51
|
Chu FW, vanMarle K, Rouder J, Geary DC. Children’s early understanding of number predicts their later problem-solving sophistication in addition. J Exp Child Psychol 2018; 169:73-92. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2017.12.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/17/2017] [Revised: 10/26/2017] [Accepted: 12/13/2017] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
|
52
|
Xenidou-Dervou I, Van Luit JEH, Kroesbergen EH, Friso-van den Bos I, Jonkman LM, van der Schoot M, van Lieshout ECDM. Cognitive predictors of children's development in mathematics achievement: A latent growth modeling approach. Dev Sci 2018; 21:e12671. [PMID: 29691952 DOI: 10.1111/desc.12671] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/22/2017] [Accepted: 03/06/2018] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Abstract
Research has identified various domain-general and domain-specific cognitive abilities as predictors of children's individual differences in mathematics achievement. However, research into the predictors of children's individual growth rates, namely between-person differences in within-person change in mathematics achievement is scarce. We assessed 334 children's domain-general and mathematics-specific early cognitive abilities and their general mathematics achievement longitudinally across four time-points within the first and second grades of primary school. As expected, a constellation of multiple cognitive abilities contributed to the children's starting level of mathematical success. Specifically, latent growth modeling revealed that WM abilities, IQ, counting skills, nonsymbolic and symbolic approximate arithmetic and comparison skills explained individual differences in the children's initial status on a curriculum-based general mathematics achievement test. Surprisingly, however, only one out of all the assessed cognitive abilities was a unique predictor of the children's individual growth rates in mathematics achievement: their performance in the symbolic approximate addition task. In this task, children were asked to estimate the sum of two large numbers and decide if this estimated sum was smaller or larger compared to a third number. Our findings demonstrate the importance of multiple domain-general and mathematics-specific cognitive skills for identifying children at risk of struggling with mathematics and highlight the significance of early approximate arithmetic skills for the development of one's mathematical success. We argue the need for more research focus on explaining children's individual growth rates in mathematics achievement.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Iro Xenidou-Dervou
- Mathematics Education Centre, Loughborough University, Loughborough, Leicestershire, UK.,Section of Educational Neuroscience, and LEARN! Research Institute, Faculty of Behavioral and Movement Sciences, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| | - Johannes E H Van Luit
- Department of Educational and Learning Sciences, Faculty of Social and Behavioral Sciences, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| | - Evelyn H Kroesbergen
- Department of Educational and Learning Sciences, Faculty of Social and Behavioral Sciences, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands.,Behavioural Science Institute, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | - Ilona Friso-van den Bos
- Department of Educational and Learning Sciences, Faculty of Social and Behavioral Sciences, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| | - Lisa M Jonkman
- Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands
| | - Menno van der Schoot
- Section of Educational Neuroscience, and LEARN! Research Institute, Faculty of Behavioral and Movement Sciences, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| | - Ernest C D M van Lieshout
- Section of Educational Neuroscience, and LEARN! Research Institute, Faculty of Behavioral and Movement Sciences, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
53
|
Clayton S, Inglis M, Gilmore C. Developmental differences in approaches to nonsymbolic comparison tasks. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2018; 72:436-445. [PMID: 29419356 DOI: 10.1177/1747021818755296] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
Nonsymbolic comparison tasks are widely used to measure children's and adults' approximate number system (ANS) acuity. Recent evidence has demonstrated that task performance can be influenced by changes to the visual characteristics of the stimuli, leading some researchers to suggest it is unlikely that an ANS exists that can extract number information independently of the visual characteristics of the arrays. Here, we analysed 124 children's and 120 adults' dot comparison accuracy scores from three separate studies to investigate individual and developmental differences in how numerical and visual information contribute to nonsymbolic numerosity judgements. We found that, in contrast to adults, the majority of children did not use numerical information over and above visual cue information to compare quantities. This finding was consistent across different studies. The results have implications for research on the relationship between dot comparison performance and formal mathematics achievement. Specifically, if most children's performance on dot comparison tasks can be accounted for without the involvement of numerical information, it seems unlikely that observed correlations with mathematics achievement stem from ANS acuity alone.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sarah Clayton
- 1 Department of Health Sciences, University of Leicester, Leicester, UK
| | - Matthew Inglis
- 2 Mathematics Education Centre, Loughborough University, Loughborough, UK
| | - Camilla Gilmore
- 2 Mathematics Education Centre, Loughborough University, Loughborough, UK
| |
Collapse
|
54
|
Mueller SM, Schiebener J, Delazer M, Brand M. Risk approximation in decision making: approximative numeric abilities predict advantageous decisions under objective risk. Cogn Process 2018; 19:297-315. [PMID: 29357078 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-018-0854-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/25/2017] [Accepted: 01/11/2018] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
Many decision situations in everyday life involve mathematical considerations. In decisions under objective risk, i.e., when explicit numeric information is available, executive functions and abilities to handle exact numbers and ratios are predictors of objectively advantageous choices. Although still debated, exact numeric abilities, e.g., normative calculation skills, are assumed to be related to approximate number processing skills. The current study investigates the effects of approximative numeric abilities on decision making under objective risk. Participants (N = 153) performed a paradigm measuring number-comparison, quantity-estimation, risk-estimation, and decision-making skills on the basis of rapid dot comparisons. Additionally, a risky decision-making task with exact numeric information was administered, as well as tasks measuring executive functions and exact numeric abilities, e.g., mental calculation and ratio processing skills, were conducted. Approximative numeric abilities significantly predicted advantageous decision making, even beyond the effects of executive functions and exact numeric skills. Especially being able to make accurate risk estimations seemed to contribute to superior choices. We recommend approximation skills and approximate number processing to be subject of future investigations on decision making under risk.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Silke M Mueller
- General Psychology: Cognition and Center for Behavioral Addiction Research (CeBAR), University of Duisburg-Essen, Forsthausweg 2, 47057, Duisburg, Germany
| | - Johannes Schiebener
- General Psychology: Cognition and Center for Behavioral Addiction Research (CeBAR), University of Duisburg-Essen, Forsthausweg 2, 47057, Duisburg, Germany
| | - Margarete Delazer
- Clinical Department of Neurology, Innsbruck Medical University, Innsbruck, Austria
| | - Matthias Brand
- General Psychology: Cognition and Center for Behavioral Addiction Research (CeBAR), University of Duisburg-Essen, Forsthausweg 2, 47057, Duisburg, Germany. .,Erwin L. Hahn Institute for Magnetic Resonance Imaging, Essen, Germany.
| |
Collapse
|
55
|
Ruggeri A, Vagharchakian L, Xu F. Icon arrays help younger children's proportional reasoning. BRITISH JOURNAL OF DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2018; 36:313-333. [DOI: 10.1111/bjdp.12233] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/02/2016] [Revised: 12/09/2017] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Azzurra Ruggeri
- MPRG iSearch
- Information Search, Ecological and Active Learning Research With Children; Max Planck Institute for Human Development; Berlin Germany
- School of Education; Technical University Munich; Germany
| | - Laurianne Vagharchakian
- Center for Adaptive Behavior and Cognition; Max Planck Institute for Human Development; Berlin Germany
| | - Fei Xu
- Department of Psychology; University of California, Berkeley; California USA
| |
Collapse
|
56
|
Dillon MR, Kannan H, Dean JT, Spelke ES, Duflo E. Cognitive science in the field: A preschool intervention durably enhances intuitive but not formal mathematics. Science 2018; 357:47-55. [PMID: 28684518 DOI: 10.1126/science.aal4724] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/03/2016] [Accepted: 05/30/2017] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
Many poor children are underprepared for demanding primary school curricula. Research in cognitive science suggests that school achievement could be improved by preschool pedagogy in which numerate adults engage children's spontaneous, nonsymbolic mathematical concepts. To test this suggestion, we designed and evaluated a game-based preschool curriculum intended to exercise children's emerging skills in number and geometry. In a randomized field experiment with 1540 children (average age 4.9 years) in 214 Indian preschools, 4 months of math game play yielded marked and enduring improvement on the exercised intuitive abilities, relative to no-treatment and active control conditions. Math-trained children also showed immediate gains on symbolic mathematical skills but displayed no advantage in subsequent learning of the language and concepts of school mathematics.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Moira R Dillon
- Department of Psychology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA.
| | - Harini Kannan
- Abdul Latif Jameel Poverty Action Lab South Asia, New Delhi, India
| | - Joshua T Dean
- Department of Economics, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, USA
| | | | - Esther Duflo
- Abdul Latif Jameel Poverty Action Lab South Asia, New Delhi, India. .,Department of Economics, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
57
|
Abstract
In this article, I argue that a growing body of evidence shows that concepts are amodal and I provide a novel interpretation of the body of evidence that was taken to support neo-empiricist theories of concepts: the offloading hypothesis in the 1990s and 2000s.
