1
|
Garcia MAC, Carvalho TSD, Matsuda RH, Baffa O, Imbiriba LA, Souza VH. Forearm Posture Affects the Corticospinal Excitability of Intrinsic and Extrinsic Hand Muscles in Dominant and Nondominant Sides. J Appl Biomech 2024; 40:316-322. [PMID: 38925535 DOI: 10.1123/jab.2022-0314] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/21/2022] [Revised: 10/06/2023] [Accepted: 04/17/2024] [Indexed: 06/28/2024]
Abstract
Different forearm postures can modulate corticospinal excitability. However, there is no consensus on whether handedness plays a role in such a mechanism. This study investigated the effects of 3 forearm postures (pronation, neutral, and supination) on the corticospinal excitability of muscles from the dominant and nondominant upper limbs. Surface electromyography was recorded from the abductor digiti minimi, flexor pollicis brevis, and flexor carpi radialis from both sides of 12 right-handed volunteers. Transcranial magnetic stimulation pulses were applied to each muscle's hotspot in both cerebral hemispheres. Motor-evoked potential peak-to-peak amplitude and latency and resting motor threshold were measured. The data were evaluated by analysis of variance. The level of significance was set at 5%. The resting motor threshold was similar for the 3 muscles and both sides. Motor-evoked potential peak-to-peak amplitude from flexor pollicis brevis was lower during supination, and the dominant upper limb latency was longer. The flexor carpi radialis presented lower motor-evoked potential peak-to-peak amplitudes for neutral and shorter latencies during supination. Abductor digiti minimi seemed not to be affected by posture or side. Different muscles from dominant and nondominant sides may undergo corticospinal modulation, even distally localized from a particular joint and under rest.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marco Antonio Cavalcanti Garcia
- Programa de Pós-Graduação em Ciências da Reabilitação e Desempenho Físico-Funcional, Faculdade de Fisioterapia, Universidade Federal de Juiz de Fora, Juiz de Fora, MG, Brazil
- Grupo de Estudos em Neuro Biomecânica, Faculdade de Fisioterapia, Universidade Federal de Juiz de Fora, Juiz de Fora, MG, Brazil
- Departamento de Biofísica e Fisiologia, Instituto de Ciências Biológicas, Universidade Federal de Juiz de Fora, Juiz de Fora, MG, Brazil
| | - Thiago Santos de Carvalho
- Departamento de Biociências e Atividades Físicas, Escola de Educação Física e Desportos, Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro, RJ, Brazil
| | - Renan Hiroshi Matsuda
- Departamento de Física, Faculdade de Filosofia, Ciências e Letras de Ribeirão Preto, Universidade de São Paulo, Ribeirão Preto, SP, Brazil
| | - Oswaldo Baffa
- Departamento de Física, Faculdade de Filosofia, Ciências e Letras de Ribeirão Preto, Universidade de São Paulo, Ribeirão Preto, SP, Brazil
| | - Luis Aureliano Imbiriba
- Departamento de Biociências e Atividades Físicas, Escola de Educação Física e Desportos, Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro, RJ, Brazil
| | - Victor Hugo Souza
- Programa de Pós-Graduação em Ciências da Reabilitação e Desempenho Físico-Funcional, Faculdade de Fisioterapia, Universidade Federal de Juiz de Fora, Juiz de Fora, MG, Brazil
- Grupo de Estudos em Neuro Biomecânica, Faculdade de Fisioterapia, Universidade Federal de Juiz de Fora, Juiz de Fora, MG, Brazil
- Departamento de Física, Faculdade de Filosofia, Ciências e Letras de Ribeirão Preto, Universidade de São Paulo, Ribeirão Preto, SP, Brazil
- Department of Neuroscience and Biomedical Engineering, Aalto University, School of Science, Espoo, Finland
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Felippin MR, Azevedo IL, Saunier G, Keniston L, Nogueira-Campos AA. Grasping affordance judgments depend on the object emotional value. Front Hum Neurosci 2024; 18:1331253. [PMID: 38566999 PMCID: PMC10986176 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2024.1331253] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/31/2023] [Accepted: 02/28/2024] [Indexed: 04/04/2024] Open
Abstract
Introduction The concept of affordance refers to the opportunities for action provided by the environment, often conveyed through visual information. It has been applied to explain visuomotor processing and movement planning. As emotion modulates both visual perception and the motor system, it is reasonable to ask whether emotion can influence affordance judgments. If present, this relationship can have important ontological implications for affordances. Thus, we investigated whether the emotional value of manipulable objects affected the judgment of the appropriate grasping that could be used to interact with them (i.e., their affordance). Methods Volunteers were instructed to use a numerical scale to report their judgment on how an observed object should be grasped. We compared these judgments across emotional categories of objects (pleasant, unpleasant and neutral), while also considering the expected effect of object size. Results We found that unpleasant objects were rated as more appropriately graspable by a precision grip than pleasant and neutral objects. Simultaneously, smaller object size also favored this judgment. This effect was seen in all emotional categories examined in equal magnitude. Discussion Our findings suggest that the emotional value of objects modulates affordance judgments in a way that favors careful manipulation and minimal physical contact with aversive stimuli. Finally, we discuss how this affective aspect of our experience of objects overlaps with what affordances are conceptualized to be, calling for further reexamination of the relationship between affordances and emotions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Matheus Ribeiro Felippin
- Laboratory of Cognitive Neurophysiology, Institute of Biological Sciences, Department of Biophysics and Physiology, Federal University of Juiz de Fora (UFJF), Juiz de Fora, Minas Gerais, Brazil
- Post-Graduation Program in Biological Sciences, Carlos Chagas Filho Institute of Biophysics, Federal University of Rio de Janeiro (UFRJ), Rio de Janeiro, Brazil
| | - Ivo Lopes Azevedo
- Laboratory of Cognitive Neurophysiology, Institute of Biological Sciences, Department of Biophysics and Physiology, Federal University of Juiz de Fora (UFJF), Juiz de Fora, Minas Gerais, Brazil
- Post-Graduation Program in Biological Sciences, Carlos Chagas Filho Institute of Biophysics, Federal University of Rio de Janeiro (UFRJ), Rio de Janeiro, Brazil
| | - Ghislain Saunier
- Laboratory of Motor Cognition, Department of Anatomy, Federal University of Pará (UFPA), Belém, Brazil
- Post-Graduation Program in Human Movement Sciences, Federal University of Pará (UFPA), Belém, Brazil
| | - Les Keniston
- Department of Biomedical Sciences, Kentucky College of Osteopathic Medicine, University of Pikeville, Pikeville, KY, United States
| | - Anaelli Aparecida Nogueira-Campos
- Laboratory of Cognitive Neurophysiology, Institute of Biological Sciences, Department of Biophysics and Physiology, Federal University of Juiz de Fora (UFJF), Juiz de Fora, Minas Gerais, Brazil
- Post-Graduation Program in Rehabilitation Sciences and Physical-Functional Performance, Federal University of Juiz de Fora (UFJF), Juiz de Fora, Minas Gerais, Brazil
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Pierrieau E, Charissou C, Vernazza-Martin S, Pageaux B, Lepers R, Amarantini D, Fautrelle L. Intermuscular coherence reveals that affective emotional pictures modulate neural control mechanisms during the initiation of arm pointing movements. Front Hum Neurosci 2024; 17:1273435. [PMID: 38249573 PMCID: PMC10799348 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2023.1273435] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/06/2023] [Accepted: 12/04/2023] [Indexed: 01/23/2024] Open
