1
|
Özdemir Ş, Şentürk YD, Ünver N, Demircan C, Olivers CNL, Egner T, Günseli E. Effects of Context Changes on Memory Reactivation. J Neurosci 2024; 44:e2096232024. [PMID: 39103222 PMCID: PMC11376331 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.2096-23.2024] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/30/2023] [Revised: 06/26/2024] [Accepted: 07/10/2024] [Indexed: 08/07/2024] Open
Abstract
While the influence of context on long-term memory (LTM) is well documented, its effects on the interaction between working memory (WM) and LTM remain less understood. In this study, we explored these interactions using a delayed match-to-sample task, where participants (6 males, 16 females) encountered the same target object across six consecutive trials, facilitating the transition from WM to LTM. During half of these target repetitions, the background color changed. We measured the WM storage of the target using the contralateral delay activity in electroencephalography. Our results reveal that task-irrelevant context changes trigger the reactivation of long-term memories in WM. This reactivation may be attributed to content-context binding in WM and hippocampal pattern separation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Şahcan Özdemir
- Department of Psychology, Sabancı University, Istanbul 34956, Turkey
| | | | - Nursima Ünver
- Department of Psychology, Sabancı University, Istanbul 34956, Turkey
| | - Can Demircan
- Department of Psychology, Sabancı University, Istanbul 34956, Turkey
| | - Christian N L Olivers
- Department of Experimental and Applied Psychology, Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam 1081 BT, the Netherlands
| | - Tobias Egner
- Department of Psychology & Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, North Carolina 27708
| | - Eren Günseli
- Department of Psychology, Sabancı University, Istanbul 34956, Turkey
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Cuthbert B, Standage D, Paré M, Blohm G. Visual working memory models of delayed estimation do not generalize to whole-report tasks. J Vis 2024; 24:16. [PMID: 39058482 PMCID: PMC11282892 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.7.16] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/31/2023] [Accepted: 04/09/2024] [Indexed: 07/28/2024] Open
Abstract
Whole-report working memory tasks provide a measure of recall for all stimuli in a trial and afford single-trial analyses that are not possible with single-report delayed estimation tasks. However, most whole-report studies assume that trial stimuli are encoded and reported independently, and they do not consider the relationships between stimuli presented and reported within the same trial. Here, we present the results of two independently conducted whole-report experiments. The first dataset was recorded by Adam, Vogel, and Awh (2017) and required participants to report color and orientation stimuli using a continuous response wheel. We recorded the second dataset, which required participants to report color stimuli using a set of discrete buttons. We found that participants often group their reports by color similarity, contradicting the assumption of independence implicit in most encoding models of working memory. Next, we showed that this behavior was consistent across participants and experiments when reporting color but not orientation, two circular variables often assumed to be equivalent.Finally, we implemented an alternative to independent encoding where stimuli are encoded as a hierarchical Bayesian ensemble and found that this model predicts biases that are not present in either dataset. Our results suggest that assumptions made by both independent and hierarchical ensemble encoding models-which were developed in the context of single-report delayed estimation tasks-do not hold for the whole-report task. This failure to generalize highlights the need to consider variations in task structure when inferring fundamental principles of visual working memory.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Benjamin Cuthbert
- Centre for Neuroscience Studies, Queen's University, Kingston, ON, Canada
| | - Dominic Standage
- Centre for Neuroscience Studies, Queen's University, Kingston, ON, Canada
- Department of Biomedical and Molecular Sciences, Queen's University, Kingston, ON, Canada
| | - Martin Paré
- Department of Biomedical and Molecular Sciences, Queen's University, Kingston, ON, Canada
- Department of Psychology, Queen's University, Kingston, ON, Canada
- School of Computing, Queen's University, Kingston, ON, Canada
| | - Gunnar Blohm
- Centre for Neuroscience Studies, Queen's University, Kingston, ON, Canada
- Department of Biomedical and Molecular Sciences, Queen's University, Kingston, ON, Canada
- Department of Psychology, Queen's University, Kingston, ON, Canada
- School of Computing, Queen's University, Kingston, ON, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Schurr R, Reznik D, Hillman H, Bhui R, Gershman SJ. Dynamic computational phenotyping of human cognition. Nat Hum Behav 2024; 8:917-931. [PMID: 38332340 PMCID: PMC11132988 DOI: 10.1038/s41562-024-01814-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/30/2023] [Accepted: 12/21/2023] [Indexed: 02/10/2024]
Abstract
Computational phenotyping has emerged as a powerful tool for characterizing individual variability across a variety of cognitive domains. An individual's computational phenotype is defined as a set of mechanistically interpretable parameters obtained from fitting computational models to behavioural data. However, the interpretation of these parameters hinges critically on their psychometric properties, which are rarely studied. To identify the sources governing the temporal variability of the computational phenotype, we carried out a 12-week longitudinal study using a battery of seven tasks that measure aspects of human learning, memory, perception and decision making. To examine the influence of state effects, each week, participants provided reports tracking their mood, habits and daily activities. We developed a dynamic computational phenotyping framework, which allowed us to tease apart the time-varying effects of practice and internal states such as affective valence and arousal. Our results show that many phenotype dimensions covary with practice and affective factors, indicating that what appears to be unreliability may reflect previously unmeasured structure. These results support a fundamentally dynamic understanding of cognitive variability within an individual.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Roey Schurr
- Department of Psychology, Center for Brain Sciences, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA.