Collapse
|
58
|
Children’s quantitative Bayesian inferences from natural frequencies and number of chances. Cognition 2017; 168:164-175. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2017.06.028] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/18/2017] [Revised: 06/16/2017] [Accepted: 06/27/2017] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
|
59
|
Tosto MG, Petrill SA, Malykh S, Malki K, Haworth CMA, Mazzocco MMM, Thompson L, Opfer J, Bogdanova OY, Kovas Y. Number sense and mathematics: Which, when and how? Dev Psychol 2017; 53:1924-1939. [PMID: 28758784 PMCID: PMC5611774 DOI: 10.1037/dev0000331] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/10/2016] [Revised: 01/16/2017] [Accepted: 03/07/2017] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
Individual differences in number sense correlate with mathematical ability and performance, although the presence and strength of this relationship differs across studies. Inconsistencies in the literature may stem from heterogeneity of number sense and mathematical ability constructs. Sample characteristics may also play a role as changes in the relationship between number sense and mathematics may differ across development and cultural contexts. In this study, 4,984 16-year-old students were assessed on estimation ability, one aspect of number sense. Estimation was measured using 2 different tasks: number line and dot-comparison. Using cognitive and achievement data previously collected from these students at ages 7, 9, 10, 12, and 14, the study explored for which of the measures and when in development these links are observed, and how strong these links are and how much these links are moderated by other cognitive abilities. The 2 number sense measures correlated modestly with each other (r = .22), but moderately with mathematics at age 16. Both measures were also associated with earlier mathematics; but this association was uneven across development and was moderated by other cognitive abilities. (PsycINFO Database Record
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | | | - Karim Malki
- King's College London at the Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience (IOPPN)
| | | | | | - Lee Thompson
- Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University
| | - John Opfer
- Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University
| | | | - Yulia Kovas
- Department of Psychology, Tomsk State University
| |
Collapse
|
60
|
Lukowski SL, Rosenberg-Lee M, Thompson LA, Hart SA, Willcutt EG, Olson RK, Petrill SA, Pennington BF. Approximate Number Sense Shares Etiological Overlap with Mathematics and General Cognitive Ability. INTELLIGENCE 2017; 65:67-74. [PMID: 29867283 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2017.08.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
Abstract
Approximate number sense (ANS), the ability to rapidly and accurately compare quantities presented non-symbolically, has been proposed as a precursor to mathematics skills. Earlier work reported low heritability of approximate number sense, which was interpreted as evidence that approximate number sense acts as a fitness trait. However, viewing ANS as a fitness trait is discordant with findings suggesting that individual differences in approximate number sense acuity correlate with mathematical performance, a trait with moderate genetic effects. Importantly, the shared etiology of approximate number sense, mathematics, and general cognitive ability has remained unexamined. Thus, the etiology of approximate number sense and its overlap with math and general cognitive ability was assessed in the current study with two independent twin samples (N = 451 pairs). Results suggested that ANS acuity had moderate but significant additive genetic influences. ANS also had overlap with generalist genetic mechanisms accounting for variance and covariance in mathematics and general cognitive ability. Furthermore, ANS may have genetic factors unique to covariance with mathematics beyond overlap with general cognitive ability. Evidence across both samples was consistent with the proposal that the etiology of approximate number sense functions similar to that of mathematics and general cognitive skills.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sarah L Lukowski
- Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH, USA, 43210
| | - Miriam Rosenberg-Lee
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford, CA, USA, 94305.,Department of Psychology, Rutgers University, Newark, NY, USA, 07102
| | - Lee A Thompson
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland, OH, USA, 44106
| | - Sara A Hart
- Department of Psychology and the Florida Center for Reading Research, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL, USA, 32310
| | - Erik G Willcutt
- Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of Colorado, Boulder, CO, USA, 80309
| | - Richard K Olson
- Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of Colorado, Boulder, CO, USA, 80309
| | - Stephen A Petrill
- Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH, USA, 43210
| | | |
Collapse
|
61
|
Cui J, Zhang Y, Cheng D, Li D, Zhou X. Visual Form Perception Can Be a Cognitive Correlate of Lower Level Math Categories for Teenagers. Front Psychol 2017; 8:1336. [PMID: 28824513 PMCID: PMC5543093 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01336] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/10/2017] [Accepted: 07/20/2017] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Numerous studies have assessed the cognitive correlates of performance in mathematics, but little research has been conducted to systematically examine the relations between visual perception as the starting point of visuospatial processing and typical mathematical performance. In the current study, we recruited 223 seventh graders to perform a visual form perception task (figure matching), numerosity comparison, digit comparison, exact computation, approximate computation, and curriculum-based mathematical achievement tests. Results showed that, after controlling for gender, age, and five general cognitive processes (choice reaction time, visual tracing, mental rotation, spatial working memory, and non-verbal matrices reasoning), visual form perception had unique contributions to numerosity comparison, digit comparison, and exact computation, but had no significant relation with approximate computation or curriculum-based mathematical achievement. These results suggest that visual form perception is an important independent cognitive correlate of lower level math categories, including the approximate number system, digit comparison, and exact computation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jiaxin Cui
- State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning and IDG/McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Beijing Normal UniversityBeijing, China
| | - Yiyun Zhang
- School of Psychology, Liaoning Normal UniversityDalian, China
| | - Dazhi Cheng
- Department of Pediatric Neurology, Capital Institute of PediatricsBeijing, China
| | - Dawei Li
- Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, Duke UniversityDurham, NC, United States
| | - Xinlin Zhou
- State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning and IDG/McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Beijing Normal UniversityBeijing, China
- Advanced Innovation Center for Future Education, Beijing Normal UniversityBeijing, China
| |
Collapse
|
62
|
Miravete S, Tricot A, Kalyuga S, Amadieu F. Configured-groups hypothesis: fast comparison of exact large quantities without counting. Cogn Process 2017; 18:447-459. [PMID: 28717829 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-017-0826-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/27/2016] [Accepted: 06/28/2017] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
Our innate number sense cannot distinguish between two large exact numbers of objects (e.g., 45 dots vs 46). Configured groups (e.g., 10 blocks, 20 frames) are traditionally used in schools to represent large numbers. Previous studies suggest that these external representations make it easier to use symbolic strategies such as counting ten by ten, enabling humans to differentiate exactly two large numbers. The main hypothesis of this work is that configured groups also allow for a differentiation of large exact numbers, even when symbolic strategies become ineffective. In experiment 1, the children from grade 3 were asked to compare two large collections of objects for 5 s. When the objects were organized in configured groups, the success rate was over .90. Without this configured grouping, the children were unable to make a successful comparison. Experiments 2 and 3 controlled for a strategy based on non-numerical parameters (areas delimited by dots or the sum areas of dots, etc.) or use symbolic strategies. These results suggest that configured grouping enables humans to distinguish between two large exact numbers of objects, even when innate number sense and symbolic strategies are ineffective. These results are consistent with what we call "the configured group hypothesis": configured groups play a fundamental role in the acquisition of exact numerical abilities.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - André Tricot
- CLLE Institute, University of Toulouse 2, Toulouse, France
| | - Slava Kalyuga
- School of Education, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia
| | - Franck Amadieu
- CLLE Institute, University of Toulouse 2, Toulouse, France
| |
Collapse
|
63
|
