised: 07/29/2023] [Accepted: 08/01/2023] [Indexed: 09/01/2023]
Abstract
Light provides a widely abundant energy source and valuable sensory cue in nature. Most animals exposed to light have photoreceptor cells and in addition to eyes, there are many extraocular strategies for light sensing. Here, we review how these simpler forms of detecting light can mediate rapid behavioural responses in animals. Examples of these behaviours include photophobic (light avoidance) or scotophobic (shadow) responses, photokinesis, phototaxis and wavelength discrimination. We review the cells and response mechanisms in these forms of elementary light detection, focusing on aquatic invertebrates with some protist and terrestrial examples to illustrate the general principles. Light cues can be used very efficiently by these simple photosensitive systems to effectively guide animal behaviours without investment in complex and energetically expensive visual structures.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Emelie Brodrick
- Living Systems Institute, University of Exeter, Stocker Road, Exeter, EX4 4QD, UK.
| | - Gáspár Jékely
- Living Systems Institute, University of Exeter, Stocker Road, Exeter, EX4 4QD, UK
- Centre for Organismal Studies, University of Heidelberg, 69120, Heidelberg, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Tsellarius AY. Notes on the Solution of Navigation Problems by the Desert Monitor (Varanus grisius, Reptilia, Sauria) in a Natural Environment. BIOL BULL+ 2022. [DOI: 10.1134/s1062359022080209] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/15/2023]
|
5
|
Rapid learning of a spatial memory task in a lacertid lizard (Podarcis liolepis). Behav Processes 2019; 169:103963. [PMID: 31545992 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2019.103963] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/10/2018] [Revised: 07/19/2019] [Accepted: 09/19/2019] [Indexed: 12/30/2022]
Abstract
Mammals and birds are capable of navigating to a goal using learned map-like representations of space (i.e. place learning), but research assessing this navigational strategy in reptiles has produced inconclusive results, in part due to the use of procedures that do not take account of the peculiarities of reptilian behavior and physiology. Here I present a procedure suitable for testing spatial cognition that exploits a naturally evolved, ethologically relevant ability common to many lizards (i.e. refuge seeking behavior). The procedure requires lizards to learn the location of an open refuge inside a rectangular arena containing artificial refuges in every corner, using distal extramaze visual cues and with no local cues marking the location of the open refuge. The procedure probes the lizards' place learning ability and effectively rules out the use of egocentric and response-based strategies. The described procedure was successfully used to demonstrate place learning in a lacertid lizard (Podarcis liolepis). Over the course of two weeks of training both the latency to entering the open refuge and the number of corners visited in each trial decreased gradually, indicating that learning had taken place in over 60% of the lizards tested. These results confirm that, under certain circumstances, lizards are capable of navigating to a goal using a place learning strategy.
Collapse
|
6
|
Lindecke O, Voigt CC, Pētersons G, Holland RA. Polarized skylight does not calibrate the compass system of a migratory bat. Biol Lett 2016; 11:20150525. [PMID: 26382077 DOI: 10.1098/rsbl.2015.0525] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
In a recent study, Greif et al. (Greif et al. Nat Commun 5, 4488. (doi:10.1038/ncomms5488)) demonstrated a functional role of polarized light for a bat species confronted with a homing task. These non-migratory bats appeared to calibrate their magnetic compass by using polarized skylight at dusk, yet it is unknown if migratory bats also use these cues for calibration. During autumn migration, we equipped Nathusius' bats, Pipistrellus nathusii, with radio transmitters and tested if experimental animals exposed during dusk to a 90° rotated band of polarized light would head in a different direction compared with control animals. After release, bats of both groups continued their journey in the same direction. This observation argues against the use of a polarization-calibrated magnetic compass by this migratory bat and questions that the ability of using polarized light for navigation is a consistent feature in bats. This finding matches with observations in some passerine birds that used polarized light for calibration of their magnetic compass before but not during migration.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Oliver Lindecke
- Leibniz Institute for Zoo and Wildlife Research, Evolutionary Ecology Research Group, Alfred-Kowalke-Str. 17, Berlin 10315, Germany Department Animal Behaviour, Freie Universität Berlin, Takustr. 3, Berlin 14195, Germany
| | - Christian C Voigt
- Leibniz Institute for Zoo and Wildlife Research, Evolutionary Ecology Research Group, Alfred-Kowalke-Str. 17, Berlin 10315, Germany Department Animal Behaviour, Freie Universität Berlin, Takustr. 3, Berlin 14195, Germany
| | - Gunārs Pētersons
- Faculty of Veterinary Medicine, Latvia University of Agriculture, K. Helmaņa 8, Jelgava 3004, Latvia
| | - Richard A Holland
- School of Biological Sciences, Queen's University Belfast, Belfast BT9 7BL, UK
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
A functional role of the sky's polarization pattern for orientation in the greater mouse-eared bat. Nat Commun 2014; 5:4488. [PMID: 25050897 PMCID: PMC4109006 DOI: 10.1038/ncomms5488] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/26/2014] [Accepted: 06/24/2014] [Indexed: 11/16/2022] Open
Abstract
Animals can call on a multitude of sensory information to orient and navigate. One such cue is the pattern of polarized light in the sky, which for example can be used by birds as a geographical reference to calibrate other cues in the compass mechanism. Here we demonstrate that the female greater mouse-eared bat (Myotis myotis) uses polarization cues at sunset to calibrate a magnetic compass, which is subsequently used for orientation during a homing experiment. This renders bats the only mammal known so far to make use of the polarization pattern in the sky. Although there is currently no clear understanding of how this cue is perceived in this taxon, our observation has general implications for the sensory biology of mammalian vision. Many animals, including insects, birds, fish and reptiles, use polarized light for navigation, but this has not been reported before in mammals. In this study, Greif et al. demonstrate that a mammal, the female greater mouse-eared bat, Myotis myotis, can also use polarized light for navigation.