| | - Daniel Reznik
- Department of Psychology, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany.
| | - Hanna Hillman
- Department of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, CT, USA
| | - Rahul Bhui
- Sloan School of Management, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, USA
- Institute for Data, Systems, and Society, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, USA
| | - Samuel J Gershman
- Department of Psychology, Center for Brain Sciences, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA
- Center for Brains, Minds, and Machines, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Lockhart HA, Dube B, MacDonald KJ, Al-Aidroos N, Emrich SM. Limitations on flexible allocation of visual short-term memory resources with multiple levels of goal-directed attentional prioritization. Atten Percept Psychophys 2024; 86:159-170. [PMID: 37985598 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-023-02813-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 10/26/2023] [Indexed: 11/22/2023]
Abstract
Studies suggest that visual short-term memory (VSTM) is a continuous resource that can be flexibly allocated using probabilistic cues that indicate test likelihood (i.e., goal-directed attentional priority to those items). Previous studies using simultaneous cues have not examined this flexible allocation beyond two distinct levels of priority. Moreover, previous studies have not examined whether there are individual differences in the ability to flexibly allocate VSTM resources, as well as whether this ability benefits from practice. The current study used a continuous report procedure to examine whether participants can use up to three levels of attentional priority to allocate VSTM resources via simultaneous probabilistic spatial cues. Three experiments were performed with differing priority levels, cues, and cue presentation times. Group level analysis demonstrated flexible allocation of VSTM resources; however, there was limited evidence that participants could use three goal-directed priority levels. A temporal analysis suggested that task fatigue, rather than practice effects, may interact with item priority. A Bayesian individual-differences analysis revealed that a minority of participants were using three levels of attentional priority, demonstrating that, while possible, it is not the predominant pattern of behaviour. Thus, we provided evidence that flexible allocation to three attention levels is possible under simultaneous cuing conditions for a minority of participants. Flexible allocation to three categories may be interpreted as a skill of high-performing participants akin to high memory capacity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Holly A Lockhart
- Department of Psychology, Brock University, 1812 Sir Isaac Brock Way, St Catharines, Ontario, L2S 3A1, Canada.
| | - Blaire Dube
- Department of Psychology, Brock University, 1812 Sir Isaac Brock Way, St Catharines, Ontario, L2S 3A1, Canada
- Department of Psychology, Memorial University of Newfoundland, 230 Elizabeth Avenue, St. John's, NL, A1C 5S7, Canada
| | - Kevin J MacDonald
- Department of Psychology, Brock University, 1812 Sir Isaac Brock Way, St Catharines, Ontario, L2S 3A1, Canada
| | - Naseem Al-Aidroos
- Department of Psychology, Brock University, 1812 Sir Isaac Brock Way, St Catharines, Ontario, L2S 3A1, Canada
- Department of Psychology, University of Guelph, 50 Stone Road East, Guelph, Ontario, N1G 2W1, Canada
| | - Stephen M Emrich
- Department of Psychology, Brock University, 1812 Sir Isaac Brock Way, St Catharines, Ontario, L2S 3A1, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Parfenov EA, Duncan NW. Increasing familiarity with the heartbeat counting task does not affect performance. Biol Psychol 2023; 183:108662. [PMID: 37598881 DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2023.108662] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/30/2023] [Revised: 07/21/2023] [Accepted: 08/17/2023] [Indexed: 08/22/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Interoception is typically defined as the processing and perception of internal signals. A common evaluation of interoceptive abilities is the heartbeat counting task, during which participants count their heartbeats over a period of time. It has been argued recently, however, that performance in this task may reflect processes other than cardiac sensation. OBJECTIVE This study aimed to: 1) observe heartbeat counting task performance changes across multiple repetitions of the task; and 2) compare performance in the heartbeat counting task with that for a visual counting task to investigate generalised propensities for how uncertain stimuli are reported. METHODS Seventy-nine healthy participants performed seven blocks each of the heartbeat and visual counting tasks. Scores for each of the tasks were compared across blocks to identify any change in performance. Performance in the two tasks was then correlated. The study was preregistered at https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/GWAB7. RESULTS Heartbeat counting task performance did not change over blocks. In contrast, scores for the visual counting task reduced over time. A positive correlation was seen between performance in the two tasks (ρ(79) = .27). CONCLUSION Heartbeat counting task performance is stable across repetitions of the task, not changing with increasing familiarity. This suggests that non-interoceptive factors, such as beliefs, may be involved. The correlation between the heartbeat and visual counting tasks may point to a general propensity in counting uncertain stimuli across sensory domains. Together, these results raise questions about the interpretation of the heartbeat counting task as a measure of interoception.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Evgeny A Parfenov
- Graduate Institute of Mind, Brain and Consciousness, Taipei Medical University, Taipei, Taiwan
| | - Niall W Duncan
- Graduate Institute of Mind, Brain and Consciousness, Taipei Medical University, Taipei, Taiwan.