Abstract
Introduction Several studies in psychology provided compelling evidence that emotions significantly impact motor control. Yet, these evidences mostly rely on behavioral investigations, whereas the underlying neurophysiological processes remain poorly understood. Methods Using a classical paradigm in motor control, we tested the impact of affective pictures associated with positive, negative or neutral valence on the kinematics and patterns of muscle activations of arm pointing movements performed from a standing position. The hand reaction and movement times were measured and electromyography (EMG) was used to measure the activities from 10 arm, leg and trunk muscles that are involved in the postural maintenance and arm displacement in pointing movements. Intermuscular coherence (IMC) between pairs of muscles was computed to measure changes in patterns of muscle activations related to the emotional stimuli. Results The hand movement time increased when an emotional picture perceived as unpleasant was presented as compared to when the emotional picture was perceived as pleasant. When an unpleasant emotional picture was presented, beta (β, 15-35 Hz) and gamma (γ, 35-60 Hz) IMC decreased in the recorded pairs of postural muscles during the initiation of pointing movements. Moreover, a linear relationship between the magnitude of the intermuscular coherence in the pairs of posturo-focal muscles and the hand movement time was found in the unpleasant scenarios. Discussion These findings reveal that emotional stimuli can significantly affect the content of the motor command sent by the central nervous system to muscles when performing voluntary goal-directed movements.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Emeline Pierrieau
- ToNIC, Toulouse NeuroImaging Center, Université de Toulouse, Inserm, Paul Sabatier University, Toulouse, France
- Aquitaine Institute for Cognitive and Integrative Neuroscience (INCIA), Université de Bordeaux, Bordeaux, France
| | - Camille Charissou
- ToNIC, Toulouse NeuroImaging Center, Université de Toulouse, Inserm, Paul Sabatier University, Toulouse, France
- Institut National Universitaire Champollion, EIAP, Département STAPS, Rodez, France
| | - Sylvie Vernazza-Martin
- Université Paris Nanterre, UFR-STAPS, Nanterre, France
- Laboratoire des interactions Cognition, Action, Émotion - LICAÉ, UFR STAPS, Université Paris Nanterre, Nanterre, France
| | - Benjamin Pageaux
- Centre de recherche de l'Institut universitaire de gériatrie de Montréal (CRIUGM), Montréal, QC, Canada
- École de kinésiologie et des sciences de l'activité physique (EKSAP), Faculté de médecine, Université de Montréal, Montréal, QC, Canada
- Centre interdisciplinaire de recherche sur le cerveau et l'apprentissage (CIRCA), Montréal, QC, Canada
| | - Romuald Lepers
- CAPS UMR1093, Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM), Faculté des Sciences du Sport, Université de Bourgogne-Franche-Comté, Dijon, France
| | - David Amarantini
- ToNIC, Toulouse NeuroImaging Center, Université de Toulouse, Inserm, Paul Sabatier University, Toulouse, France
| | - Lilian Fautrelle
- ToNIC, Toulouse NeuroImaging Center, Université de Toulouse, Inserm, Paul Sabatier University, Toulouse, France
- Institut National Universitaire Champollion, EIAP, Département STAPS, Rodez, France
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Budini F, Christova M. Enhanced corticospinal excitability in the tibialis anterior during static stretching of the soleus in young healthy individuals. PLoS One 2023; 18:e0284289. [PMID: 37040389 PMCID: PMC10089312 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0284289] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/10/2022] [Accepted: 03/28/2023] [Indexed: 04/12/2023] Open
Abstract
Corticospinal excitability is known to be affected by afferent inflow arising from the proprioceptors during active or passive muscle movements. Also during static stretching (SS) afferent activity is enhanced, but its effect on corticospinal excitability received limited attention and has only been investigated as a single average value spread over the entire stretching period. Using transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) the present study was conducted to explore the time course of corticospinal excitability during 30 seconds SS. Motor evoked potentials (MEPs) after TMS were recorded from soleus (SOL) and tibialis anterior (TA) muscles in 14 participants during: a passive dynamic ankle dorsiflexion (DF), at six different time points during maximal individual SS (3, 6, 9, 18, 21 and 25 seconds into stretching), during a passive dynamic ankle plantar flexion (PF) and following SS. To explore the time course of corticospinal excitability during the static lengthened phase of a muscle stretch, the stretching protocol was repeated several times so that it was possible to collect a sufficient number of stimulations at each specific time point into SS, as well as during DF and PF. During passive DF, MEPs amplitude was greater than baseline in both TA and SOL (p = .001 and p = .005 respectively). During SS, MEPs amplitude was greater than baseline in TA (p = .006), but not in SOL. No differences between the investigated time points were found and no trend was detected throughout the stretching time. No effect in either muscle was observed during passive PF and after SS. These results could suggest that an increased activity of secondary afferents from SOL muscle spindles exert a corticomotor facilitation on TA. The muscle-nonspecific response observed during passive DF could instead be attributed to an increased activation within the sensorimotor cortical areas as a result of the awareness of the foot passive displacements.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Francesco Budini
- Institute of Human Movement Science, Sport and Health, Graz University, Graz, Austria
| | - Monica Christova
- Otto Loewi Research Center, Physiology Section, Medical University of Graz, Graz, Austria
- Institute of Physiotherapy, Institute of Applied Sciences FH-Joanneum, Graz, Austria
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
The pleasantness and unpleasantness of an object distinctively drives its grasping prediction: behavioral evidence. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2022; 87:1491-1500. [PMID: 36346479 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-022-01765-w] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/15/2022] [Accepted: 10/31/2022] [Indexed: 11/11/2022]
Abstract
Action and perception share a common sensorimotor network permitting a functional action-perception coupling. This coupling would permit to predict the outcome of others' actions. Moreover, recent findings suggest that action-perception linkage could be sensitive to emotional content of the visual scene. The present study sought to address how emotion inherent to an object (pleasantness and unpleasantness) affects action prediction processing. To this end, we compared the participants' temporal estimative of the hand contact with emotional objects in occlusion and full vision conditions. We found that the emotion strongly interfered in the prediction of its grasping. Indeed, the participants highly anticipated the touch instant for unpleasant valence compared to pleasant and neutral ones. Moreover, the visual conditions (i.e., occlusion and full vision) affect the magnitude of the predictive error except to unpleasant object. Accordingly, the present results unveil that pleasantness and unpleasantness of an object distinctively drive the prediction of its touch instant.