Li Y, Zhang M, Chen Y, Zhu X, Deng Z, Yan S. Children's Non-symbolic, Symbolic Addition and Their Mapping Capacity at 4-7 Years Old. Front Psychol 2017; 8:1203. [PMID: 28769845 PMCID: PMC5511823 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01203] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/02/2017] [Accepted: 06/30/2017] [Indexed: 01/29/2023] Open
Abstract
The study aimed to examine the developmental trajectories of non-symbolic and symbolic addition capacities in children and the mapping ability between these two. We assessed 106 4- to 7-year-old children and found that 4-year-olds were able to do non-symbolic addition but not symbolic addition. Five-year-olds and older were able to do symbolic addition and their performance in symbolic addition exceeded non-symbolic addition in grade 1 (approximate age 7). These results suggested non-symbolic addition ability emerges earlier and is less affected by formal mathematical education than symbolic addition. Meanwhile, we tested children's bi-directional mapping ability using a novel task and found that children were able to map between symbolic and non-symbolic representations of number at age 5. Their ability in mapping non-symbolic to symbolic number became more proficient in grade 1 (approximate age 7). This suggests children at age 7 have developed a relatively mature symbolic representation system.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yanjun Li
- Institute of Developmental Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, Bejing Normal UniversityBeijing, China
| | - Meng Zhang
- Department of Psychology, Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey, New BrunswickNJ, United States
| | - Yinghe Chen
- Institute of Developmental Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, Bejing Normal UniversityBeijing, China
| | - Xiaoshuang Zhu
- Institute of Developmental Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, Bejing Normal UniversityBeijing, China
| | - Zhijun Deng
- Institute of Developmental Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, Bejing Normal UniversityBeijing, China
| | - Shijia Yan
- Institute of Information Control, China Aerospace Academy of Systems Science and EngineeringBeijing, China
| |
Collapse
|
64
|
Attout L, Noël MP, Vossius L, Rousselle L. Evidence of the impact of visuo-spatial processing on magnitude representation in 22q11.2 microdeletion syndrome. Neuropsychologia 2017; 99:296-305. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.03.023] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/30/2016] [Revised: 03/13/2017] [Accepted: 03/22/2017] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
|
65
|
Collins E, Park J, Behrmann M. Numerosity representation is encoded in human subcortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2017; 114:E2806-E2815. [PMID: 28320968 PMCID: PMC5389276 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1613982114] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/29/2023] Open
Abstract
Certain numerical abilities appear to be relatively ubiquitous in the animal kingdom, including the ability to recognize and differentiate relative quantities. This skill is present in human adults and children, as well as in nonhuman primates and, perhaps surprisingly, is also demonstrated by lower species such as mosquitofish and spiders, despite the absence of cortical computation available to primates. This ubiquity of numerical competence suggests that representations that connect to numerical tasks are likely subserved by evolutionarily conserved regions of the nervous system. Here, we test the hypothesis that the evaluation of relative numerical quantities is subserved by lower-order brain structures in humans. Using a monocular/dichoptic paradigm, across four experiments, we show that the discrimination of displays, consisting of both large (5-80) and small (1-4) numbers of dots, is facilitated in the monocular, subcortical portions of the visual system. This is only the case, however, when observers evaluate larger ratios of 3:1 or 4:1, but not smaller ratios, closer to 1:1. This profile of competence matches closely the skill with which newborn infants and other species can discriminate numerical quantity. These findings suggest conservation of ontogenetically and phylogenetically lower-order systems in adults' numerical abilities. The involvement of subcortical structures in representing numerical quantities provokes a reconsideration of current theories of the neural basis of numerical cognition, inasmuch as it bolsters the cross-species continuity of the biological system for numerical abilities.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elliot Collins
- Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh PA 15213-3890
- Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh PA 15213-3890
- School of Medicine, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA 15261
| | - Joonkoo Park
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Massachusetts, Amherst, MA 01003
| | - Marlene Behrmann
- Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh PA 15213-3890;
- Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh PA 15213-3890
| |
Collapse
|
66
|
McCrink K, Shafto P, Barth H. The relationship between non-symbolic multiplication and division in childhood. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2017; 70:686-702. [PMID: 26880261 PMCID: PMC5228418 DOI: 10.1080/17470218.2016.1151060] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
Abstract
Children without formal education in addition and subtraction are able to perform multi-step operations over an approximate number of objects. Further, their performance improves when solving approximate (but not exact) addition and subtraction problems that allow for inversion as a shortcut (e.g., a + b - b = a). The current study examines children's ability to perform multi-step operations, and the potential for an inversion benefit, for the operations of approximate, non-symbolic multiplication and division. Children were trained to compute a multiplication and division scaling factor (*2 or /2, *4 or /4), and were then tested on problems that combined two of these factors in a way that either allowed for an inversion shortcut (e.g., 8*4/4) or did not (e.g., 8*4/2). Children's performance was significantly better than chance for all scaling factors during training, and they successfully computed the outcomes of the multi-step testing problems. They did not exhibit a performance benefit for problems with the a*b/b structure, suggesting that they did not draw upon inversion reasoning as a logical shortcut to help them solve the multi-step test problems.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Koleen McCrink
- a Psychology Department , Barnard College, Columbia University , New York , NY , USA
| | - Patrick Shafto
- b Department of Mathematics and Computer Science , Rutgers University , Newark , NJ , USA
| | - Hilary Barth
- c Psychology Department , Wesleyan University , Middletown , CT , USA
| |
Collapse
|
67
|
Szkudlarek E, Brannon EM. Does the approximate number system serve as a foundation for symbolic mathematics? LANGUAGE LEARNING AND DEVELOPMENT : THE OFFICIAL JOURNAL OF THE SOCIETY FOR LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT 2017; 13:171-190. [PMID: 28344520 PMCID: PMC5362122 DOI: 10.1080/15475441.2016.1263573] [Citation(s) in RCA: 37] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/06/2023]
|
68
|
Kibbe MM, Feigenson L. A dissociation between small and large numbers in young children's ability to "solve for x" in non-symbolic math problems. Cognition 2017; 160:82-90. [PMID: 28068528 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.12.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/12/2016] [Revised: 12/02/2016] [Accepted: 12/20/2016] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
Abstract
Solving for an unknown addend in problems like 5+x=17 is challenging for children. Yet, previous work (Kibbe & Feigenson, 2015) found that even before formal math education, young children, aged 4- to 6-years, succeeded when problems were presented using non-symbolic collections of objects rather than symbolic digits. This reveals that the Approximate Number System (ANS) can support pre-algebraic intuitions. Here, we asked whether children also could intuitively "solve for x" when problems contained arrays of four or fewer objects that encouraged representations of individual objects instead of ANS representations. In Experiment 1, we first confirmed that children could solve for an unknown addend with larger quantities, using the ANS. Next, in Experiment 2a, we presented addend-unknown problems containing arrays of four or fewer objects (e.g., 1+x=3). This time, despite the identical task conditions, children were unable to solve for the unknown addend. In Experiment 2b, we replicated this failure with a new sample of children. Finally, in Experiment 3, we confirmed that children's failures in Experiments 2a and b were not due to lack of motivation to compute with small arrays, or to the discriminability of the quantities used: children succeeded at solving for an unknown sum with arrays containing four or fewer objects. Together, these results suggest that children's ability to intuitively solve for an unknown addend may be limited to problems that can be represented using the ANS.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Melissa M Kibbe
- Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, Boston University, 64 Cummington Mall, Boston, MA 02215, United States.
| | - Lisa Feigenson
- Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, Johns Hopkins University, 3400 N Charles St, Baltimore, MD 21218, United States.