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
de Winter JCF, Dodou D, Eisma YB. Responses to Raven matrices: Governed by visual complexity and centrality. Perception 2023; 52:645-661. [PMID: 37264787 PMCID: PMC10469510 DOI: 10.1177/03010066231178149] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/03/2022] [Accepted: 05/08/2023] [Indexed: 06/03/2023]
Abstract
Raven matrices are widely considered a pure test of cognitive abilities. Previous research has examined the extent to which cognitive strategies are predictive of the number of correct responses to Raven items. This study examined whether response times can be explained directly from the centrality and visual complexity of the matrix cells (edge density and perceived complexity). A total of 159 participants completed a 12-item version of the Raven Advanced Progressive Matrices. In addition to item number (an index of item difficulty), the findings demonstrated a positive correlation between the visual complexity of Raven items and both the mean response time and the number of fixations on the matrix (a strong correlate of response time). Moreover, more centrally placed cells as well as more complex cells received more fixations. It is concluded that response times on Raven matrices are impacted by low-level stimulus attributes, namely, visual complexity and eccentricity.
Collapse
|
7
|
Kerver GA, Engel SG, Gunstad J, Crosby RD, Steffen KJ. Deficits in cognitive control during alcohol consumption after bariatric surgery. Surg Obes Relat Dis 2023; 19:344-349. [PMID: 36443210 PMCID: PMC10040420 DOI: 10.1016/j.soard.2022.10.014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/10/2022] [Revised: 09/21/2022] [Accepted: 10/12/2022] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND While bariatric surgery results in substantial weight loss, one negative side effect of surgery is that patients often experience more rapid and intense intoxication effects after consuming alcohol. OBJECTIVES Given that alcohol use has been associated with impaired cognitive functioning in the general population, this study examined whether acute alcohol consumption after bariatric surgery immediately led to impaired cognitive control, and whether this effect was impacted by baseline levels of cognitive control. SETTING Nonprofit teaching hospital, United States. METHODS Participants were 34 adults who attended a laboratory visit before and 1 year after Roux-en-Y gastric bypass surgery, wherein they consumed a weight-based dose of alcohol and completed cognitive testing over the course of 3 hours. RESULTS A series of generalized mixed-effect models demonstrated that performance on the cognitive task generally improved over time, likely due to practice effects. However, following bariatric surgery, individuals with impaired cognitive control before consuming alcohol experienced greater commission errors immediately afterward. CONCLUSIONS These findings suggest that alcohol use after bariatric surgery may produce immediate deficits in inhibitory control among individuals who are already vulnerable to impaired cognitive control. Clinicians should seek to educate bariatric surgery candidates on this possible effect, as deficits in inhibitory control may ultimately lead to risky behaviors and poor adherence with postsurgical medical recommendations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gail A Kerver
- Sanford Center for Biobehavioral Research, Fargo, North Dakota; Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Science, School of Medicine and Health Sciences, University of North Dakota, Fargo, North Dakota.
