1
|
Boven E, Cerminara NL. Cerebellar contributions across behavioural timescales: a review from the perspective of cerebro-cerebellar interactions. Front Syst Neurosci 2023; 17:1211530. [PMID: 37745783 PMCID: PMC10512466 DOI: 10.3389/fnsys.2023.1211530] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/24/2023] [Accepted: 08/21/2023] [Indexed: 09/26/2023] Open
Abstract
Performing successful adaptive behaviour relies on our ability to process a wide range of temporal intervals with certain precision. Studies on the role of the cerebellum in temporal information processing have adopted the dogma that the cerebellum is involved in sub-second processing. However, emerging evidence shows that the cerebellum might be involved in suprasecond temporal processing as well. Here we review the reciprocal loops between cerebellum and cerebral cortex and provide a theoretical account of cerebro-cerebellar interactions with a focus on how cerebellar output can modulate cerebral processing during learning of complex sequences. Finally, we propose that while the ability of the cerebellum to support millisecond timescales might be intrinsic to cerebellar circuitry, the ability to support supra-second timescales might result from cerebellar interactions with other brain regions, such as the prefrontal cortex.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ellen Boven
- Sensory and Motor Systems Group, Faculty of Life Sciences, School of Physiology, Pharmacology and Neuroscience, University of Bristol, Bristol, United Kingdom
- Neural and Machine Learning Group, Bristol Computational Neuroscience Unit, Intelligent Systems Labs, School of Engineering Mathematics and Technology, Faculty of Engineering, University of Bristol, Bristol, United Kingdom
| | - Nadia L. Cerminara
- Sensory and Motor Systems Group, Faculty of Life Sciences, School of Physiology, Pharmacology and Neuroscience, University of Bristol, Bristol, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Monteiro T, Rodrigues FS, Pexirra M, Cruz BF, Gonçalves AI, Rueda-Orozco PE, Paton JJ. Using temperature to analyze the neural basis of a time-based decision. Nat Neurosci 2023; 26:1407-1416. [PMID: 37443279 DOI: 10.1038/s41593-023-01378-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/28/2020] [Accepted: 06/12/2023] [Indexed: 07/15/2023]
Abstract
The basal ganglia are thought to contribute to decision-making and motor control. These functions are critically dependent on timing information, which can be extracted from the evolving state of neural populations in their main input structure, the striatum. However, it is debated whether striatal activity underlies latent, dynamic decision processes or kinematics of overt movement. Here, we measured the impact of temperature on striatal population activity and the behavior of rats, and compared the observed effects with neural activity and behavior collected in multiple versions of a temporal categorization task. Cooling caused dilation, and warming contraction, of both neural activity and patterns of judgment in time, mimicking endogenous decision-related variability in striatal activity. However, temperature did not similarly affect movement kinematics. These data provide compelling evidence that the timecourse of evolving striatal activity dictates the speed of a latent process that is used to guide choices, but not continuous motor control. More broadly, they establish temporal scaling of population activity as a likely neural basis for variability in timing behavior.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tiago Monteiro
- Neuroscience Programme, Champalimaud Foundation, Lisbon, Portugal
- Department of Biology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
| | | | - Margarida Pexirra
- Neuroscience Programme, Champalimaud Foundation, Lisbon, Portugal
- Sainsbury Wellcome Centre for Neural Circuits and Behaviour, University College London, London, UK
| | - Bruno F Cruz
- Neuroscience Programme, Champalimaud Foundation, Lisbon, Portugal
- NeuroGEARS Ltd., London, UK
| | - Ana I Gonçalves
- Neuroscience Programme, Champalimaud Foundation, Lisbon, Portugal
| | | | - Joseph J Paton
- Neuroscience Programme, Champalimaud Foundation, Lisbon, Portugal.