| |
Collapse
|
69
|
de Hevia MD, Addabbo M, Nava E, Croci E, Girelli L, Macchi Cassia V. Infants’ detection of increasing numerical order comes before detection of decreasing number. Cognition 2017; 158:177-188. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.10.022] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/27/2016] [Revised: 10/27/2016] [Accepted: 10/29/2016] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
|
70
|
Gilmore C, Cragg L, Hogan G, Inglis M. Congruency effects in dot comparison tasks: convex hull is more important than dot area. JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2016; 28:923-931. [PMID: 28163886 PMCID: PMC5213839 DOI: 10.1080/20445911.2016.1221828] [Citation(s) in RCA: 44] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/30/2015] [Accepted: 08/04/2016] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
The dot comparison task, in which participants select the more numerous of two dot arrays, has become the predominant method of assessing Approximate Number System (ANS) acuity. Creation of the dot arrays requires the manipulation of visual characteristics, such as dot size and convex hull. For the task to provide a valid measure of ANS acuity, participants must ignore these characteristics and respond on the basis of number. Here, we report two experiments that explore the influence of dot area and convex hull on participants' accuracy on dot comparison tasks. We found that individuals' ability to ignore dot area information increases with age and display time. However, the influence of convex hull information remains stable across development and with additional time. This suggests that convex hull information is more difficult to inhibit when making judgements about numerosity and therefore it is crucial to control this when creating dot comparison tasks.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Camilla Gilmore
- Mathematics Education Centre, Loughborough University , Loughborough , UK
| | - Lucy Cragg
- School of Psychology, University of Nottingham , Nottingham , UK
| | - Grace Hogan
- Department of Mathematical Sciences, Loughborough University , Loughborough , UK
| | - Matthew Inglis
- Mathematics Education Centre, Loughborough University , Loughborough , UK
| |
Collapse
|
71
|
Park J, Brannon EM. How to interpret cognitive training studies: A reply to Lindskog & Winman. Cognition 2016; 150:247-51. [PMID: 26972469 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.02.012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/29/2016] [Accepted: 02/14/2016] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
In our previous studies, we demonstrated that repeated training on an approximate arithmetic task selectively improves symbolic arithmetic performance (Park & Brannon, 2013, 2014). We proposed that mental manipulation of quantity is the common cognitive component between approximate arithmetic and symbolic arithmetic, driving the causal relationship between the two. In a commentary to our work, Lindskog and Winman argue that there is no evidence of performance improvement during approximate arithmetic training and that this challenges the proposed causal relationship between approximate arithmetic and symbolic arithmetic. Here, we argue that causality in cognitive training experiments is interpreted from the selectivity of transfer effects and does not hinge upon improved performance in the training task. This is because changes in the unobservable cognitive elements underlying the transfer effect may not be observable from performance measures in the training task. We also question the validity of Lindskog and Winman's simulation approach for testing for a training effect, given that simulations require a valid and sufficient model of a decision process, which is often difficult to achieve. Finally we provide an empirical approach to testing the training effects in adaptive training. Our analysis reveals new evidence that approximate arithmetic performance improved over the course of training in Park and Brannon (2014). We maintain that our data supports the conclusion that approximate arithmetic training leads to improvement in symbolic arithmetic driven by the common cognitive component of mental quantity manipulation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Joonkoo Park
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Massachusetts, United States; Commonwealth Honors College, University of Massachusetts, United States.
| | | |
Collapse
|
72
|
Abstract
Human infants appear to be capable of the rudimentary mathematical operations of addition, subtraction, and ordering. To determine whether infants are capable of extracting ratios, we presented 6-month-old infants with multiple examples of a single ratio. After repeated presentations of this ratio, the infants were presented with new examples of a new ratio, as well as new examples of the previously habituated ratio. Infants were able to successfully discriminate two ratios that differed by a factor of 2, but failed to detect the difference between two numerical ratios that differed by a factor of 1.5. We conclude that infants can extract a common ratio across test scenes and use this information while examining new displays. The results support an approximate magnitude-estimation system, which has also been found in animals and human adults.
Collapse
|
73
|
Lindskog M, Winman A. No evidence of learning in non-symbolic numerical tasks – A comment on. Cognition 2016; 150:243-7. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.01.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/01/2014] [Revised: 12/27/2015] [Accepted: 01/07/2016] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
|
74
|
|
75
|
vanMarle K, Mou Y, Seok JH. Analog Magnitudes Support Large Number Ordinal Judgments in Infancy. Perception 2015; 45:32-43. [PMID: 26562862 DOI: 10.1177/0301006615602630] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
Few studies have explored the source of infants' ordinal knowledge, and those that have are equivocal regarding the underlying representational system. The present study sought clear evidence that the approximate number system, which underlies children's cardinal knowledge, may also support ordinal knowledge in infancy; 10 - to 12-month-old infants' were tested with large sets (>3) in an ordinal choice task in which they were asked to choose between two hidden sets of food items. The difficulty of the comparison varied as a function of the ratio between the sets. Infants reliably chose the greater quantity when the sets differed by a 2:3 ratio (4v6 and 6v9), but not when they differed by a 3:4 ratio (6v8) or a 7:8 ratio (7v8). This discrimination function is consistent with previous studies testing the precision of number and time representations in infants of roughly this same age, thus providing evidence that the approximate number system can support ordinal judgments in infancy. The findings are discussed in light of recent proposals that different mechanisms underlie infants' reasoning about small and large numbers.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Yi Mou
- University of Missouri, Columbia, MO, USA
| | - Jin H Seok
- University of Missouri, Columbia, MO, USA
| |
Collapse
|
76
|
Being Sticker Rich: Numerical Context Influences Children's Sharing Behavior. PLoS One 2015; 10:e0138928. [PMID: 26535900 PMCID: PMC4633172 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0138928] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/01/2015] [Accepted: 09/05/2015] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Young children spontaneously share resources with anonymous recipients, but little is known about the specific circumstances that promote or hinder these prosocial tendencies. Children (ages 3-11) received a small (12) or large (30) number of stickers, and were then given the opportunity to share their windfall with either one or multiple anonymous recipients (Dictator Game). Whether a child chose to share or not varied as a function of age, but was uninfluenced by numerical context. Moreover, children's giving was consistent with a proportion-based account, such that children typically donated a similar proportion (but different absolute number) of the resources given to them, regardless of whether they originally received a small or large windfall. The proportion of resources donated, however, did vary based on the number of recipients with whom they were allowed to share, such that on average, children shared more when there were more recipients available, particularly when they had more resources, suggesting they take others into consideration when making prosocial decisions. Finally, results indicated that a child's gender also predicted sharing behavior, with males generally sharing more resources than females. Together, findings suggest that the numerical contexts under which children are asked to share, as well as the quantity of resources that they have to share, may interact to promote (or hinder) altruistic behaviors throughout childhood.
Collapse
|
77
|
Clayton S, Gilmore C, Inglis M. Dot comparison stimuli are not all alike: the effect of different visual controls on ANS measurement. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2015; 161:177-84. [PMID: 26408864 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2015.09.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 80] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/29/2014] [Revised: 06/10/2015] [Accepted: 09/08/2015] [Indexed: 01/29/2023] Open
Abstract
The most common method of indexing Approximate Number System (ANS) acuity is to use a nonsymbolic dot comparison task. Currently there is no standard protocol for creating the dot array stimuli and it is unclear whether tasks that control for different visual cues, such as cumulative surface area and convex hull size, measure the same cognitive constructs. Here we investigated how the accuracy and reliability of magnitude judgements is influenced by visual controls through a comparison of performance on dot comparison trials created with two standard methods: the Panamath program and Gebuis & Reynvoet's script. Fifty-one adult participants completed blocks of trials employing images constructed using the two protocols twice to obtain a measure of immediate test-retest reliability. We found no significant correlation between participants' accuracy scores on trials created with the two protocols, suggesting that tasks employing these protocols may measure different cognitive constructs. Additionally, there were significant differences in the test-retest reliabilities for trials created with each protocol. Finally, strong congruency effects for convex hull size were found for both sets of protocol trials, which provides some clarification for conflicting results in the literature.
