1
|
Pérez V, Martínez V, Diez-Itza E. Late phonological development in Williams syndrome. Front Psychol 2022; 13:992512. [PMID: 36467192 PMCID: PMC9709339 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.992512] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/12/2022] [Accepted: 11/01/2022] [Indexed: 01/11/2024] Open
Abstract
Williams syndrome is a neurodevelopmental genetic disorder characterized by a unique phenotype, including mild to moderate intellectual disability and an uneven neuropsychological profile of relative strengths and weaknesses. Language structure components (i.e., phonology, morphosyntax, and vocabulary) have been considered an area of specific ability compared to pragmatic language use. However, research on phonological development in Williams syndrome is very scarce, and it suggests atypical patterns. Therefore, the aim of the present study was to explore the profiles of late phonological development in Spanish-speaking children, adolescents, and adults with Williams syndrome, based on the analysis of five classes of processes (Syllable Structure, Substitution, Omission, Assimilation, and Addition) in spontaneous speech. The phonological profiles of seven children (aged 3-8 years), and seven adolescents and young adults (aged 14-25 years) with Williams syndrome were compared with two normative groups of typically developing (TD) children at different stages of late phonological development (aged 3 and 5 years). The frequency of phonological processes in the group of children with Williams syndrome was similar to that of 3-year-old TD children, which suggests that they would be in the first stage of late phonological development (expansion stage). The group of older individuals with Williams syndrome showed a much lower frequency of processes, similar to that of 5-year-old TD children in the last stage of phonological development (resolution stage). However, their phonological processes appeared to be persistent and independent of chronological age. Furthermore, asynchronies in quantitative and qualitative profiles (relative frequency) indicated atypical and complex trajectories in late phonological development, which cannot be described as simply delayed or protracted. Remarkable individual differences were observed, especially in the group of adolescents and adults with Williams syndrome, although the majority of cases conformed to the modal profiles of their groups. A major tendency for Omission, including final consonant deletion, may be considered atypical and specific to Williams syndrome at all ages. The results of the present study raise the need for continued and appropriate phonological assessment and treatment for people with Williams syndrome across the lifespan.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Vanesa Pérez
- LOGIN Research Group, University of Oviedo, Oviedo, Spain
- Escuelas Universitarias Gimbernat, University of Cantabria, Torrelavega, Spain
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
2
|
Thurman AJ, Bullard L, Kelly L, Wong C, Nguyen V, Esbensen AJ, Bekins J, Schworer EK, Fidler DJ, Daunhauer LA, Mervis CB, Pitts CH, Becerra AM, Abbeduto L. Defining Expressive Language Benchmarks for Children with Down Syndrome. Brain Sci 2022; 12:743. [PMID: 35741628 PMCID: PMC9221379 DOI: 10.3390/brainsci12060743] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/14/2022] [Revised: 06/02/2022] [Accepted: 06/04/2022] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Establishing expressive language benchmarks (ELBs) for children with Down syndrome (DS), as developed by Tager-Flusberg et al. for children with autism, is critically needed to inform the development of novel treatments, identify individualized treatment targets, and promote accurate monitoring of progress. In the present study, we assessed ELB assignments in three language domains (phonology, vocabulary, and grammar) for 53 young children with DS (CA range: 2.50-7.99 years) using standardized assessments. The participants were classified into one of four ELB levels (preverbal, first words, word combinations, and sentences) in each language domain. Associations with additional measures of language, chronological age, nonverbal cognition, and verbal short-term memory were considered. Analyses of individual ELB profiles indicated substantial variability across the three language domains, with six different patterns of variation across domains emerging. At the same time, the ELB categories were significantly associated with independent language measures and broader developmental domains. Moreover, ELB changes were observed in a small sample of children with DS reassessed 18-24 months after the initial visit. Results from the present study suggest the procedures outlined by Tager-Flusberg et al. for defining ELBs are a potentially useful tool for describing the language abilities of children with DS.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Angela John Thurman
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of California Davis Health, Sacramento, CA 95817, USA; (L.B.); (L.K.); (C.W.); (V.N.); (L.A.)
- MIND Institute, University of California Davis, Sacramento, CA 95817, USA
| | - Lauren Bullard
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of California Davis Health, Sacramento, CA 95817, USA; (L.B.); (L.K.); (C.W.); (V.N.); (L.A.)
- MIND Institute, University of California Davis, Sacramento, CA 95817, USA
| | - Leona Kelly
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of California Davis Health, Sacramento, CA 95817, USA; (L.B.); (L.K.); (C.W.); (V.N.); (L.A.)
- MIND Institute, University of California Davis, Sacramento, CA 95817, USA
| | - Caitlyn Wong
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of California Davis Health, Sacramento, CA 95817, USA; (L.B.); (L.K.); (C.W.); (V.N.); (L.A.)
- MIND Institute, University of California Davis, Sacramento, CA 95817, USA
| | - Vivian Nguyen
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of California Davis Health, Sacramento, CA 95817, USA; (L.B.); (L.K.); (C.W.); (V.N.); (L.A.)
- MIND Institute, University of California Davis, Sacramento, CA 95817, USA
| | - Anna J. Esbensen
- Division of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, Cincinnati Children’s Hospital Medical Center, Cincinnati, OH 45229, USA; (A.J.E.); (J.B.); (E.K.S.)
- Department of Pediatrics, University of Cincinnati College of Medicine, Cincinnati, OH 45267, USA
| | - Jennifer Bekins
- Division of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, Cincinnati Children’s Hospital Medical Center, Cincinnati, OH 45229, USA; (A.J.E.); (J.B.); (E.K.S.)
| | - Emily K. Schworer
- Division of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, Cincinnati Children’s Hospital Medical Center, Cincinnati, OH 45229, USA; (A.J.E.); (J.B.); (E.K.S.)
| | - Deborah J. Fidler
- Department of Human Development and Family Studies, Colorado State University Fort Collins, Fort Collins, CO 80523, USA; (D.J.F.); (L.A.D.)
| | - Lisa A. Daunhauer
- Department of Human Development and Family Studies, Colorado State University Fort Collins, Fort Collins, CO 80523, USA; (D.J.F.); (L.A.D.)
| | - Carolyn B. Mervis
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Louisville, Louisville, KY 40292, USA; (C.B.M.); (C.H.P.); (A.M.B.)
| | - C. Holley Pitts
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Louisville, Louisville, KY 40292, USA; (C.B.M.); (C.H.P.); (A.M.B.)
| | - Angela M. Becerra
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Louisville, Louisville, KY 40292, USA; (C.B.M.); (C.H.P.); (A.M.B.)
| | - Leonard Abbeduto
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of California Davis Health, Sacramento, CA 95817, USA; (L.B.); (L.K.); (C.W.); (V.N.); (L.A.)
- MIND Institute, University of California Davis, Sacramento, CA 95817, USA
| |
Collapse
|