| | - Scott G Engel
- Sanford Center for Biobehavioral Research, Fargo, North Dakota; Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Science, School of Medicine and Health Sciences, University of North Dakota, Fargo, North Dakota
| | - John Gunstad
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Kent State University, Kent, Ohio
| | - Ross D Crosby
- Sanford Center for Biobehavioral Research, Fargo, North Dakota; Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Science, School of Medicine and Health Sciences, University of North Dakota, Fargo, North Dakota
| | - Kristine J Steffen
- Sanford Center for Biobehavioral Research, Fargo, North Dakota; Department of Pharmaceutical Sciences, North Dakota State University, Fargo, North Dakota
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Chen L, Chang H, Rudoler J, Arnardottir E, Zhang Y, de Los Angeles C, Menon V. Cognitive training enhances growth mindset in children through plasticity of cortico-striatal circuits. NPJ SCIENCE OF LEARNING 2022; 7:30. [PMID: 36371438 PMCID: PMC9653476 DOI: 10.1038/s41539-022-00146-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/30/2022] [Accepted: 10/21/2022] [Indexed: 06/16/2023]
Abstract
Growth mindset, the belief that one's abilities can improve through cognitive effort, is an important psychological construct with broad implications for enabling children to reach their highest potential. However, surprisingly little is known about malleability of growth mindset in response to cognitive interventions in children and its neurobiological underpinnings. Here we address critical gaps in our knowledge by investigating behavioral and brain changes in growth mindset associated with a four-week training program designed to enhance foundational, academically relevant, cognitive skills in 7-10-year-old children. Cognitive training significantly enhanced children's growth mindset. Cross-lagged panel analysis of longitudinal pre- and post-training data revealed that growth mindset prior to training predicted cognitive abilities after training, providing support for the positive role of growth mindset in fostering academic achievement. We then examined training-induced changes in brain response and connectivity associated with problem solving in relation to changes in growth mindset. Children's gains in growth mindset were associated with increased neural response and functional connectivity of the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, striatum, and hippocampus, brain regions crucial for cognitive control, motivation, and memory. Plasticity of cortico-striatal circuitry emerged as the strongest predictor of growth mindset gains. Taken together, our study demonstrates that children's growth mindset can be enhanced by cognitive training, and elucidates the potential neurobiological mechanisms underlying its malleability. Findings provide important insights into effective interventions that simultaneously promote growth mindset and learning during the early stages of cognitive development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lang Chen
- Department of Psychology, Santa Clara University, Santa Clara, CA, 95053, USA.
- Neuroscience Program, Santa Clara University, Santa Clara, CA, 95053, USA.
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, 94305, USA.
| | - Hyesang Chang
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, 94305, USA.
| | - Jeremy Rudoler
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, 94305, USA
| | - Eydis Arnardottir
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, 94305, USA
| | - Yuan Zhang
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, 94305, USA
| | - Carlo de Los Angeles
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, 94305, USA
| | - Vinod Menon
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, 94305, USA.
- Department of Neurology and Neurological Sciences, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, 94305, USA.
- Stanford Neuroscience Institute, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, 94305, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Schmiedek F, Lövdén M, Ratcliff R, Lindenberger U. Practice-related changes in perceptual evidence accumulation correlate with changes in working memory. J Exp Psychol Gen 2022; 152:763-779. [PMID: 36136813 PMCID: PMC10030378 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001290] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