Collapse
|
78
|
An Investigation into 2, 4 Year Old Children’s Nonsymbolic Arithmetic Ability According to Task Difficulty. ADONGHAKOEJI 2015. [DOI: 10.5723/kjcs.2015.36.4.229] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
|
79
|
Xenidou-Dervou I, van der Schoot M, van Lieshout ECDM. Working memory and number line representations in single-digit addition: Approximate versus exact, nonsymbolic versus symbolic. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2015; 68:1148-67. [DOI: 10.1080/17470218.2014.977303] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
Abstract
How do kindergarteners solve different single-digit addition problem formats? We administered problems that differed solely on the basis of two dimensions: response type (approximate or exact), and stimulus type (nonsymbolic, i.e., dots, or symbolic, i.e., Arabic numbers). We examined how performance differs across these dimensions, and which cognitive mechanism (mental model, transcoding, or phonological storage) underlies performance in each problem format with respect to working memory (WM) resources and mental number line representations. As expected, nonsymbolic problem formats were easier than symbolic ones. The visuospatial sketchpad was the primary predictor of nonsymbolic addition. Symbolic problem formats were harder because they either required the storage and manipulation of quantitative symbols phonologically or taxed more WM resources than their nonsymbolic counterparts. In symbolic addition, WM and mental number line results showed that when an approximate response was needed, children transcoded the information to the nonsymbolic code. When an exact response was needed, however, they phonologically stored numerical information in the symbolic code. Lastly, we found that more accurate symbolic mental number line representations were related to better performance in exact addition problem formats, not the approximate ones. This study extends our understanding of the cognitive processes underlying children's simple addition skills.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Iro Xenidou-Dervou
- Department of Educational Neuroscience and LEARN! Research Institute for Learning and Education, Faculty of Psychology and Education, VU University Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
| | - Menno van der Schoot
- Department of Educational Neuroscience and LEARN! Research Institute for Learning and Education, Faculty of Psychology and Education, VU University Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
| | - Ernest C. D. M. van Lieshout
- Department of Educational Neuroscience and LEARN! Research Institute for Learning and Education, Faculty of Psychology and Education, VU University Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
80
|
Rubinsten O. Link between cognitive neuroscience and education: the case of clinical assessment of developmental dyscalculia. Front Hum Neurosci 2015; 9:304. [PMID: 26074805 PMCID: PMC4446534 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00304] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/17/2014] [Accepted: 05/12/2015] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
In recent years, cognitive neuroscience research has identified several biological and cognitive features of number processing deficits that may now make it possible to diagnose mental or educational impairments in arithmetic, even earlier and more precisely than is possible using traditional assessment tools. We provide two sets of recommendations for improving cognitive assessment tools, using the important case of mathematics as an example. (1) neurocognitive tests would benefit substantially from incorporating assessments (based on findings from cognitive neuroscience) that entail systematic manipulation of fundamental aspects of number processing. Tests that focus on evaluating networks of core neurocognitive deficits have considerable potential to lead to more precise diagnosis and to provide the basis for designing specific intervention programs tailored to the deficits exhibited by the individual child. (2) implicit knowledge, derived from inspection of variables that are irrelevant to the task at hand, can also provide a useful assessment tool. Implicit knowledge is powerful and plays an important role in human development, especially in cases of psychiatric or neurological deficiencies (such as math learning disabilities or math anxiety).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Orly Rubinsten
- Department of Learning Disabilities, Edmond J. Safra Brain Research Center for the Study of Learning Disabilities, University of HaifaHaifa, Israel
| |
Collapse
|
81
|
Xenidou-Dervou I, Gilmore C, van der Schoot M, van Lieshout ECDM. The developmental onset of symbolic approximation: beyond nonsymbolic representations, the language of numbers matters. Front Psychol 2015; 6:487. [PMID: 25972822 PMCID: PMC4413728 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00487] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/30/2014] [Accepted: 04/03/2015] [Indexed: 01/29/2023] Open
Abstract
Symbolic (i.e., with Arabic numerals) approximate arithmetic with large numerosities is an important predictor of mathematics. It was previously evidenced to onset before formal schooling at the kindergarten age (Gilmore et al., 2007) and was assumed to map onto pre-existing nonsymbolic (i.e., abstract magnitudes) representations. With a longitudinal study (Experiment 1), we show, for the first time, that nonsymbolic and symbolic arithmetic demonstrate different developmental trajectories. In contrast to Gilmore et al.’s (2007) findings, Experiment 1 showed that symbolic arithmetic onsets in grade 1, with the start of formal schooling, not earlier. Gilmore et al. (2007) had examined English-speaking children, whereas we assessed a large Dutch-speaking sample. The Dutch language for numbers can be cognitively more demanding, for example, due to the inversion property in numbers above 20. Thus, for instance, the number 48 is named in Dutch “achtenveertig” (eight and forty) instead of “forty eight.” To examine the effect of the language of numbers, we conducted a cross-cultural study with English- and Dutch-speaking children that had similar SES and math achievement skills (Experiment 2). Results demonstrated that Dutch-speaking kindergarteners lagged behind English-speaking children in symbolic arithmetic, not nonsymbolic and demonstrated a working memory overload in symbolic arithmetic, not nonsymbolic. Also, we show for the first time that the ability to name two-digit numbers highly correlates with symbolic approximate arithmetic not nonsymbolic. Our experiments empirically demonstrate that the symbolic number system is modulated more by development and education than the nonsymbolic system. Also, in contrast to the nonsymbolic system, the symbolic system is modulated by language.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Iro Xenidou-Dervou
- Department of Educational Neuroscience and LEARN! Research Institute for Learning and Education, Faculty of Psychology and Education, VU University Amsterdam Amsterdam, Netherlands
| | - Camilla Gilmore
- Mathematics Education Centre, Loughborough University Loughborough, UK
| | - Menno van der Schoot
- Department of Educational Neuroscience and LEARN! Research Institute for Learning and Education, Faculty of Psychology and Education, VU University Amsterdam Amsterdam, Netherlands
| | - Ernest C D M van Lieshout
- Department of Educational Neuroscience and LEARN! Research Institute for Learning and Education, Faculty of Psychology and Education, VU University Amsterdam Amsterdam, Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
82
|
Johnson-Laird P, Khemlani SS, Goodwin GP. Logic, probability, and human reasoning. Trends Cogn Sci 2015; 19:201-14. [DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.02.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 58] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/11/2014] [Revised: 02/03/2015] [Accepted: 02/09/2015] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
|
83
|
Assessing the Approximate Number System: no relation between numerical comparison and estimation tasks. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2015; 80:248-58. [PMID: 25742706 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-015-0657-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/27/2014] [Accepted: 02/28/2015] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
Whether our general numerical skills and the mathematical knowledge that we acquire at school are entwined is a debated issue, which many researchers are still striving to investigate. The findings reported in the literature are actually inconsistent; some studies emphasized the existence of a relationship between the acuity of the Approximate Number System (ANS) and arithmetic competence, while some others did not observe any significant correlation. One potential explanation of the discrepancy might stem from the evaluation of the ANS itself. In the present study, we correlated two measures used to index ANS acuity with arithmetic performance. These measures were the Weber fraction (w), computed from a numerical comparison task and the coefficient of variation (CV), computed from a numerical estimation task. Arithmetic performance correlated with estimation CV but not with comparison w. We further investigated the meaning of this result by taking the relationship between w and CV into account. We expected a tight relation as both these measures are believed to assess ANS acuity. Crucially, however, w and CV did not correlate with each other. Moreover, the value of w was modulated by the congruity of the relation between numerical magnitude and non-numerical visual cues, potentially accounting for the lack of correlation between the measures. Our findings thus challenge the overuse of w to assess ANS acuity and more generally put into question the relevance of correlating this measure with arithmetic without any deeper understanding of what they are really indexing.