Collapse
|
6
|
Qiu F, Zhou Y, Zhang L, Zhang J, Liu H. Gender dimorphic M1 excitability during emotional processing: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study. PeerJ 2022; 10:e13987. [PMID: 36061749 PMCID: PMC9438768 DOI: 10.7717/peerj.13987] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/29/2022] [Accepted: 08/11/2022] [Indexed: 01/19/2023] Open
Abstract
Background It is widely held that emotions prime the body for action. However, the influence of gender on primary motor cortex (M1) excitability during emotional processing is not well explored. Methods Using single-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS), we stimulated the right or left M1 at 150 ms and 300 ms after emotional stimulation onset (presentation of negative, neutral, and positive pictures to male and female subjects). Motor-evoked potentials (MEPs) ratio induced by single-pulse TMS was used to assess changes in corticospinal excitability. Results In response to right M1 stimulation, males demonstrated higher MEP ratios following presentation of negative pictures at 150 ms while MEP ratios in response to presentation of positive pictures were greater at 300 ms. Furthermore, male subjects showed larger MEP ratios in right M1 versus left M1 at 300 ms after initiation of positive pictures, indicating lateralization of motor excitability in male subjects. Conclusions The current study thus provides neurophysiological evidence to support gender differences and functional lateralization of motor excitability in response to emotional stimuli.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Fanghui Qiu
- Department of Physical Education, Qingdao University, Qingdao, China
| | - Yu Zhou
- Department of Physiology and Pathophysiology, School of Basic Medical Sciences, Qingdao University, China
| | - Lanlan Zhang
- Department of Leisure Sports and Management, Guangzhou Sport University, Guangzhou, China
| | - Jian Zhang
- School of Psychology, Shanghai University of Sport, Shanghai, China
| | - Hui Liu
- Shanghai Punan Hospital of Pudong New District, Shanghai, China
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Forearm and Hand Muscles Exhibit High Coactivation and Overlapping of Cortical Motor Representations. Brain Topogr 2022; 35:322-336. [PMID: 35262840 PMCID: PMC9098558 DOI: 10.1007/s10548-022-00893-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/23/2021] [Accepted: 02/04/2022] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
Abstract
Most of the motor mapping procedures using navigated transcranial magnetic stimulation (nTMS) follow the conventional somatotopic organization of the primary motor cortex (M1) by assessing the representation of a particular target muscle, disregarding the possible coactivation of synergistic muscles. In turn, multiple reports describe a functional organization of the M1 with an overlapping among motor representations acting together to execute movements. In this context, the overlap degree among cortical representations of synergistic hand and forearm muscles remains an open question. This study aimed to evaluate the muscle coactivation and representation overlapping common to the grasping movement and its dependence on the stimulation parameters. The nTMS motor maps were obtained from one carpal muscle and two intrinsic hand muscles during rest. We quantified the overlapping motor maps in size (area and volume overlap degree) and topography (similarity and centroid Euclidean distance) parameters. We demonstrated that these muscle representations are highly overlapped and similar in shape. The overlap degrees involving the forearm muscle were significantly higher than only among the intrinsic hand muscles. Moreover, the stimulation intensity had a stronger effect on the size compared to the topography parameters. Our study contributes to a more detailed cortical motor representation towards a synergistic, functional arrangement of M1. Understanding the muscle group coactivation may provide more accurate motor maps when delineating the eloquent brain tissue during pre-surgical planning.