It has been proposed that evidence accumulation determines not only the speed and accuracy of simple perceptual decisions but also influences performance on tasks assessing higher-order cognitive abilities, such as working memory (WM). Accordingly, estimates of evidence accumulation based on diffusion decision modeling of perceptual decision-making tasks have been found to correlate with WM performance. Here we use diffusion decision modeling in combination with latent factor modeling to test the stronger prediction that practice-induced changes in evidence accumulation correlate with changes in WM performance. Analyses are based on data from the COGITO Study, in which 101 young adults practiced a battery of cognitive tasks, including three simple two-choice reaction time tasks and three WM tasks, in 100 day-to-day training sessions distributed over 6 months. In initial analyses, drift rates were found to correlate across the three choice tasks, such that latent factors of evidence accumulation could be established. These latent factors of evidence accumulation were positively correlated with latent factors of practiced and unpracticed WM tasks, both before and after practice. As predicted, individual differences in changes of evidence accumulation correlated positively with changes in WM performance. Our findings support the proposition that decision making and WM both rely on the active maintenance of task-relevant internal representations. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Florian Schmiedek
- Center for Lifespan Psychology, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany
- Department of Education and Human Development, DIPF j Leibniz Institute for Research and Information in Education, Frankfurt am Main, Germany
- Correspondence can go to
| | - Martin Lövdén
- Center for Lifespan Psychology, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany
- Department of Psychology, University of Gothenburg
| | | | - Ulman Lindenberger
- Center for Lifespan Psychology, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany
- Max Planck UCL Centre for Computational Psychiatry and Ageing Research, Berlin, Germany, and London, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Bertaccini R, Ellena G, Macedo-Pascual J, Carusi F, Trajkovic J, Poch C, Romei V. Parietal Alpha Oscillatory Peak Frequency Mediates the Effect of Practice on Visuospatial Working Memory Performance. Vision (Basel) 2022; 6:vision6020030. [PMID: 35737417 PMCID: PMC9230002 DOI: 10.3390/vision6020030] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/24/2022] [Revised: 05/25/2022] [Accepted: 05/27/2022] [Indexed: 11/23/2022] Open
Abstract
Visuospatial working memory (WM) requires the activity of a spread network, including right parietal regions, to sustain storage capacity, attentional deployment, and active manipulation of information. Notably, while the electrophysiological correlates of such regions have been explored using many different indices, evidence for a functional involvement of the individual frequency peaks in the alpha (IAF) and theta bands (ITF) is still poor despite their relevance in many influential theories regarding WM. Interestingly, there is also a parallel lack of literature about the effect of short-term practice on WM performance. Here, we aim to clarify whether the simple repetition of a change-detection task might be beneficial to WM performance and to which degree these effects could be predicted by IAF and ITF. For this purpose, 25 healthy participants performed a change-detection task at baseline and in a retest session, while IAF and ITF were also measured. Results show that task repetition improves WM performance. In addition, right parietal IAF, but not ITF, accounts for performance gain such that faster IAF predicts higher performance gain. Our findings align with recent literature suggesting that the faster the posterior alpha, the finer the perceptual sampling rate, and the higher the WM performance gain.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Riccardo Bertaccini
- Centro Studi e Ricerche in Neuroscienze Cognitive, Dipartimento di Psicologia, Alma Mater Studiorum—Università di Bologna, Campus di Cesena, 47521 Cesena, Italy; (R.B.); (G.E.); (J.M.-P.); (F.C.); (J.T.)
| | - Giulia Ellena
- Centro Studi e Ricerche in Neuroscienze Cognitive, Dipartimento di Psicologia, Alma Mater Studiorum—Università di Bologna, Campus di Cesena, 47521 Cesena, Italy; (R.B.); (G.E.); (J.M.-P.); (F.C.); (J.T.)
- Center for Neuroscience and Cognitive Systems@UniTn, Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia, 38068 Rovereto, Italy
| | - Joaquin Macedo-Pascual
- Centro Studi e Ricerche in Neuroscienze Cognitive, Dipartimento di Psicologia, Alma Mater Studiorum—Università di Bologna, Campus di Cesena, 47521 Cesena, Italy; (R.B.); (G.E.); (J.M.-P.); (F.C.); (J.T.)
- Departamento de Psicología Experimental, Procesos Cognitivos y Logopedia, Universidad Complutense de Madrid, 28223 Madrid, Spain
| | - Fabrizio Carusi
- Centro Studi e Ricerche in Neuroscienze Cognitive, Dipartimento di Psicologia, Alma Mater Studiorum—Università di Bologna, Campus di Cesena, 47521 Cesena, Italy; (R.B.); (G.E.); (J.M.-P.); (F.C.); (J.T.)
| | - Jelena Trajkovic
- Centro Studi e Ricerche in Neuroscienze Cognitive, Dipartimento di Psicologia, Alma Mater Studiorum—Università di Bologna, Campus di Cesena, 47521 Cesena, Italy; (R.B.); (G.E.); (J.M.-P.); (F.C.); (J.T.)
| | - Claudia Poch
- Departamento de Educación, Universidad de Nebrija, 28015 Madrid, Spain;
| | - Vincenzo Romei
- Centro Studi e Ricerche in Neuroscienze Cognitive, Dipartimento di Psicologia, Alma Mater Studiorum—Università di Bologna, Campus di Cesena, 47521 Cesena, Italy; (R.B.); (G.E.); (J.M.-P.); (F.C.); (J.T.)