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Xie T, Huang C, Zhang Y, Liu J, Yao H. Influence of Recent Trial History on Interval Timing. Neurosci Bull 2023; 39:559-575. [PMID: 36209314 PMCID: PMC10073370 DOI: 10.1007/s12264-022-00954-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/01/2022] [Accepted: 07/10/2022] [Indexed: 11/30/2022] Open
Abstract
Interval timing is involved in a variety of cognitive behaviors such as associative learning and decision-making. While it has been shown that time estimation is adaptive to the temporal context, it remains unclear how interval timing behavior is influenced by recent trial history. Here we found that, in mice trained to perform a licking-based interval timing task, a decrease of inter-reinforcement interval in the previous trial rapidly shifted the time of anticipatory licking earlier. Optogenetic inactivation of the anterior lateral motor cortex (ALM), but not the medial prefrontal cortex, for a short time before reward delivery caused a decrease in the peak time of anticipatory licking in the next trial. Electrophysiological recordings from the ALM showed that the response profiles preceded by short and long inter-reinforcement intervals exhibited task-engagement-dependent temporal scaling. Thus, interval timing is adaptive to recent experience of the temporal interval, and ALM activity during time estimation reflects recent experience of interval.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Taorong Xie
- Institute of Neuroscience, State Key Laboratory of Neuroscience, CAS Center for Excellence in Brain Science and Intelligence Technology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Shanghai, 200031, China
| | - Can Huang
- Institute of Neuroscience, State Key Laboratory of Neuroscience, CAS Center for Excellence in Brain Science and Intelligence Technology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Shanghai, 200031, China
- University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, 100049, China
| | - Yijie Zhang
- Institute of Neuroscience, State Key Laboratory of Neuroscience, CAS Center for Excellence in Brain Science and Intelligence Technology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Shanghai, 200031, China
- University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, 100049, China
| | - Jing Liu
- Institute of Neuroscience, State Key Laboratory of Neuroscience, CAS Center for Excellence in Brain Science and Intelligence Technology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Shanghai, 200031, China
- University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, 100049, China
| | - Haishan Yao
- Institute of Neuroscience, State Key Laboratory of Neuroscience, CAS Center for Excellence in Brain Science and Intelligence Technology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Shanghai, 200031, China.
- Shanghai Center for Brain Science and Brain-Inspired Intelligence Technology, Shanghai, 201210, China.
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Rodents monitor their error in self-generated duration on a single trial basis. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2022; 119:2108850119. [PMID: 35193973 PMCID: PMC8892352 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.2108850119] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 12/16/2021] [Indexed: 01/19/2023] Open
Abstract
A fundamental question in neuroscience is what type of internal representation leads to complex, adaptive behavior. When faced with a deadline, individuals' behavior suggests that they represent the mean and the uncertainty of an internal timer to make near-optimal, time-dependent decisions. Whether this ability relies on simple trial-and-error adjustments or whether it involves richer representations is unknown. Richer representations suggest a possibility of error monitoring, that is, the ability for an individual to assess its internal representation of the world and estimate discrepancy in the absence of external feedback. While rodents show timing behavior, whether they can represent and report temporal errors in their own produced duration on a single-trial basis is unknown. We designed a paradigm requiring rats to produce a target time interval and, subsequently, evaluate its error. Rats received a reward in a given location depending on the magnitude of their timing errors. During the test trials, rats had to choose a port corresponding to the error magnitude of their just-produced duration to receive a reward. High-choice accuracy demonstrates that rats kept track of the values of the timing variables on which they based their decision. Additionally, the rats kept a representation of the mapping between those timing values and the target value, as well as the history of the reinforcements. These findings demonstrate error-monitoring abilities in evaluating self-generated timing in rodents. Together, these findings suggest an explicit representation of produced duration and the possibility to evaluate its relation to the desired target duration.
Collapse
|
5
|
Activation of Subthalamic Nucleus Stop Circuit Disrupts Cognitive Performance. eNeuro 2020; 7:ENEURO.0159-20.2020. [PMID: 32887694 PMCID: PMC7545431 DOI: 10.1523/eneuro.0159-20.2020] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/22/2020] [Revised: 07/14/2020] [Accepted: 08/26/2020] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
Much evidence supports a fundamental role for the subthalamic nucleus (STN) in rapidly stopping behavior when a stop signal or surprising event occurs, but the extent to which the STN may be involved in stopping cognitive processes is less clear. Here, we used an optogenetic approach to control STN activity in a delayed-match-to-position (DMTP) task where mice had to recall a response location after a delay. We first demonstrated that a surprising event impaired performance by both slowing the latency to respond and increasing the rate of errors. We next showed that these effects could be mimicked by brief optogenetic activation of the STN. Further, inhibiting STN during surprise blocked surprise-induced slowing, although without changing surprise-induced errors. These data are consistent with the hypothesis that STN is recruited by surprise to slow responding and that this can also interrupt cognitive processes. Under normal conditions STN-mediated stopping of behavior may slow or stop ongoing cognition to facilitate cognitive reorienting and adaptive responses to unexpected sensory information, but when malfunctioning, it could produce pathologies related to over-rigidity or increased distractibility.