Collapse
|
8
|
Dilek B, Osumi M, Nobusako S, Erdoğan SB, Morioka S. Effect of Painful Electrical Stimuli on Readiness Potential in the Human Brain. Clin EEG Neurosci 2022; 53:114-123. [PMID: 34213973 DOI: 10.1177/15500594211030137] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
The readiness potential (RP), which is a slow negative electrical brain potential that occurs before voluntary movement, can be interpreted as a measure of intrinsic brain activity originating from self-regulating mechanisms. Early and late components of the RP may indicate clinical-neurophysiological features such as motivation, preparation, intention, and initiation of voluntary movements. In the present study, we hypothesized that electrical pain stimuli modulate the preparatory brain activity for movement. The grand average evoked potentials were measured at sensory motor regions with EEG during an experimental protocol consisting of painful and nonpainful stimuli. Our results demonstrated that painful stimuli were preceded by an enhanced RP when compared to non-painful stimuli at the Cz channel (p < 0.05). Furthermore, the mean amplitude of the RP at the early phase was significantly higher for the painful stimuli when compared to the non-painful stimuli (p < 0.05). Our results indicate that electrical painful stimuli, which can be considered as an unpleasant and stressful condition, modulate the motor preparation at sensory motor regions to a different extent when compared to non-painful electrical stimuli. Since early component of the RP represents cortical activation due to anticipation of the stimuli and the allocation of attentional resources, our results suggest that painful stimuli may affect the motor preparation processes and the prediction of the movement at the cortical level.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Burcu Dilek
- Faculty of Health Sciences, Department of Occupational Therapy, 37521Trakya University, Edirne, Turkey.,Department of Neuroscience, Istanbul Medipol University, Institute of Health Sciences, Istanbul, Turkey
| | - Michihiro Osumi
- Neuro Rehabilitatition Research Center, 111074Kio University, Kitakatsuragi-gun, Japan
| | - Satoshi Nobusako
- Neuro Rehabilitatition Research Center, 111074Kio University, Kitakatsuragi-gun, Japan
| | - Sinem Burcu Erdoğan
- Faculty of Engineering, Department of Medical Engineering, 162328Acıbadem Mehmet Ali Aydınlar University, İstanbul, Turkey
| | - Shu Morioka
- Neuro Rehabilitatition Research Center, 111074Kio University, Kitakatsuragi-gun, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Osumi M, Sumitani M, Nishi Y, Nobusako S, Dilek B, Morioka S. Fear of movement-related pain disturbs cortical preparatory activity after becoming aware of motor intention. Behav Brain Res 2021; 411:113379. [PMID: 34051229 DOI: 10.1016/j.bbr.2021.113379] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/20/2020] [Revised: 05/14/2021] [Accepted: 05/19/2021] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
Fear of movement-related pain is known to disturb the process of motor preparation in patients with chronic pain. In the present study, we aimed to clarify the neural mechanisms underlying the influence of fear movement-related pain on motor preparatory brain activity using Libet's clock and electroencephalography (EEG). Healthy participants were asked to press a button while watching a rotating Libet's clock-hand, and report the number on the clock ("W time") when they made the "decision" to press the button with their right index finger. Immediately after pressing the button, a painful electrical stimulus was delivered to the dorsum of the left hand, causing participants to feel fear of movement (button press-related pain). We found that fear of movement-related pain caused the W time to be early, and that the amplitudes of readiness potentials (RPs) increased after awareness of motor intention emerged. In addition, fear of movement-related pain caused over-activation of the medial frontal cortex, supplementary motor area, cingulate motor area, and primary motor cortex after participants became aware of their motor intention. Such over-activation might result from conflict between the unrealized desire to escape from a painful experience and motivation to perform a required motor task.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michihiro Osumi
- Graduate School of Health Science, Kio University, Nara, Japan; Neurorehabilitation Research Center, Kio University, Nara, Japan.
| | - Masahiko Sumitani
- Department of Pain and Palliative Medicine, The University of Tokyo Hospital, Tokyo, Japan
| | - Yuki Nishi
- Graduate School of Health Science, Kio University, Nara, Japan
| | - Satoshi Nobusako
- Graduate School of Health Science, Kio University, Nara, Japan; Neurorehabilitation Research Center, Kio University, Nara, Japan
| | - Burcu Dilek
- Trakya University, Faculty of Health Sciences, Department of Occupational Therapy, Edirne, Turkey; Institute of Health Sciences, PhD Candidate in Neuroscience, Istanbul Medipol University, Istanbul, Turkey
| | - Shu Morioka
- Graduate School of Health Science, Kio University, Nara, Japan; Neurorehabilitation Research Center, Kio University, Nara, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Azevedo IL, Keniston L, Rocha HR, Imbiriba LA, Saunier G, Nogueira-Campos AA. Emotional categorization of objects: A novel clustering approach and the effect of depression. Behav Brain Res 2021; 406:113223. [PMID: 33677014 DOI: 10.1016/j.bbr.2021.113223] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/18/2020] [Revised: 02/16/2021] [Accepted: 02/26/2021] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
Abstract
Most everyday actions engender interactions with meaningful emotionally-laden stimuli. This study aimed to select pictures of objects as emotional stimulus of affordance to be grasped. The participant's depression trait was also assessed to examine its effect on the judgment of these pictures, and time spent in the classification was computed. Sixty-three participants joined this study. Self-Assessment-Manikin scale was used to classify pictures of the objects, and Beck Depression Inventory was applied to distribute the sample according depression trait. Cluster analysis was used in the classification of 123 objects based on valence and arousal values. Cluster results returned 102 classified pictures in three categories: pleasant (21), neutral (48) and unpleasant (33). Where cluster analysis did not agree, the picture was excluded and not used any further (21). Pleasant pictures presented the highest valence values and unpleasant pictures the lowest, and both categories returned the highest arousal level. In the middle of the valence range, the neutral category evoked the lowest arousal levels. Participants were slower to classify unpleasant pictures in valence sub-scale and faster to classify neutral pictures in arousal one. There was no effect of depression in the response time needed to score the pictures. Thus, agreement of high-performance soft clustering algorithms emerged as a good tool to classify pictures representing objects based on valence and arousal dimensions. Depression trait does not significantly affect the accuracy or time-order of emotional classification. Finally, we presented a set of emotional stimuli that can be employed to examine distinct aspects of emotion over physiology and behavior.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ivo Lopes Azevedo
- Cognitive Neurophysiology Laboratory (LabNeuro), Department of Physiology, Institute of Biological Sciences, Federal University of Juiz de Fora, Minas Gerais, Brazil
| | - Les Keniston
- Department of Physical Therapy, University of Maryland Eastern Shore, MD, United States
| | - Helena Ribeiro Rocha
- Cognitive Neurophysiology Laboratory (LabNeuro), Department of Physiology, Institute of Biological Sciences, Federal University of Juiz de Fora, Minas Gerais, Brazil
| | - Luiz Aureliano Imbiriba
- Center of Study of Human Movement (NEMoH), Department of Bioscience, Federal University of Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro, Brazil
| | - Ghislain Saunier
- Motor Cognition Laboratory, Department of Anatomy, Federal University of Pará, Pará, Brazil
| | - Anaelli A Nogueira-Campos
- Cognitive Neurophysiology Laboratory (LabNeuro), Department of Physiology, Institute of Biological Sciences, Federal University of Juiz de Fora, Minas Gerais, Brazil.