- IRCCS Fondazione Santa Lucia, Via Ardeatina, 306/354, 00179 Roma, Italy
- Correspondence:
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Ades J, Mishra J. Systematic Review of the Longitudinal Sensitivity of Precision Tasks in Visual Working Memory. Vision (Basel) 2022; 6:7. [PMID: 35225968 PMCID: PMC8883912 DOI: 10.3390/vision6010007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/22/2021] [Revised: 12/23/2021] [Accepted: 01/07/2022] [Indexed: 11/17/2022] Open
Abstract
Precision of working memory (WM) refers to the objective performance of individuals when trying to recall the features of the encoded WM items. Studies of precision in VWM aim to identify whether differences in WM performance within individuals are sensitive to individual states or traits. In this systematic review, we study VWM precision and whether it reflects true differences in ability to accurately store information, and thereby possibly a more sensitive measure than discrete VWM span alone. Sifting through 327 abstracts, we identified 34 relevant articles. After assessing these articles with regard to our inclusion criteria to test participants at two separate time points and have a sample size of at least fifteen participants, we found four longitudinal studies regarding VWM precision. One review author and two reviewers independently assessed all studies in the screening and selection process and extracted outcome measures, study characteristics, and, when possible, test-retest reliability metrics. Given the small and heterogeneous sample, this systematic review could not yet provide conclusive evidence on the sensitivity of VWM precision paradigms. Future research of VWM should include longitudinal studies of precision, and address both test-retest reliability in healthy adults and changes in precision during key developmental trajectory periods and in clinical populations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- James Ades
- Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, CA 92093, USA;
| | | |
Collapse
|
12
|
Xu J, Deng M, Nan W, Cai D. The effects of working memory training in children revealed by behavioral responses and ERP. Brain Behav 2021; 11:e2310. [PMID: 34333867 PMCID: PMC8413785 DOI: 10.1002/brb3.2310] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/27/2020] [Revised: 07/12/2021] [Accepted: 07/13/2021] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND Recent studies have examined the effect of computerized cognitive training on working memory (WM), but the behavioral and neural effects were uncertain. Also, few studies have explored WM training effects on children using event-related potentials. The purpose of our study was to investigate the effects of WM training in children, including the effects on behavioral performance and neurophysiological outcomes. METHODS Forty-four healthy children (mean age = 7.76 years, SD = 0.57 years, 18 females) were assigned to the training and control groups. Over 20 training sessions, the training group participated in the computation-span and spatial N-back tasks, whereas the control group joined in normal class activities. They all completed the pre- and post-test evaluation of WM tasks (digit span backwards task and N-back task). RESULTS The results showed that WM training led to improved performance in the digit span backwards task and 2-back task of post-test evaluation, shortened P3a and P3b latencies in nontarget trials during the spatial 1-back task, shortened P3a latency in target and nontarget trials, as well as increased P3b amplitude and shortened P3b latency in target trials during the spatial 2-back task. CONCLUSIONS These results suggested that WM training might enhance children's behavioral performance on WM tasks and brought about neurophysiological changes. This study gives insights into the potential of WM training effects on children's behavioral performance and neurophysiological outcomes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jie Xu
- Department of PsychologyShanghai Normal UniversityShanghaiChina
| | - Meiqi Deng
- Department of PsychologyShanghai Normal UniversityShanghaiChina
- School of Foreign Languages in TourismShanghai Institute of TourismShanghaiChina
| | - Wenya Nan
- Department of PsychologyShanghai Normal UniversityShanghaiChina
| | - Dan Cai
- Department of PsychologyShanghai Normal UniversityShanghaiChina
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Adam KCS, Doss MK, Pabon E, Vogel EK, de Wit H. Δ 9-Tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) impairs visual working memory performance: a randomized crossover trial. Neuropsychopharmacology 2020; 45:1807-1816. [PMID: 32386395 PMCID: PMC7608353 DOI: 10.1038/s41386-020-0690-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/17/2020] [Revised: 04/01/2020] [Accepted: 04/23/2020] [Indexed: 01/02/2023]
Abstract
With the increasing prevalence of legal cannabis use and availability, there is an urgent need to identify cognitive impairments related to its use. It is widely believed that cannabis, or its main psychoactive component Δ9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC), impairs working memory, i.e., the ability to temporarily hold information in mind. However, our review of the literature yielded surprisingly little empirical support for an effect of THC or cannabis on working memory. We thus conducted a study with three main goals: (1) quantify the effect of THC on visual working memory in a well-powered sample, (2) test the potential role of cognitive effects (mind wandering and metacognition) in disrupting working memory, and (3) demonstrate how insufficient sample size and task duration reduce the likelihood of detecting a drug effect. We conducted two double-blind, randomized crossover experiments in which healthy adults (N = 23, 23) performed a reliable and validated visual working memory task (the "Discrete Whole Report task", 90 trials) after administration of THC (7.5 and/or 15 mg oral) or placebo. We also assessed self-reported "mind wandering" (Exp 1) and metacognitive accuracy about ongoing task performance (Exp 2). THC impaired working memory performance (d = 0.65), increased mind wandering (Exp 1), and decreased metacognitive accuracy about task performance (Exp 2). Thus, our findings indicate that THC does impair visual working memory, and that this impairment may be related to both increased mind wandering and decreased monitoring of task performance. Finally, we used a down-sampling procedure to illustrate the effects of task length and sample size on power to detect the acute effect of THC on working memory.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kirsten C S Adam
- Department of Psychology, University of California San Diego, San Diego, CA, USA.