Collapse
|
84
|
Children's mappings between number words and the approximate number system. Cognition 2015; 138:102-21. [PMID: 25721021 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.01.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 47] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/21/2014] [Revised: 10/14/2014] [Accepted: 01/19/2015] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
Humans can represent number either exactly--using their knowledge of exact numbers as supported by language, or approximately--using their approximate number system (ANS). Adults can map between these two systems--they can both translate from an approximate sense of the number of items in a brief visual display to a discrete number word estimate (i.e., ANS-to-Word), and can generate an approximation, for example by rapidly tapping, when provided with an exact verbal number (i.e., Word-to-ANS). Here we ask how these mappings are initially formed and whether one mapping direction may become functional before the other during development. In two experiments, we gave 2-5 year old children both an ANS-to-Word task, where they had to give a verbal number response to an approximate presentation (i.e., after seeing rapidly flashed dots, or watching rapid hand taps), and a Word-to-ANS task, where they had to generate an approximate response to a verbal number request (i.e., rapidly tapping after hearing a number word). Replicating previous results, children did not successfully generate numerically appropriate verbal responses in the ANS-to-Word task until after 4 years of age--well after they had acquired the Cardinality Principle of verbal counting. In contrast, children successfully generated numerically appropriate tapping sequences in the Word-to-ANS task before 4 years of age--well before many understood the Cardinality Principle. We further found that the accuracy of the mapping between the ANS and number words, as captured by error rates, continues to develop after this initial formation of the interface. These results suggest that the mapping between the ANS and verbal number representations is not functionally bidirectional in early development, and that the mapping direction from number representations to the ANS is established before the reverse.
Collapse
|
85
|
When you don't have to be exact: investigating computational estimation skills with a comparison task. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2015; 154:1-9. [PMID: 25461437 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.10.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/07/2014] [Revised: 08/01/2014] [Accepted: 10/30/2014] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
The present study is the first systematic investigation of computational estimation skills of multi-digit multiplication problems using an estimation comparison task. In two experiments, participants judged whether an estimated answer to a multi-digit multiplication problem was larger or smaller than a given reference number. Performance was superior in terms of speed and accuracy for smaller problem sizes, for trials in which the reference numbers were smaller vs. larger than the exact answers (consistent with the size effect) and for trials in which the reference numbers were numerically far compared to close to the exact answers (consistent with the distance effect). Strategy analysis showed that two main strategies were used to solve this task-approximate calculation and sense of magnitude. Most participants reported using the two strategies. Strategy choice was influenced by the distance between the reference number and exact answer, and by the interaction of problem size and reference number size. Theoretical implications as to the nature of numerical representations in the ANS (approximate number system) and to the estimation processes are suggested.
Collapse
|
86
|
Wilson AJ, Andrewes SG, Struthers H, Rowe VM, Bogdanovic R, Waldie KE. Dyscalculia and dyslexia in adults: Cognitive bases of comorbidity. LEARNING AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES 2015. [DOI: 10.1016/j.lindif.2014.11.017] [Citation(s) in RCA: 69] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
|
87
|
Pinheiro-Chagas P, Wood G, Knops A, Krinzinger H, Lonnemann J, Starling-Alves I, Willmes K, Haase VG. In how many ways is the approximate number system associated with exact calculation? PLoS One 2014; 9:e111155. [PMID: 25409446 PMCID: PMC4237330 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0111155] [Citation(s) in RCA: 36] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/12/2014] [Accepted: 09/23/2014] [Indexed: 01/29/2023] Open
Abstract
The approximate number system (ANS) has been consistently found to be associated with math achievement. However, little is known about the interactions between the different instantiations of the ANS and in how many ways they are related to exact calculation. In a cross-sectional design, we investigated the relationship between three measures of ANS acuity (non-symbolic comparison, non-symbolic estimation and non-symbolic addition), their cross-sectional trajectories and specific contributions to exact calculation. Children with mathematical difficulties (MD) and typically achieving (TA) controls attending the first six years of formal schooling participated in the study. The MD group exhibited impairments in multiple instantiations of the ANS compared to their TA peers. The ANS acuity measured by all three tasks positively correlated with age in TA children, while no correlation was found between non-symbolic comparison and age in the MD group. The measures of ANS acuity significantly correlated with each other, reflecting at least in part a common numerosity code. Crucially, we found that non-symbolic estimation partially and non-symbolic addition fully mediated the effects of non-symbolic comparison in exact calculation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Pedro Pinheiro-Chagas
- Cognitive Neuroimaging Unit, Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM) U992, Gif/Yvette, France
- NeuroSpin Center, Institute of BioImaging Commissariat à l'Energie Atomique (CEA), Gif/Yvette, France
- Developmental Neuropsychology Laboratory, Department of Psychology, Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais (UFMG), Belo Horizonte, Brazil
- * E-mail:
| | - Guilherme Wood
- Department of Neuropsychology, Institute of Psychology, Karl-Franzens University of Graz, Graz, Austria
| | - André Knops
- Faculty of Life Sciences, Humboldt Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Helga Krinzinger
- Section Child Neuropsychology, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, University Hospital of the RWTH, Aachen, Germany
| | - Jan Lonnemann
- German Institute for International Educational Research, Frankfurt, Germany
- Center for Individual Development and Adaptive Education of Children at Risk (IDeA), Frankfurt, Germany
| | - Isabella Starling-Alves
- Developmental Neuropsychology Laboratory, Department of Psychology, Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais (UFMG), Belo Horizonte, Brazil
| | - Klaus Willmes
- Section Neuropsychology, Department of Neurology, University Hospital of the RWTH, Aachen, Germany
| | - Vitor Geraldi Haase
- Developmental Neuropsychology Laboratory, Department of Psychology, Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais (UFMG), Belo Horizonte, Brazil
- Programa de Pós-graduação em Neurociências, Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais (UFMG), Belo Horizonte, Brazil
- Programa de Pós-graduação em Saúde da Criança e do Adolescente, Faculdade de Medicina, Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais (UFMG), Belo Horizonte, Brazil
| |
Collapse
|
88
|
Khemlani SS, Lotstein M, Johnson-Laird PN. Naive Probability: Model-Based Estimates of Unique Events. Cogn Sci 2014; 39:1216-58. [DOI: 10.1111/cogs.12193] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/27/2013] [Revised: 05/31/2014] [Accepted: 06/03/2014] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Sangeet S. Khemlani
- Navy Center for Applied Research in Artificial Intelligence; Naval Research Laboratory
| | - Max Lotstein
- Center for Cognitive Science; University of Freiburg
| | | |
Collapse
|
89
|
Spotorno N, McMillan CT, Powers JP, Clark R, Grossman M. Counting or chunking? Mathematical and heuristic abilities in patients with corticobasal syndrome and posterior cortical atrophy. Neuropsychologia 2014; 64:176-83. [PMID: 25278132 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.09.030] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/20/2014] [Revised: 09/15/2014] [Accepted: 09/17/2014] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
A growing amount of empirical data is showing that the ability to manipulate quantities in a precise and efficient fashion is rooted in cognitive mechanisms devoted to specific aspects of numbers processing. The analog number system (ANS) has a reasonable representation of quantities up to about 4, and represents larger quantities on the basis of a numerical ratio between quantities. In order to represent the precise cardinality of a number, the ANS may be supported by external algorithms such as language, leading to a "precise number system". In the setting of limited language, other number-related systems can appear. For example the parallel individuation system (PIS) supports a "chunking mechanism" that clusters units of larger numerosities into smaller subsets. In the present study we investigated number processing in non-aphasic patients with corticobasal syndrome (CBS) and posterior cortical atrophy (PCA), two neurodegenerative conditions that are associated with progressive parietal atrophy. The present study investigated these number systems in CBS and PCA by assessing the property of the ANS associated with smaller and larger numerosities, and the chunking property of the PIS. The results revealed that CBS/PCA patients are impaired in simple calculations (e.g., addition and subtraction) and that their performance strongly correlates with the size of the numbers involved in these calculations, revealing a clear magnitude effect. This magnitude effect was correlated with gray matter atrophy in parietal regions. Moreover, a numeral-dots transcoding task showed that CBS/PCA patients were able to take advantage of clustering in the spatial distribution of the dots of the array. The relative advantage associated with chunking compared to a random spatial distribution correlated with both parietal and prefrontal regions. These results shed light on the properties of systems for representing number knowledge in non-aphasic patients with CBS and PCA.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nicola Spotorno
- University of Pennsylvania, Perelman School of Medicine, Penn Frontotemporal Degeneration Center, Philadelphia, PA 19104, USA.