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Garcia MAC, Souza VH, Lindolfo-Almas J, Matsuda RH, Nogueira-Campos AA. Motor potential evoked by transcranial magnetic stimulation depends on the placement protocol of recording electrodes: a pilot study. Biomed Phys Eng Express 2020; 6:047003. [PMID: 33444285 DOI: 10.1088/2057-1976/ab950a] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE There seems to be no consensus in the literature regarding the protocol of surface electromyography (sEMG) electrode placement for recording motor evoked potentials (MEP) in transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) applications. Thus, the aim of this study was to investigate the effect on the MEP amplitude bytwo different protocols for electrode placement. METHODS sEMG electrodes were placed on three upper arm muscles (biceps brachii, flexor carpi radialis, and flexor pollicis brevis) of six right-handed subjects following two different protocols (1 and 2), which varied according to the interelectrode distance and location relative to the muscle. TMS pulses were applied to the hotspot of biceps brachii, while sEMGwas recorded from the two protocols and for each muscle simultaneously. MAIN RESULTS Greater MEP amplitudes were obtained for Protocol 1 compared to Protocol 2 (P < 0.05). SIGNIFICANCE Different electrode placement protocols may result in distinct MEP amplitudes, which should be taken into account when adjusting the intensity on single and repetitive TMS sessions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marco Antonio Cavalcanti Garcia
- Programa de Pós-Graduação em Ciências da Reabilitação e Desempenho Físico-Funcional, Faculdade de Fisioterapia, Universidade Federal de Juiz de Fora, Juiz de Fora, Brazil. Laboratório de Neurofisiologia Cognitiva, Departamento de Fisiologia, Instituto de Ciências Biológicas, Universidade Federal de Juiz de Fora, Juiz de Fora, Brazil. Laboratório de Biomagnetismo, Faculdade de Filosofia, Ciências e Letras de Ribeirão Preto, Universidade de São Paulo, Ribeirão Preto, Brazil
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
12
|
Lima Portugal LC, Alves RDCS, Junior OF, Sanchez TA, Mocaiber I, Volchan E, Smith Erthal F, David IA, Kim J, Oliveira L, Padmala S, Chen G, Pessoa L, Pereira MG. Interactions between emotion and action in the brain. Neuroimage 2020; 214:116728. [PMID: 32199954 PMCID: PMC7485650 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116728] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/26/2019] [Revised: 03/04/2020] [Accepted: 03/07/2020] [Indexed: 11/16/2022] Open
Abstract
A growing literature supports the existence of interactions between emotion and action in the brain, and the central participation of the anterior midcingulate cortex (aMCC) in this regard. In the present functional magnetic resonance imaging study, we sought to investigate the role of self-relevance during such interactions by varying the context in which threating pictures were presented (with guns pointed towards or away from the observer). Participants performed a simple visual detection task following exposure to such stimuli. Except for voxelwise tests, we adopted a Bayesian analysis framework which evaluated evidence for the hypotheses of interest, given the data, in a continuous fashion. Behaviorally, our results demonstrated a valence by context interaction such that there was a tendency of speeding up responses to targets after viewing threat pictures directed towards the participant. In the brain, interaction patterns that paralleled those observed behaviorally were observed most notably in the middle temporal gyrus, supplementary motor area, precentral gyrus, and anterior insula. In these regions, activity was overall greater during threat conditions relative to neutral ones, and this effect was enhanced in the directed towards context. A valence by context interaction was observed in the aMCC too, where we also observed a correlation (across participants) of evoked responses and reaction time data. Taken together, our study revealed the context-sensitive engagement of motor-related areas during emotional perception, thus supporting the idea that emotion and action interact in important ways in the brain.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Liana Catarina Lima Portugal
- Department of Physiology and Pharmacology, Laboratory of Neurophysiology of Behavior, Biomedical Institute, Federal Fluminense University, Niterói, RJ, Brazil
| | - Rita de Cássia Soares Alves
- Department of Physiology and Pharmacology, Laboratory of Neurophysiology of Behavior, Biomedical Institute, Federal Fluminense University, Niterói, RJ, Brazil
| | - Orlando Fernandes Junior
- Laboratory of Neuroimaging and Psychophysiology, Department of Radiology, Faculty of Medicine, Federal University of Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro, RJ, Brazil
| | - Tiago Arruda Sanchez
- Laboratory of Neuroimaging and Psychophysiology, Department of Radiology, Faculty of Medicine, Federal University of Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro, RJ, Brazil
| | - Izabela Mocaiber
- Laboratory of Cognitive Psychophysiology, Department of Natural Sciences, Institute of Humanities and Health, Federal Fluminense University, Rio das Ostras, RJ, Brazil
| | - Eliane Volchan
- Laboratory of Neurobiology II, Institute of Biophysics Carlos Chagas Filho, Federal University of Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro, RJ, Brazil
| | - Fátima Smith Erthal
- Laboratory of Neurobiology II, Institute of Biophysics Carlos Chagas Filho, Federal University of Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro, RJ, Brazil
| | - Isabel Antunes David
- Department of Physiology and Pharmacology, Laboratory of Neurophysiology of Behavior, Biomedical Institute, Federal Fluminense University, Niterói, RJ, Brazil
| | - Jongwan Kim
- Department of Psychology, University of Maryland, College Park, MD, USA
| | - Leticia Oliveira
- Department of Physiology and Pharmacology, Laboratory of Neurophysiology of Behavior, Biomedical Institute, Federal Fluminense University, Niterói, RJ, Brazil
| | | | - Gang Chen
- Scientific and Statistical Computing Core, National Institute of Mental Health, USA
| | - Luiz Pessoa
- Department of Psychology, University of Maryland, College Park, MD, USA; Maryland Neuroimaging Center, University of Maryland, College Park, MD, USA
| | - Mirtes Garcia Pereira
- Department of Physiology and Pharmacology, Laboratory of Neurophysiology of Behavior, Biomedical Institute, Federal Fluminense University, Niterói, RJ, Brazil.