Collapse
|
6
|
Turning the body into a clock: Accurate timing is facilitated by simple stereotyped interactions with the environment. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2020; 117:13084-13093. [PMID: 32434909 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1921226117] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/15/2022] Open
Abstract
How animals adapt their behavior according to regular time intervals between events is not well understood, especially when intervals last several seconds. One possibility is that animals use disembodied internal neuronal representations of time to decide when to initiate a given action at the end of an interval. However, animals rarely remain immobile during time intervals but tend to perform stereotyped behaviors, raising the possibility that motor routines improve timing accuracy. To test this possibility, we used a task in which rats, freely moving on a motorized treadmill, could obtain a reward if they approached it after a fixed interval. Most animals took advantage of the treadmill length and its moving direction to develop, by trial-and-error, the same motor routine whose execution resulted in the precise timing of their reward approaches. Noticeably, when proficient animals did not follow this routine, their temporal accuracy decreased. Then, naïve animals were trained in modified versions of the task designed to prevent the development of this routine. Compared to rats trained in the first protocol, these animals didn't reach a comparable level of timing accuracy. Altogether, our results indicate that timing accuracy in rats is improved when the environment affords cues that animals can incorporate into motor routines.
Collapse
|
7
|
Paton JJ, Buonomano DV. The Neural Basis of Timing: Distributed Mechanisms for Diverse Functions. Neuron 2019; 98:687-705. [PMID: 29772201 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.03.045] [Citation(s) in RCA: 214] [Impact Index Per Article: 35.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/02/2018] [Revised: 02/26/2018] [Accepted: 03/24/2018] [Indexed: 12/15/2022]
Abstract
Timing is critical to most forms of learning, behavior, and sensory-motor processing. Converging evidence supports the notion that, precisely because of its importance across a wide range of brain functions, timing relies on intrinsic and general properties of neurons and neural circuits; that is, the brain uses its natural cellular and network dynamics to solve a diversity of temporal computations. Many circuits have been shown to encode elapsed time in dynamically changing patterns of neural activity-so-called population clocks. But temporal processing encompasses a wide range of different computations, and just as there are different circuits and mechanisms underlying computations about space, there are a multitude of circuits and mechanisms underlying the ability to tell time and generate temporal patterns.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Joseph J Paton
- Champalimaud Research, Champalimaud Centre for the Unknown, Lisbon, Portugal.
| | - Dean V Buonomano
- Departments of Neurobiology and Psychology and Brain Research Institute, Integrative Center for Learning and Memory, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Remington ED, Egger SW, Narain D, Wang J, Jazayeri M. A Dynamical Systems Perspective on Flexible Motor Timing. Trends Cogn Sci 2018; 22:938-952. [PMID: 30266152 PMCID: PMC6166486 DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.07.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 70] [Impact Index Per Article: 10.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/07/2018] [Revised: 07/10/2018] [Accepted: 07/16/2018] [Indexed: 12/22/2022]
Abstract
A hallmark of higher brain function is the ability to rapidly and flexibly adjust behavioral responses based on internal and external cues. Here, we examine the computational principles that allow decisions and actions to unfold flexibly in time. We adopt a dynamical systems perspective and outline how temporal flexibility in such a system can be achieved through manipulations of inputs and initial conditions. We then review evidence from experiments in nonhuman primates that support this interpretation. Finally, we explore the broader utility and limitations of the dynamical systems perspective as a general framework for addressing open questions related to the temporal control of movements, as well as in the domains of learning and sequence generation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Evan D Remington
- McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA 02139, USA; These authors contributed equally to this work
| | - Seth W Egger
- McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA 02139, USA; These authors contributed equally to this work
| | - Devika Narain
- McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA 02139, USA; Department of Brain & Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA 02139, USA; Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience, Amsterdam, BA 1105, The Netherlands; Erasmus Medical Center, Rotterdam, The Netherlands
| | - Jing Wang
- McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA 02139, USA; Department of Bioengineering, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO 65201, USA
| | - Mehrdad Jazayeri
- McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA 02139, USA; Department of Brain & Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA 02139, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Gyenes B, Brown AEX. Deriving Shape-Based Features for C. elegans Locomotion Using Dimensionality Reduction Methods. Front Behav Neurosci 2016; 10:159. [PMID: 27582697 PMCID: PMC4987360 DOI: 10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00159] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/19/2016] [Accepted: 08/03/2016] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
High-throughput analysis of animal behavior is increasingly common following the advances of recording technology, leading to large high-dimensional data sets. This dimensionality can sometimes be reduced while still retaining relevant information. In the case of the nematode worm Caenorhabditis elegans, more than 90% of the shape variance can be captured using just four principal components. However, it remains unclear if other methods can achieve a more compact representation or contribute further biological insight to worm locomotion. Here we take a data-driven approach to worm shape analysis using independent component analysis (ICA), non-negative matrix factorization (NMF), a cosine series, and jPCA (a dynamic variant of principal component analysis [PCA]) and confirm that the dimensionality of worm shape space is close to four. Projecting worm shapes onto the bases derived using each method gives interpretable features ranging from head movements to tail oscillation. We use these as a comparison method to find differences between the wild type N2 worms and various mutants. For example, we find that the neuropeptide mutant nlp-1(ok1469) has an exaggerated head movement suggesting a mode of action for the previously described increased turning rate. The different bases provide complementary views of worm behavior and we expect that closer examination of the time series of projected amplitudes will lead to new results in the future.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Bertalan Gyenes
- MRC Clinical Sciences CentreLondon, UK; Institute of Clinical Sciences, Faculty of Medicine, Imperial College LondonLondon, UK; Department of Mathematics, Imperial College LondonLondon, UK
| | - André E X Brown
- MRC Clinical Sciences CentreLondon, UK; Institute of Clinical Sciences, Faculty of Medicine, Imperial College LondonLondon, UK
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Kobak D, Brendel W, Constantinidis C, Feierstein CE, Kepecs A, Mainen ZF, Qi XL, Romo R, Uchida N, Machens CK. Demixed principal component analysis of neural population data. eLife 2016; 5. [PMID: 27067378 PMCID: PMC4887222 DOI: 10.7554/elife.10989] [Citation(s) in RCA: 306] [Impact Index Per Article: 34.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/19/2015] [Accepted: 04/07/2016] [Indexed: 01/22/2023] Open
Abstract
Neurons in higher cortical areas, such as the prefrontal cortex, are often tuned to a variety of sensory and motor variables, and are therefore said to display mixed selectivity. This complexity of single neuron responses can obscure what information these areas represent and how it is represented. Here we demonstrate the advantages of a new dimensionality reduction technique, demixed principal component analysis (dPCA), that decomposes population activity into a few components. In addition to systematically capturing the majority of the variance of the data, dPCA also exposes the dependence of the neural representation on task parameters such as stimuli, decisions, or rewards. To illustrate our method we reanalyze population data from four datasets comprising different species, different cortical areas and different experimental tasks. In each case, dPCA provides a concise way of visualizing the data that summarizes the task-dependent features of the population response in a single figure. DOI:http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.10989.001 Many neuroscience experiments today involve using electrodes to record from the brain of an animal, such as a mouse or a monkey, while the animal performs a task. The goal of such experiments is to understand how a particular brain region works. However, modern experimental techniques allow the activity of hundreds of neurons to be recorded simultaneously. Analysing such large amounts of data then becomes a challenge in itself. This is particularly true for brain regions such as the prefrontal cortex that are involved in the cognitive processes that allow an animal to acquire knowledge. Individual neurons in the prefrontal cortex encode many different types of information relevant to a given task. Imagine, for example, that an animal has to select one of two objects to obtain a reward. The same group of prefrontal cortex neurons will encode the object presented to the animal, the animal’s decision and its confidence in that decision. This simultaneous representation of different elements of a