| | - Corey T McMillan
- University of Pennsylvania, Perelman School of Medicine, Penn Frontotemporal Degeneration Center, Philadelphia, PA 19104, USA
| | - John P Powers
- University of Pennsylvania, Perelman School of Medicine, Penn Frontotemporal Degeneration Center, Philadelphia, PA 19104, USA
| | - Robin Clark
- University of Pennsylvania, Department of Linguistics, Philadelphia, PA 19104, USA
| | - Murray Grossman
- University of Pennsylvania, Perelman School of Medicine, Penn Frontotemporal Degeneration Center, Philadelphia, PA 19104, USA
| |
Collapse
|
90
|
Garland A, Low J. Addition and subtraction in wild New Zealand robins. Behav Processes 2014; 109 Pt B:103-10. [PMID: 25193352 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2014.08.022] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/31/2014] [Revised: 08/18/2014] [Accepted: 08/22/2014] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
Abstract
This experiment aimed to investigate proto-arithmetic ability in a wild population of New Zealand robins. We investigated numerical competence from the context of computation: behavioural responses to arithmetic operations over small numbers of prey objects (mealworms). Robins' behavioural responses (such as search time) to the simple addition and subtraction problems presented in a Violation of Expectancy (VoE) paradigm were measured. Either a congruent (expected) or incongruent (unexpected) quantity of food items were hidden in a trap door out of view of the subject. Within view of the subject, a quantity of items were added into (and in some cases subtracted from) the apparatus which was either the same as that hidden, or different. Robins were then allowed them to find a quantity that either preserved or violated addition and subtraction outcomes. Robins searched around the apparatus longer when presented with an incongruent scenario violating arithmetic rules, demonstrating potential proto-arithmetic awareness of changes in prey quantity. This article is part of a Special Issue entitled: Cognition in the wild.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alexis Garland
- Victoria University of Wellington, School of Psychology, New Zealand.
| | - Jason Low
- Victoria University of Wellington, School of Psychology, New Zealand
| |
Collapse
|
91
|
Lindskog M, Winman A, Juslin P. The association between higher education and approximate number system acuity. Front Psychol 2014; 5:462. [PMID: 24904478 PMCID: PMC4033103 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00462] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/11/2014] [Accepted: 04/30/2014] [Indexed: 01/29/2023] Open
Abstract
Humans are equipped with an approximate number system (ANS) supporting non-symbolic numerosity representation. Studies indicate a relationship between ANS-precision (acuity) and math achievement. Whether the ANS is a prerequisite for learning mathematics or if mathematics education enhances the ANS remains an open question. We investigated the association between higher education and ANS acuity with university students majoring in subjects with varying amounts of mathematics (mathematics, business, and humanities), measured either early (First year) or late (Third year) in their studies. The results suggested a non-significant trend where students taking more mathematics had better ANS acuity and a significant improvement in ANS acuity as a function of study length that was mainly confined to the business students. The results provide partial support for the hypothesis that education in mathematics can enhance the ANS acuity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marcus Lindskog
- Department of Psychology, Uppsala University Uppsala, Sweden
| | - Anders Winman
- Department of Psychology, Uppsala University Uppsala, Sweden
| | - Peter Juslin
- Department of Psychology, Uppsala University Uppsala, Sweden
| |
Collapse
|
92
|
Belacchi C, Passolunghi MC, Brentan E, Dante A, Persi L, Cornoldi C. Approximate additions and working memory in individuals with Down syndrome. RESEARCH IN DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES 2014; 35:1027-1035. [PMID: 24602332 DOI: 10.1016/j.ridd.2014.01.036] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/01/2013] [Revised: 01/26/2014] [Accepted: 01/28/2014] [Indexed: 06/03/2023]
Abstract
There is some evidence that individuals with Down syndrome (DS) may have a poorer mathematical performance and a poorer working memory (WM) than typically developing (TD) children of the same mental age. In both typical and atypical individuals, different aspects of arithmetic and their relationships with WM have been largely studied, but the specific contribution of WM to the representation and elaboration of non-symbolic quantities has received little attention. The present study examined whether individuals with DS are as capable as TD children matched for fluid intelligence of estimating numerosity both of single sets and of added sets resulting when two sequentially presented sets are added together, also considering how these tasks related to verbal and visuospatial WM. Results showed that the DS group's performance was significantly worse than the TD group's in numerosity estimation involving one set, but not when estimating the numerosity resulting from the addition. Success in the addition task was related to success in the working memory tasks, but only for the group with DS; this applied especially to the visuospatial component, which (unlike the verbal component) was not impaired in the group with DS. It is concluded that the two numerosity tasks involve different processes. It is concluded that the arithmetical and working memory difficulties of individuals with DS are not general, and they can draw on their WM resources when estimating the numerosity of additions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Carmen Belacchi
- Department of Human Sciences, University of Urbino "Carlo Bo", Italy.
| | | | - Elena Brentan
- Department of General Psychology, University of Padova, Italy
| | - Arianna Dante
- Department of General Psychology, University of Padova, Italy
| | - Lara Persi
- Department of Human Sciences, University of Urbino "Carlo Bo", Italy
| | - Cesare Cornoldi
- Department of General Psychology, University of Padova, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
93
|
Association between individual differences in non-symbolic number acuity and math performance: a meta-analysis. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2014; 148:163-72. [PMID: 24583622 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.01.016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 256] [Impact Index Per Article: 23.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/05/2013] [Revised: 01/15/2014] [Accepted: 01/28/2014] [Indexed: 01/29/2023] Open
Abstract
Many recent studies have examined the association between number acuity, which is the ability to rapidly and non-symbolically estimate the quantity of items appearing in a scene, and symbolic math performance. However, various contradictory results have been reported. To comprehensively evaluate the association between number acuity and symbolic math performance, we conduct a meta-analysis to synthesize the results observed in previous studies. First, a meta-analysis of cross-sectional studies (36 samples, N = 4705) revealed a significant positive correlation between these skills (r = 0.20, 95% CI = [0.14, 0.26]); the association remained after considering other potential moderators (e.g., whether general cognitive abilities were controlled). Moreover, a meta-analysis of longitudinal studies revealed 1) that number acuity may prospectively predict later math performance (r = 0.24, 95% CI = [0.11, 0.37]; 6 samples) and 2) that number acuity is retrospectively correlated to early math performance as well (r = 0.17, 95% CI = [0.07, 0.26]; 5 samples). In summary, these pieces of evidence demonstrate a moderate but statistically significant association between number acuity and math performance. Based on the estimated effect sizes, power analyses were conducted, which suggested that many previous studies were underpowered due to small sample sizes. This may account for the disparity between findings in the literature, at least in part. Finally, the theoretical and practical implications of our meta-analytic findings are presented, and future research questions are discussed.