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Abstract
A novel two-dimensional matrix taxonomy, or atlas, of personality, emotion and behaviour is presented. The two dimensions of the atlas, affiliation and dominance, are demonstrated to have theoretical foundations in neurobiology and social psychology. Both dimensions are divided into five ordinal categories, creating a square matrix of 25 cells. A new catalogue of 20,669 English words descriptive of personality, emotion, behaviour, and power is also presented. The catalogue is more comprehensive than previous catalogues, and is novel in its inclusion of intrapersonal, group, and societal behaviours. All words in the catalogue were scored according to the atlas, facilitating visualisation in two dimensions. This enabled a contiguous and novel comparison of existing psychological taxonomies, as well as broader societal concepts such as leadership, ethics, and crime. Using the atlas, a novel psychological test is developed with improved sensitivity and specificity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anthony E. D. Mobbs
- Department of Psychology, Faculty of Human Sciences, Macquarie University, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Budini F, Kemper D, Christova M, Gallasch E, Rafolt D, Tilp M. Five minutes static stretching influences neural responses at spinal level in the background of unchanged corticospinal excitability. JOURNAL OF MUSCULOSKELETAL & NEURONAL INTERACTIONS 2019; 19:30-37. [PMID: 30839301 PMCID: PMC6454261] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVES Corticospinal tract excitability and spinal reflex pathways are transiently affected by short applications of static stretching. However, it remains unclear whether the duration and magnitude of these neurophysiological responses can be increased with a longer duration of the applied stretch. The purpose of this study was to investigate alterations in cortical and spinal excitability following five minutes static stretching. METHODS Seventeen participants (22.8±2.3 years old) were tested for the tendon tap reflex (T-reflex), Hoffman reflex (H-reflex) and motor-evoked potentials (MEPs) after transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) of the ankle flexor muscles in two separate occasions: before and after 5 minute static stretching or 5 minute control period, in a randomized order. RESULTS No changes were observed following the control condition. H/M ratio increased by 16.2% after stretching (P=.036). Furthermore, immediately after stretching it was observed a strong inhibition of the T-reflex (57.6% inhibition, P=.003) that persisted up to five minutes after stretching (16.2% inhibition, P=.013) but returned to baseline following 10 minutes. MEPs were not affected by stretching. CONCLUSIONS This study suggests that the neuromuscular responses that follow five minute of static stretching do not influence the excitability of the corticospinal tract and follow a different time course within spinal reflex pathways.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Francesco Budini
- Institute for Sport Science, Graz University, Austria,Corresponding author: Francesco Budini, Institut für Sportwissenschaft, Karl-Franzens-Universität, Mozartgasse 14/I 8010 Graz, Austria E-mail:
| | - Daniela Kemper
- Otto Loewi Research Center, Physiology Section, Medical University of Graz, Austria
| | - Monica Christova
- Otto Loewi Research Center, Physiology Section, Medical University of Graz, Austria,Institute of Physiotherapy, Institute of Applied Sciences FH-Joanneum, Graz, Austria
| | - Eugen Gallasch
- Otto Loewi Research Center, Physiology Section, Medical University of Graz, Austria
| | - Dietmar Rafolt
- Center for Medical Physics and Biomedical Engineering, Medical University of Vienna, Austria
| | - Markus Tilp
- Institute for Sport Science, Graz University, Austria
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Budini F, Christova M, Gallasch E, Kressnik P, Rafolt D, Tilp M. Transient Increase in Cortical Excitability Following Static Stretching of Plantar Flexor Muscles. Front Physiol 2018; 9:530. [PMID: 29942261 PMCID: PMC6004398 DOI: 10.3389/fphys.2018.00530] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/23/2018] [Accepted: 04/24/2018] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Spinal excitability in humans is inhibited by both passively holding a static position with the muscle lengthened (static stretching) and by a single non-active lengthening movement. However, whilst immediately after a passive lengthening movement the inhibition persists for several seconds, there seem to be an immediate recovery following static stretching. This result is counter intuitive and could be attributed to methodological procedures. Indeed, differently to what has been done until now, in order to study whether static stretching has a transient effect on the neuromuscular pathway, the procedure should be repeated many times and measurements collected at different time points after stretching. In the present study we repeated 60 times 30 s static stretching of ankle plantar flexors and measured tap reflex (T-reflex), Hoffman reflex (H-reflex), and motor evoked potentials (MEPs) from the Soleus muscle at several time points, starting from immediately after until 30 s following the procedure. T-reflex was strongly inhibited (range 31–91%, p = 0.005) and the inhibition persisted for 30 s showing a slow recovery (r = 0.541, p = 0.037). H-reflex was not affected by the procedure. Stretching increased the size of the MEPs (p < 0.0001), differences at times 0 and 2 s after stretching (p = 0.015 and p = 0.047, respectively). These results confirm that static stretching reduces muscle spindle sensitivity. Moreover it is suggested that post-activation depression of Ia afferents, which is commonly considered the cause of H-reflex depression during both dorsiflexion and static stretching, vanished immediately following stretching or is counteracted by an increased corticospinal excitability.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Monica Christova
- Otto Loewi Research Center, Physiology Section, Medical University of Graz, Graz, Austria.,Institute of Physiotherapy, Institute of Applied Sciences FH-Joanneum, Graz, Austria
| | - Eugen Gallasch
- Otto Loewi Research Center, Physiology Section, Medical University of Graz, Graz, Austria
| | - Paul Kressnik
- Center for Medical Physics and Biomedical Engineering, Medical University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
| | - Dietmar Rafolt
- Center for Medical Physics and Biomedical Engineering, Medical University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
| | - Markus Tilp
- Institute for Sport Science, Graz University, Graz, Austria
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Fear of movement modulates the feedforward motor control of the affected limb in complex regional pain syndrome (CRPS): A single-case study. Med Hypotheses 2018; 110:114-119. [DOI: 10.1016/j.mehy.2017.12.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/31/2017] [Revised: 11/06/2017] [Accepted: 12/01/2017] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
|
17
|
Ackerley R, Aimonetti JM, Ribot-Ciscar E. Emotions alter muscle proprioceptive coding of movements in humans. Sci Rep 2017; 7:8465. [PMID: 28814736 PMCID: PMC5559453 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-017-08721-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/24/2016] [Accepted: 07/18/2017] [Indexed: 12/29/2022] Open