- Institute for Neural Computation, University of California San Diego, San Diego, CA, USA.
| | - Manoj K Doss
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, USA
- Center for Psychedelic and Consciousness Research, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Elisa Pabon
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neuroscience, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA
- Grossman Institute for Neuroscience, Quantitative Biology, and Human Behavior, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA
| | - Edward K Vogel
- Grossman Institute for Neuroscience, Quantitative Biology, and Human Behavior, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA
- Department of Psychology, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA
- Institute for Mind and Biology, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA
| | - Harriet de Wit
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neuroscience, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Yam C, Merlo D, Stankovich J, Darby D, Gresle M, Kalincik T, Kilpatrick TJ, Lechner-Scott J, Taylor B, Barnett M, Butzkueven H, van der Walt A. The MSReactor computerized cognitive battery correlates with the processing speed test in relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler Relat Disord 2020; 43:102212. [PMID: 32474286 DOI: 10.1016/j.msard.2020.102212] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/03/2020] [Revised: 05/06/2020] [Accepted: 05/17/2020] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Monitoring and screening of cognitive function in the ambulatory setting requires simple, brief cognitive tests that are reproducible. MSReactor (MSR) is a web-based platform that screens psychomotor (processing) speed, attention and working memory using a game-like interface. The Processing Speed Test (PST) is a validated computerized version of the Symbol Digit Modalities test (SDMT) and component of the Multiple Sclerosis Performance Test (MSPT). OBJECTIVE To determine the baseline and 6-month predictive correlations between the MSReactor computerised cognitive battery and the PST. METHODS Prospectively enrolled relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis (RRMS) patients completed the MSR and the PST during 6-monthly clinic visits. Pearson's product-moment coefficients with partial correlation adjustment were calculated between the PST and MSR reaction times for Simple reaction test (SRT), Choice reaction test (CRT) and One- back test (OBK). RESULTS 379 RRMS patients from six tertiary MS centres in Australia were enrolled. The mean age was 40.4 years (SD 10.3) and median Expanded Disability Status Scale (EDSS) score was 1.5 (IQR 1.0 - 2.0). Most (66%) were on high efficacy disease-modifying treatment. Baseline PST scores correlated with the MSR reaction times: SRT (R=-0.40), CRT (R= -0.44) and OBK (R= -0.47), p <0.05. There was a moderate correlation between the first visit MSR and 6-month PST test for SRT (R= -0.37, p<0.001), CRT (R=-0.44, p < 0.001) and OBK (R= -0.43, p < 0.001) speed. CONCLUSIONS MSR-measured psychomotor speed, attention and working memory at baseline moderately correlates with baseline and 6-month PST; suggesting overlapping cognitive processes are being tested. Six-month test-retest reliability was acceptable for both tests.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Charmaine Yam
- Department of Neurology, Box Hill Hospital, Melbourne, Australia
| | - Daniel Merlo
- Department of Neuroscience, Central Clinical School, Monash University, Melbourne, Australia; Department of Neurology, Box Hill Hospital, Melbourne, Australia
| | - Jim Stankovich
- Department of Neuroscience, Central Clinical School, Monash University, Melbourne, Australia
| | - David Darby
- Department of Neurology, MSNI Service, Alfred Health, Melbourne, Australia; Department of Neuroscience, Central Clinical School, Monash University, Melbourne, Australia; Florey Institute of Neuroscience and Mental Health, Melbourne, Australia; Department of Neurology, Box Hill Hospital, Melbourne, Australia; Department of Neurology, Royal Melbourne Hospital, Parkville, Melbourne, Australia
| | - Melissa Gresle
- Department of Neuroscience, Central Clinical School, Monash University, Melbourne, Australia; Department of Neurology, Royal Melbourne Hospital, Parkville, Melbourne, Australia
| | - Tomas Kalincik
- Department of Neurology, Royal Melbourne Hospital, Parkville, Melbourne, Australia; CORe, Department of Medicine at RMH, University of Melbourne, Melbourne, Australia
| | - Trevor J Kilpatrick
- Florey Institute of Neuroscience and Mental Health, Melbourne, Australia; Department of Neurology, Royal Melbourne Hospital, Parkville, Melbourne, Australia
| | - Jeanette Lechner-Scott
- Department of Neurology, John Hunter Hospital, Newcastle, Australia; School of Medicine and Public Health, University Newcastle, Newcastle, Australia
| | - Bruce Taylor
- Department of Neurology, Royal Hobart Hospital, Hobart, Melbourne, Australia
| | - Michael Barnett
- Brain and Mind Centre, University of Sydney, Sydney, Australia
| | - Helmut Butzkueven
- Department of Neurology, MSNI Service, Alfred Health, Melbourne, Australia; Department of Neuroscience, Central Clinical School, Monash University, Melbourne, Australia; Department of Neurology, Box Hill Hospital, Melbourne, Australia
| | - Anneke van der Walt
- Department of Neurology, MSNI Service, Alfred Health, Melbourne, Australia; Department of Neuroscience, Central Clinical School, Monash University, Melbourne, Australia; Department of Neurology, Royal Prince Alfred Hospital, Sydney, Australia.