Collapse
|
8
|
Maoret F, Beltrami G, Bertolucci C, Foà A. Celestial Orientation with the Sun Not in View. J Biol Rhythms 2014; 29:144-7. [DOI: 10.1177/0748730414525741] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
The present investigation was aimed at testing whether the lizard sky polarization compass is time compensated. For this purpose, ruin lizards, Podarcis sicula, were both trained and tested for orientation inside a Morris water maze under clear skies with the sun not in view. During training, lizards showed a striking bimodal orientation along the training axis, demonstrating their capability of determining the symmetry plane of the sky polarization pattern and thus the use of polarization information in orientation. After reaching criteria, lizards were kept 7 days in a 6-h fast clock-shift treatment and then released with the sun not in view. Six-hour clock-shifted lizards showed a bimodal distribution of directional choices, which was oriented perpendicularly to the training axis, as it was expected on the basis of the clock-shift. The results show that the only celestial diurnal compass mechanism that does not need a direct vision of the sun disk (i.e., the sky polarization compass) is a time-compensated compass.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Francesco Maoret
- Dipartimento di Scienze della Vita e Biotecnologie, Università di Ferrara, Ferrara, Italy
| | - Giulia Beltrami
- Dipartimento di Scienze della Vita e Biotecnologie, Università di Ferrara, Ferrara, Italy
| | - Cristiano Bertolucci
- Dipartimento di Scienze della Vita e Biotecnologie, Università di Ferrara, Ferrara, Italy
| | - Augusto Foà
- Dipartimento di Scienze della Vita e Biotecnologie, Università di Ferrara, Ferrara, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Scali S, Sacchi R, Azzusi M, Daverio S, Oppedisano T, Mangiacotti M. Homeward bound: factors affecting homing ability in a polymorphic lizard. J Zool (1987) 2012. [DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-7998.2012.00977.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- S. Scali
- Museo Civico di Storia Naturale di Milano; Milano; Italy
| | - R. Sacchi
- DSTA - Dipartimento di Scienze della Terra e dell'Ambiente; Università di Pavia; Pavia; Italy
| | - M. Azzusi
- Museo Civico di Storia Naturale di Milano; Milano; Italy
| | - S. Daverio
- Museo Civico di Storia Naturale di Milano; Milano; Italy
| | - T. Oppedisano
- Museo Civico di Storia Naturale di Milano; Milano; Italy
| | - M. Mangiacotti
- Museo Civico di Storia Naturale di Milano; Milano; Italy
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Beltrami G, Parretta A, Petrucci F, Buttini P, Bertolucci C, Foà A. The lizard celestial compass detects linearly polarized light in the blue. J Exp Biol 2012; 215:3200-6. [DOI: 10.1242/jeb.074419] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
Summary
The present study first examined whether ruin lizards Podarcis sicula are able to orientate using plane polarized light produced by a LCD screen. Ruin lizards were trained and tested indoors, inside an hexagonal Morris water maze, positioned under the LCD screen producing white polarized light with a single E-vector, which provided an axial cue. White polarized light did not include wavelengths in the UV. Lizards orientated correctly either when tested with E-vector parallel to the training axis or after 90° rotation of the E-vector direction, and thus validating the apparatus. Further experiments examined whether in ruin lizards there is a preferential region of the light spectrum to perceive the E-vector direction of polarized light. For this purpose, lizards reaching learning criteria under white polarized light were subdivided into 4 experimental groups. Each group was respectively tested for orientation under a different spectrum of plane polarized light (named red, green, cyan and blue) with equalized photon flux density. Lizards tested under blue polarized light orientated correctly, whereas lizards tested under red polarized light were completely disoriented. Green polarized light was barely discernible by lizards, and thus insufficient for a correct functioning of their compass. When exposed to cyan polarized light, lizard orientation performances were optimal, indistinguishable from lizards detecting blue polarized light. Overall, the present results demonstrate that perception of linear polarization in the blue is necessary - and sufficient - for a proper functioning of the sky polarization compass of ruin lizards. This may be adaptively important, since detection of polarized light in the blue improves functioning of the polarization compass under cloudy skies, i.e. when the alternative celestial compass based on detection of the sun disk is rendered useless because the sun is obscured by clouds.
Collapse
|
11
|
LABRA ANTONIETA, VOJE KJETILL, SELIGMANN HERVÉ, HANSEN THOMASF. Evolution of the third eye: a phylogenetic comparative study of parietal-eye size as an ecophysiological adaptation in Liolaemus lizards. Biol J Linn Soc Lond 2010. [DOI: 10.1111/j.1095-8312.2010.01541.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
|