Abstract
Emotions can evoke strong reactions that have profound influences, from gross changes in our internal environment to small fluctuations in facial muscles, and reveal our feelings overtly. Muscles contain proprioceptive afferents, informing us about our movements and regulating motor activities. Their firing reflects changes in muscle length, yet their sensitivity can be modified by the fusimotor system, as found in animals. In humans, the sensitivity of muscle afferents is modulated by cognitive processes, such as attention; however, it is unknown if emotional processes can modulate muscle feedback. Presently, we explored whether muscle afferent sensitivity adapts to the emotional situation. We recorded from single muscle afferents in the leg, using microneurography, and moved the ankle joint of participants, while they listened to evocative classical music to induce sad, neutral, or happy emotions, or sat passively (no music). We further monitored their physiological responses using skin conductance, heart rate, and electromyography measures. We found that muscle afferent firing was modified by the emotional context, especially for sad emotions, where the muscle spindle dynamic response increased. We suggest that this allows us to prime movements, where the emotional state prepares the body for consequent behaviour-appropriate reactions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rochelle Ackerley
- Aix Marseille Univ, CNRS, LNIA, FR3C, Marseille, France.,Department of Physiology, University of Gothenburg, 40530, Göteborg, Sweden
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
18
|
Abstract
In action theory, emotional actions are standardly treated as exceptions—cases where the “normal” springs of action are not functioning properly. My aim here is to argue that this is not so. We have plenty of evidence—beautifully brought together in the present special issue—that emotions play a crucial and often constitutive role in all the important phases of action preparation and initiation. Most of our actions are less stupid than, say, Zidane’s head-butt (a paradigmatic emotional action), but all of our actions have emotional components. Actions can be more or less emotional, but they are never completely nonemotional.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Bence Nanay
- Centre for Philosophical Psychology, University of Antwerp, Belgium and University of Cambridge, UK
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Budini F, Gallasch E, Christova M, Rafolt D, Rauscher AB, Tilp M. One minute static stretch of plantar flexors transiently increases H reflex excitability and exerts no effect on corticospinal pathways. Exp Physiol 2017; 102:901-910. [PMID: 28585766 DOI: 10.1113/ep086374] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/23/2017] [Accepted: 05/24/2017] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
NEW FINDINGS What is the central question of this study? What mediates neural responses following static stretching, and how long do these influences last? What is the main finding and its importance? This study shows that 1 min of static stretching inhibits the tendon tap reflex and facilitates the H reflex without influencing motor-evoked potentials. The results indicate that at least two different mechanisms mediate neural responses after static stretching. The purpose of this study was to determine whether the neural responses observed after static stretching are mediated by sensitivity of muscle spindles, spinal excitability or cortical excitability and how long these influences last. Nineteen volunteers (25.7 ± 5.6 years old) were tested for the tendon tap reflex (T-reflex), H reflex and motor-evoked potentials on ankle flexors and extensors immediately, 5 and 10 min after 1 min static stretching applied at individual maximal ankle dorsiflexion, as well as immediately, 5 and 10 min after a control period of the same duration. Comparison of measurements collected immediately after stretching or control conditions revealed that the T-reflex was weaker after stretching than after control (-59.2% P = 0.000). The T-reflex showed a slow recovery rate within the first 150 s after stretching, but 5 min after the inhibition had disappeared. The H reflex increased immediately after stretching (+18.3%, P = 0.036), showed a quick tendency to recover and returned to control values within 5 min from stretching. Motor-evoked potentials were not affected by the procedure. These results suggest that 1 min of static stretching primarily decreases muscle spindle sensitivity and facilitates the H reflex, whereas effects on the motor cortex can be excluded.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Eugen Gallasch
- Institute of Physiology, Medical University of Graz, Graz, Austria
| | - Monica Christova
- Institute of Physiology, Medical University of Graz, Graz, Austria.,Institute of Physiotherapy, Institute of Applied Sciences FH-Joanneum, Graz, Austria
| | - Dietmar Rafolt
- Center for Medical Physics and Biomedical Engineering, Medical University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
| | | | - Markus Tilp
- Institute for Sport Science, Graz University, Graz, Austria
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Nogueira-Campos AA, Saunier G, Della-Maggiore V, De Oliveira LAS, Rodrigues EC, Vargas CD. Observing Grasping Actions Directed to Emotion-Laden Objects: Effects upon Corticospinal Excitability. Front Hum Neurosci 2016; 10:434. [PMID: 27625602 PMCID: PMC5004483 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00434] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/15/2016] [Accepted: 08/12/2016] [Indexed: 12/24/2022] Open
Abstract
The motor system is recruited whenever one executes an action as well as when one observes the same action being executed by others. Although it is well established that emotion modulates the motor system, the effect of observing other individuals acting in an emotional context is particularly elusive. The main aim of this study was to investigate the effect induced by the observation of grasping directed to emotion-laden objects upon corticospinal excitability (CSE). Participants classified video-clips depicting the right-hand of an actor grasping emotion-laden objects. Twenty video-clips differing in terms of valence but balanced in arousal level were selected. Motor evoked potentials (MEPs) were then recorded from the first dorsal interosseous using transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) while the participants observed the selected emotional video-clips. During the video-clip presentation, TMS pulses were randomly applied at one of two different time points of grasping: (1) maximum grip aperture, and (2) object contact time. CSE was higher during the observation of grasping directed to unpleasant objects compared to pleasant ones. These results indicate that when someone observes an action of grasping directed to emotion-laden objects, the effect of the object valence promotes a specific modulation over the motor system.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Ghislain Saunier
- Laboratory of Motor Cognition, Department of Anatomy, Federal University of Pará Belém, Brazil
| | - Valeria Della-Maggiore