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Oudman E, Schut MJ, Ten Brink AF, Postma A, Van der Stigchel S. Visual working memory capacity in Korsakoff's amnesia. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2020; 42:363-370. [PMID: 32028852 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2020.1722800] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
Abstract
Introduction: Despite the interest in memory functioning in Korsakoff's syndrome (KS), visual working memory capacity in KS is still poorly understood. Results from the last decades on working memory limitations in KS appear to be mixed, clearly calling for further investigations.Method: We investigated visual working memory storage capacity of patients diagnosed with KS using a change detection paradigm. Nine patients diagnosed with KS and thirty age- and education- matched healthy controls were presented twice with 2, 3, 4, or 6 bars with different orientations and had to detect whether the orientation of the target bar had changed.Results: As a group, KS patients performed significantly worse than controls on all set sizes, with an average capacity of 1.13 items, compared to 1.75 items in the control participants. In case study analysis, 4 out of 9 patients performed significantly worse than control participants, while 5 had relatively intact visual working memory capacity. In patients, deficits in visual working memory could not be explained by other cognitive deficits.Conclusion: Based on these results, we conclude that visual working memory deficits are a prominent characteristic in some, but not all KS patients. Training visual working memory capacity could possibly optimize other cognitive difficulties in this population.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Erik Oudman
- Experimental Psychology, Helmholtz Institute, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands.,Korsakoff Center Slingedael, Lelie Care Group, Rotterdam, The Netherlands
| | - Martijn J Schut
- Experimental Psychology, Helmholtz Institute, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| | - Antonia F Ten Brink
- Experimental Psychology, Helmholtz Institute, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands.,Department of Psychology, University of Bath, Bath, UK
| | - Albert Postma
- Experimental Psychology, Helmholtz Institute, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands.,Korsakoff Center Slingedael, Lelie Care Group, Rotterdam, The Netherlands
| | | |
Collapse
|
16
|
Chen CC, Kuo JC, Wang WJ. Distinguishing the Visual Working Memory Training and Practice Effects by the Effective Connectivity During n-back Tasks: A DCM of ERP Study. Front Behav Neurosci 2019; 13:84. [PMID: 31057376 PMCID: PMC6478890 DOI: 10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00084] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/22/2018] [Accepted: 04/05/2019] [Indexed: 11/25/2022] Open
Abstract
Visual working memory (WM) training and practice can result in improved task performance and increased P300 amplitude; however, only training can yield N160 enhancements. N160 amplitudes are related to the spatial attention, the detection of novelty and the inhibitory control, while P300 amplitudes are related to the selective attention. Therefore, it could be speculated that the mechanisms underlying N160 and P300 production may differ to accommodate to their functions. Based on the different N160 engagements and different functional roles of N160 and P300, we hypothesized that the effects of visual WM training and practice can be dissociated by their brain effective connectivity patterns. We compared different neural connectivity configurations for the main task-related brain activities including N160 and P300 during the visual three-back task in subjects after visual WM training (the WM group) and after repetitive task practice (the control group). The behavioral result shows significantly greater improvement in accuracy after training and suggests that visual WM training can boost the learning process of this simple task. The N160 peak amplitude increased significantly after training over the anterior and posterior brain areas but decreased after practice over the posterior areas, indicating different mechanisms for mediating the training and practice effects. In support of our hypothesis, we observed that visual WM training alters the frontal-parietal connections, which comprise the executive control network (ECN) and the dorsal attention network (DAN), whereas practice modulates the parietal-frontal connections underpinning P300 production for selective attention. It should be noted that the analytic results in this study are conditional on the plausible models being tested and the experimental settings. Studies that employ different tasks, devices and plausible models may lead to different results. Nevertheless, our findings provide a reference for distinguishing the visual WM training and practice effects by the underlying neuroplasticity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chun-Chuan Chen
- Department of Biomedical Sciences and Engineering, National Central University, Taoyuan, Taiwan.,Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, National Central University, Taoyuan, Taiwan
| | - Ju-Che Kuo
- Department of Biomedical Sciences and Engineering, National Central University, Taoyuan, Taiwan
| | - Wei-Jen Wang
- Department of Computer Science and Information Engineering, National Central University, Taoyuan, Taiwan
| |
Collapse
|