task is called a ‘mixed’ representation, and is difficult to analyse. Kobak, Brendel et al. have now developed a data analysis tool that can ‘demix’ neural activity. The tool breaks down the activity of a population of neurons into its individual components. Each of these relates to only a single aspect of the task and is thus easier to interpret. Information about stimuli, for example, is distinguished from information about the animal’s confidence levels. Kobak, Brendel et al. used the demixing tool to reanalyse existing datasets recorded from several different animals, tasks and brain regions. In each case, the tool provided a complete, concise and transparent summary of the data. The next steps will be to apply the analysis tool to new datasets to see how well it performs in practice. At a technical level, the tool could also be extended in a number of different directions to enable it to deal with more complicated experimental designs in future. DOI:http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.10989.002
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dmitry Kobak
- Champalimaud Neuroscience Program, Champalimaud Centre for the Unknown, Lisbon, Portugal
| | - Wieland Brendel
- Champalimaud Neuroscience Program, Champalimaud Centre for the Unknown, Lisbon, Portugal.,École Normale Supérieure, Paris, France.,Centre for Integrative Neuroscience, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
| | | | - Claudia E Feierstein
- Champalimaud Neuroscience Program, Champalimaud Centre for the Unknown, Lisbon, Portugal
| | - Adam Kepecs
- Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory, Cold Spring Harbor, United States
| | - Zachary F Mainen
- Champalimaud Neuroscience Program, Champalimaud Centre for the Unknown, Lisbon, Portugal
| | - Xue-Lian Qi
- Wake Forest University School of Medicine, Winston-Salem, United States
| | - Ranulfo Romo
- Instituto de Fisiología Celular-Neurociencias, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Mexico City, Mexico.,El Colegio Nacional, Mexico City, Mexico
| | | | - Christian K Machens
- Champalimaud Neuroscience Program, Champalimaud Centre for the Unknown, Lisbon, Portugal
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Gouvêa TS, Monteiro T, Motiwala A, Soares S, Machens C, Paton JJ. Striatal dynamics explain duration judgments. eLife 2015; 4. [PMID: 26641377 PMCID: PMC4721960 DOI: 10.7554/elife.11386] [Citation(s) in RCA: 109] [Impact Index Per Article: 10.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/03/2015] [Accepted: 12/07/2015] [Indexed: 11/25/2022] Open
Abstract
The striatum is an input structure of the basal ganglia implicated in several time-dependent functions including reinforcement learning, decision making, and interval timing. To determine whether striatal ensembles drive subjects' judgments of duration, we manipulated and recorded from striatal neurons in rats performing a duration categorization psychophysical task. We found that the dynamics of striatal neurons predicted duration judgments, and that simultaneously recorded ensembles could judge duration as well as the animal. Furthermore, striatal neurons were necessary for duration judgments, as muscimol infusions produced a specific impairment in animals' duration sensitivity. Lastly, we show that time as encoded by striatal populations ran faster or slower when rats judged a duration as longer or shorter, respectively. These results demonstrate that the speed with which striatal population state changes supports the fundamental ability of animals to judge the passage of time. DOI:http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.11386.001 You know someone is a good cook from their rice - grains must be well cooked, but not to the point of being mushy. Despite consistently using the same pot and stove, we, however, will sometimes overcook it. It is as if our inner sense of time itself is variable. What is it about the brain that explains this variability in time estimation and indeed our ability to estimate time in the first place? One issue the brain must confront in order to estimate time is that individual brain cells typically fire in bursts that last for tens of milliseconds. So how does the brain use this short-lived activity to track minutes and hours? One possibility is that individual neurons in a given brain region are programmed to fire at different points in time. The overall firing pattern of a group of neurons will therefore change in a predictable way as time passes. Gouvêa, Monteiro et al. found such predictably changing patterns of activity in the striatum of rats trained to estimate and categorize the duration of time intervals as longer or shorter than 1.5 seconds. Interestingly, when rats mistakenly categorized a short interval as a long one, population activity had travelled farther down its path than it would normally (and vice-versa for long intervals incorrectly categorized as short), suggesting that variability in subjective estimates of the passage of time might arise from variability in the speed of a changing pattern of activity across groups of neurons. As further evidence for the involvement of the striatum, inactivating the structure impaired the rats’ ability to correctly classify even the longest and shortest interval durations. The next challenge is to determine exactly how the striatum generates these time-keeping signals, at which stage variability originates, and how the brain regions that the striatum signals to use them to control an animal’s behavior. DOI:http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.11386.002