- IFIBIO Houssay, Department of Physiology and Biophysics, School of Medicine, University of Buenos Aires Buenos Aires, Argentina
| | | | - Erika C Rodrigues
- Post-Graduate Program in Rehabilitation Sciences, Unisuam Rio de Janeiro, Brazil
| | - Claudia D Vargas
- Laboratory of Neurobiology II, Neurobiology Program, Institute of Biophysics Carlos Chagas Filho, Federal University of Rio de JaneiroRio de Janeiro, Brazil; Instituto de Neurologia Deolindo Couto, Federal University of Rio de JaneiroRio de Janeiro, Brazil
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Esteves PO, Oliveira LAS, Nogueira-Campos AA, Saunier G, Pozzo T, Oliveira JM, Rodrigues EC, Volchan E, Vargas CD. Motor planning of goal-directed action is tuned by the emotional valence of the stimulus: a kinematic study. Sci Rep 2016; 6:28780. [PMID: 27364868 PMCID: PMC4929477 DOI: 10.1038/srep28780] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/30/2016] [Accepted: 06/08/2016] [Indexed: 12/03/2022] Open
Abstract
The basic underpinnings of homeostatic behavior include interacting with positive items and avoiding negative ones. As the planning aspects of goal-directed actions can be inferred from their movement features, we investigated the kinematics of interacting with emotion-laden stimuli. Participants were instructed to grasp emotion-laden stimuli and bring them toward their bodies while the kinematics of their wrist movement was measured. The results showed that the time to peak velocity increased for bringing pleasant stimuli towards the body compared to unpleasant and neutral ones, suggesting higher easiness in undertaking the task with pleasant stimuli. Furthermore, bringing unpleasant stimuli towards the body increased movement time in comparison with both pleasant and neutral ones while the time to peak velocity for unpleasant stimuli was the same as for that of neutral stimuli. There was no change in the trajectory length among emotional categories. We conclude that during the “reach-to-grasp” and “bring-to-the-body” movements, the valence of the stimuli affects the temporal but not the spatial kinematic features of motion. To the best of our knowledge, we show for the first time that the kinematic features of a goal-directed action are tuned by the emotional valence of the stimuli.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- P O Esteves
- Laboratório de Neurobiologia II, Instituto de Biofísica Carlos Chagas Filho, Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro, Brasil
| | - L A S Oliveira
- Programa de Pós-graduação em Ciências da Reabilitação - Centro Universitário Augusto Motta, Rio de Janeiro, Brasil
| | - A A Nogueira-Campos
- Departamento de Fisiologia, Instituto de Ciências Biológicas, Universidade Federal de Juiz de Fora, Juiz de Fora, Brasil
| | - G Saunier
- Laboratório de Cognição Motora, Instituto de Ciências Biológicas, Universidade Federal do Pará, Belém, Brasil
| | - T Pozzo
- INSERM - U1093 Cognition, Action, et Plasticité Sensorimotrice, Campus Universitaire, UFR STAPS, Dijon, France
| | - J M Oliveira
- Laboratório de Neurobiologia II, Instituto de Biofísica Carlos Chagas Filho, Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro, Brasil
| | - E C Rodrigues
- Programa de Pós-graduação em Ciências da Reabilitação - Centro Universitário Augusto Motta, Rio de Janeiro, Brasil
| | - E Volchan
- Laboratório de Neurobiologia II, Instituto de Biofísica Carlos Chagas Filho, Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro, Brasil
| | - C D Vargas
- Laboratório de Neurobiologia II, Instituto de Biofísica Carlos Chagas Filho, Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro, Brasil
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Kilteni K, Grau-Sánchez J, Veciana De Las Heras M, Rodríguez-Fornells A, Slater M. Decreased Corticospinal Excitability after the Illusion of Missing Part of the Arm. Front Hum Neurosci 2016; 10:145. [PMID: 27148005 PMCID: PMC4830822 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00145] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/19/2015] [Accepted: 03/21/2016] [Indexed: 12/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Previous studies on body ownership illusions have shown that under certain multimodal conditions, healthy people can experience artificial body-parts as if they were part of their own body, with direct physiological consequences for the real limb that gets 'substituted.' In this study we wanted to assess (a) whether healthy people can experience 'missing' a body-part through illusory ownership of an amputated virtual body, and (b) whether this would cause corticospinal excitability changes in muscles associated with the 'missing' body-part. Forty right-handed participants saw a virtual body from a first person perspective but for half of them the virtual body was missing a part of its right arm. Single pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation was applied before and after the experiment to left and right motor cortices. Motor evoked potentials (MEPs) were recorded from the first dorsal interosseous (FDI) and the extensor digitorum communis (EDC) of each hand. We found that the stronger the illusion of amputation and arm ownership, the more the reduction of MEP amplitudes of the EDC muscle for the contralateral sensorimotor cortex. In contrast, no association was found for the EDC amplitudes in the ipsilateral cortex and for the FDI amplitudes in both contralateral and ipsilateral cortices. Our study provides evidence that a short-term illusory perception of missing a body-part can trigger inhibitory effects on corticospinal pathways and importantly in the absence of any limb deafferentation or disuse.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Konstantina Kilteni
- Event Lab, Department of Personality, Evaluation and Psychological Treatment, University of BarcelonaBarcelona, Spain; IR3C Institute for Brain, Cognition, and Behaviour, University of BarcelonaBarcelona, Spain
| | - Jennifer Grau-Sánchez
- Cognition and Brain Plasticity Group, Bellvitge Biomedical Research Institute, L'Hospitalet de LlobregatBarcelona, Spain; Department of Basic Psychology, L'Hospitalet de Llobregat, University of BarcelonaBarcelona, Spain
| | - Misericordia Veciana De Las Heras
- Neurophysiology Section, Neurology Service, Hospital Universitari de Bellvitge, Bellvitge Biomedical Research Institute Barcelona, Spain
| | - Antoni Rodríguez-Fornells
- Cognition and Brain Plasticity Group, Bellvitge Biomedical Research Institute, L'Hospitalet de LlobregatBarcelona, Spain; Department of Basic Psychology, L'Hospitalet de Llobregat, University of BarcelonaBarcelona, Spain; Institució Catalana de Recerca i Estudis AvançatsBarcelona, Spain
| | - Mel Slater
- Event Lab, Department of Personality, Evaluation and Psychological Treatment, University of BarcelonaBarcelona, Spain; IR3C Institute for Brain, Cognition, and Behaviour, University of BarcelonaBarcelona, Spain; Institució Catalana de Recerca i Estudis AvançatsBarcelona, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Vicario CM, Rafal RD, Avenanti A. Counterfactual thinking affects the excitability of the motor cortex. Cortex 2015; 65:139-48. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.12.017] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/06/2014] [Revised: 12/03/2014] [Accepted: 12/18/2014] [Indexed: 12/12/2022]
|