Collapse
|
94
|
Kibbe MM, Feigenson L. Young children 'solve for x' using the Approximate Number System. Dev Sci 2014; 18:38-49. [PMID: 24589420 DOI: 10.1111/desc.12177] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/09/2013] [Accepted: 12/09/2013] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
The Approximate Number System (ANS) supports basic arithmetic computation in early childhood, but it is unclear whether the ANS also supports the more complex computations introduced later in formal education. 'Solving for x' in addend-unknown problems is notoriously difficult for children, who often struggle with these types of problems well into high school. Here we asked whether 4-6-year-old children could solve for an unknown addend using the ANS. We presented problems either symbolically, using Arabic numerals or verbal number words, or non-symbolically, using collections of objects while preventing verbal counting. Across five experiments, children failed to identify the value of the unknown addend when problems were presented symbolically, but succeeded when problems were presented non-symbolically. Our results suggest that, well before formal exposure to unknown-addend problems, children appear to 'solve for x' in an intuitive way, using the ANS.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Melissa M Kibbe
- Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, Johns Hopkins University, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
95
|
|
96
|
Tosto M, Petrill S, Halberda J, Trzaskowski M, Tikhomirova T, Bogdanova O, Ly R, Wilmer J, Naiman D, Germine L, Plomin R, Kovas Y. Why do we differ in number sense? Evidence from a genetically sensitive investigation. INTELLIGENCE 2014; 43:35-46. [PMID: 24696527 PMCID: PMC3969293 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2013.12.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/05/2013] [Revised: 12/10/2013] [Accepted: 12/21/2013] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
Basic intellectual abilities of quantity and numerosity estimation have been detected across animal species. Such abilities are referred to as 'number sense'. For human species, individual differences in number sense are detectable early in life, persist in later development, and relate to general intelligence. The origins of these individual differences are unknown. To address this question, we conducted the first large-scale genetically sensitive investigation of number sense, assessing numerosity discrimination abilities in 837 pairs of monozygotic and 1422 pairs of dizygotic 16-year-old twin pairs. Univariate genetic analysis of the twin data revealed that number sense is modestly heritable (32%), with individual differences being largely explained by non-shared environmental influences (68%) and no contribution from shared environmental factors. Sex-Limitation model fitting revealed no differences between males and females in the etiology of individual differences in number sense abilities. We also carried out Genome-wide Complex Trait Analysis (GCTA) that estimates the population variance explained by additive effects of DNA differences among unrelated individuals. For 1118 unrelated individuals in our sample with genotyping information on 1.7 million DNA markers, GCTA estimated zero heritability for number sense, unlike other cognitive abilities in the same twin study where the GCTA heritability estimates were about 25%. The low heritability of number sense, observed in this study, is consistent with the directional selection explanation whereby additive genetic variance for evolutionary important traits is reduced.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- M.G. Tosto
- Department of Psychology, University of York, Heslington, York YO10 5DD, United Kingdom
- Department of Psychology, Tomsk State University, 36 Lenin Prospekt, 634050, Tomsk, Russia
| | - S.A. Petrill
- The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH 43210, United States
| | - J. Halberda
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, The Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD 21218-268, United States
| | - M. Trzaskowski
- King's College London, MRC Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, De Crespigny Park, London SE5 8AF, United Kingdom
| | - T.N. Tikhomirova
- Department of Psychology, Tomsk State University, 36 Lenin Prospekt, 634050, Tomsk, Russia
| | - O.Y. Bogdanova
- Department of Psychology, Tomsk State University, 36 Lenin Prospekt, 634050, Tomsk, Russia
| | - R. Ly
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, The Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD 21218-268, United States
| | - J.B. Wilmer
- Department of Psychology, Wellesley College, Central Street, Wellesley, MA 02481, United States
| | - D.Q. Naiman
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, The Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD 21218-268, United States
| | - L. Germine
- Center for Human Genetic Research, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA 02114, United States
| | - R. Plomin
- King's College London, MRC Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, De Crespigny Park, London SE5 8AF, United Kingdom
| | - Y. Kovas
- Department of Psychology, Tomsk State University, 36 Lenin Prospekt, 634050, Tomsk, Russia
- King's College London, MRC Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, De Crespigny Park, London SE5 8AF, United Kingdom
- Goldsmiths, University of London, London SE14 6NW, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
97
|
Hyde DC, Khanum S, Spelke ES. Brief non-symbolic, approximate number practice enhances subsequent exact symbolic arithmetic in children. Cognition 2014; 131:92-107. [PMID: 24462713 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.12.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 146] [Impact Index Per Article: 13.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/15/2013] [Revised: 11/05/2013] [Accepted: 12/19/2013] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
Recent research reveals a link between individual differences in mathematics achievement and performance on tasks that activate the approximate number system (ANS): a primitive cognitive system shared by diverse animal species and by humans of all ages. Here we used a brief experimental paradigm to test one causal hypothesis suggested by this relationship: activation of the ANS may enhance children's performance of symbolic arithmetic. Over 2 experiments, children who briefly practiced tasks that engaged primitive approximate numerical quantities performed better on subsequent exact, symbolic arithmetic problems than did children given other tasks involving comparison and manipulation of non-numerical magnitudes (brightness and length). The practice effect appeared specific to mathematics, as no differences between groups were observed on a comparable sentence completion task. These results move beyond correlational research and provide evidence that the exercise of non-symbolic numerical processes can enhance children's performance of symbolic mathematics.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Daniel C Hyde
- Brain and Cognitive Development Lab, Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana Champaign
| | - Saeeda Khanum
- National Institute of Psychology, Quaid-i-Azam University, Islamabad, Pakistan.,Laboratory for Developmental Studies, Department of Psychology, Harvard University
| | - Elizabeth S Spelke
- Laboratory for Developmental Studies, Department of Psychology, Harvard University
| |
Collapse
|
98
|
Measuring the approximate number system in children: Exploring the relationships among different tasks. LEARNING AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES 2014. [DOI: 10.1016/j.lindif.2013.10.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 39] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
|
99
|
Inglis M, Gilmore C. Indexing the approximate number system. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2014; 145:147-55. [PMID: 24361686 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.11.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 111] [Impact Index Per Article: 10.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/15/2013] [Revised: 11/26/2013] [Accepted: 11/27/2013] [Indexed: 01/29/2023] Open
Abstract
Much recent research attention has focused on understanding individual differences in the approximate number system, a cognitive system believed to underlie human mathematical competence. To date researchers have used four main indices of ANS acuity, and have typically assumed that they measure similar properties. Here we report a study which questions this assumption. We demonstrate that the numerical ratio effect has poor test-retest reliability and that it does not relate to either Weber fractions or accuracy on nonsymbolic comparison tasks. Furthermore, we show that Weber fractions follow a strongly skewed distribution and that they have lower test-retest reliability than a simple accuracy measure. We conclude by arguing that in the future researchers interested in indexing individual differences in ANS acuity should use accuracy figures, not Weber fractions or numerical ratio effects.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Matthew Inglis
- Mathematics Education Centre, Loughborough University, United Kingdom.
| | - Camilla Gilmore
- Mathematics Education Centre, Loughborough University, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
100
|
Aagten-Murphy D, Iversen J, Williams C, Meck W. Novel Inversions in Auditory Sequences Provide Evidence for Spontaneous Subtraction of Time and Number. TIMING & TIME PERCEPTION 2014. [DOI: 10.1163/22134468-00002028] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
Animals, including fish, birds, rodents, non-human primates, and pre-verbal infants are able to discriminate the duration and number of events without the use of language. In this paper, we present the results of six experiments exploring the capability of adult rats to count 2–6 sequentially presented white-noise stimuli. The investigation focuses on the animal’s ability to exhibit spontaneous subtraction following the presentation of novel stimulus inversions in the auditory signals being counted. Results suggest that a subtraction operation between two opposite sensory representations may be a general processing strategy used for the comparison of stimulus magnitudes. These findings are discussed within the context of a mode-control model of timing and counting that relies on an analog temporal-integration process for the addition and subtraction of sequential events.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- David Aagten-Murphy
- Department of Psychology, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München, Münich, Germany
| | - John R. Iversen
- Swartz Center for Computational Neuroscience and Institute for Neural Computation, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA
| | | | - Warren H. Meck
- Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, NC, USA
| |
Collapse
|