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Thiago S Gouvêa
- Champalimaud Neuroscience Programme, Champalimaud Centre for the Unknown, Lisbon, Portugal
| | - Tiago Monteiro
- Champalimaud Neuroscience Programme, Champalimaud Centre for the Unknown, Lisbon, Portugal
| | - Asma Motiwala
- Champalimaud Neuroscience Programme, Champalimaud Centre for the Unknown, Lisbon, Portugal
| | - Sofia Soares
- Champalimaud Neuroscience Programme, Champalimaud Centre for the Unknown, Lisbon, Portugal
| | - Christian Machens
- Champalimaud Neuroscience Programme, Champalimaud Centre for the Unknown, Lisbon, Portugal
| | - Joseph J Paton
- Champalimaud Neuroscience Programme, Champalimaud Centre for the Unknown, Lisbon, Portugal
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Lopes G, Bonacchi N, Frazão J, Neto JP, Atallah BV, Soares S, Moreira L, Matias S, Itskov PM, Correia PA, Medina RE, Calcaterra L, Dreosti E, Paton JJ, Kampff AR. Bonsai: an event-based framework for processing and controlling data streams. Front Neuroinform 2015; 9:7. [PMID: 25904861 PMCID: PMC4389726 DOI: 10.3389/fninf.2015.00007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 299] [Impact Index Per Article: 29.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/01/2014] [Accepted: 03/19/2015] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
The design of modern scientific experiments requires the control and monitoring of many different data streams. However, the serial execution of programming instructions in a computer makes it a challenge to develop software that can deal with the asynchronous, parallel nature of scientific data. Here we present Bonsai, a modular, high-performance, open-source visual programming framework for the acquisition and online processing of data streams. We describe Bonsai's core principles and architecture and demonstrate how it allows for the rapid and flexible prototyping of integrated experimental designs in neuroscience. We specifically highlight some applications that require the combination of many different hardware and software components, including video tracking of behavior, electrophysiology and closed-loop control of stimulation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gonçalo Lopes
- Champalimaud Neuroscience Programme, Champalimaud Centre for the Unknown Lisbon, Portugal
| | - Niccolò Bonacchi
- Champalimaud Neuroscience Programme, Champalimaud Centre for the Unknown Lisbon, Portugal
| | - João Frazão
- Champalimaud Neuroscience Programme, Champalimaud Centre for the Unknown Lisbon, Portugal
| | - Joana P Neto
- Champalimaud Neuroscience Programme, Champalimaud Centre for the Unknown Lisbon, Portugal ; Departamento de Ciência dos Materiais, CENIMAT/I3N and CEMOP/Uninova Lisbon, Portugal
| | - Bassam V Atallah
- Champalimaud Neuroscience Programme, Champalimaud Centre for the Unknown Lisbon, Portugal
| | - Sofia Soares
- Champalimaud Neuroscience Programme, Champalimaud Centre for the Unknown Lisbon, Portugal
| | - Luís Moreira
- Champalimaud Neuroscience Programme, Champalimaud Centre for the Unknown Lisbon, Portugal
| | - Sara Matias
- Champalimaud Neuroscience Programme, Champalimaud Centre for the Unknown Lisbon, Portugal
| | - Pavel M Itskov
- Champalimaud Neuroscience Programme, Champalimaud Centre for the Unknown Lisbon, Portugal
| | - Patrícia A Correia
- Champalimaud Neuroscience Programme, Champalimaud Centre for the Unknown Lisbon, Portugal
| | - Roberto E Medina
- Champalimaud Neuroscience Programme, Champalimaud Centre for the Unknown Lisbon, Portugal
| | - Lorenza Calcaterra
- Champalimaud Neuroscience Programme, Champalimaud Centre for the Unknown Lisbon, Portugal
| | - Elena Dreosti
- Department of Cell and Developmental Biology, University College London London, UK
| | - Joseph J Paton
- Champalimaud Neuroscience Programme, Champalimaud Centre for the Unknown Lisbon, Portugal
| | - Adam R Kampff
- Champalimaud Neuroscience Programme, Champalimaud Centre for the Unknown Lisbon, Portugal
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Maniadakis M, Wittmann M, Droit-Volet S, Choe Y. Toward embodied artificial cognition: TIME is on my side. Front Neurorobot 2014; 8:25. [PMID: 25538614 PMCID: PMC4259165 DOI: 10.3389/fnbot.2014.00025] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/29/2014] [Accepted: 11/19/2014] [Indexed: 12/03/2022] Open
Affiliation(s)
| | - Marc Wittmann
- Institute for Frontier Areas of Psychology and Mental Health Freiburg, Germany
| | - Sylvie Droit-Volet
- Laboratoire de Psychologie Sociale et Cognitive, CNRS (UMR 6024), Department of Psychology, Université Blaise Pascal Clermont-Ferrand, France
| | - Yoonsuck Choe
- Department of Computer Science and Engineering, Texas A&M University Austin, TX, USA
| |
Collapse
|