101
|
Westlund Schreiner M, Klimes-Dougan B, Begnel ED, Cullen KR. Conceptualizing the neurobiology of non-suicidal self-injury from the perspective of the Research Domain Criteria Project. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2015; 57:381-91. [PMID: 26424412 DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.09.011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 57] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/14/2015] [Revised: 09/11/2015] [Accepted: 09/24/2015] [Indexed: 12/30/2022]
Abstract
Non-suicidal self-injury (NSSI) commonly starts in adolescence and is associated with an array of negative outcomes. Neurobiological research investigating NSSI is in its early stages and most studies have examined this behavior within the context of specific diagnoses. However, the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) initiative encourages researchers to examine brain-behavior relationships across diagnoses. This review on the neurobiology associated with NSSI is organized using the domains proposed by RDoC: Negative Valence, Positive Valence, Cognitive, Social Processes, and Arousal/Regulatory Systems. Evidence of neurobiological anomalies is found in each of these domains. We also propose future research directions, especially in regard to human development. Future NSSI studies should address this behavior independent of diagnosis, examine relevant constructs across multiple units of analysis, and assess how systems change across development and course of illness. These advances will be essential for guiding neurobiologically informed intervention and prevention strategies to target NSSI. In doing so, we may prevent the associated negative outcomes across the lifespan.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Melinda Westlund Schreiner
- Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, N218 Elliott Hall, 75 East River Road, Minneapolis, MN 55455, United States.
| | - Bonnie Klimes-Dougan
- Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, N218 Elliott Hall, 75 East River Road, Minneapolis, MN 55455, United States.
| | - Erin D Begnel
- Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, N218 Elliott Hall, 75 East River Road, Minneapolis, MN 55455, United States.
| | - Kathryn R Cullen
- Department of Psychiatry, University of Minnesota Medical School, F268 West Building, 2450 Riverside Avenue, Minneapolis, MN 55454, United States.
| |
Collapse
|
102
|
Kawasaki I, Ito A, Fujii T, Ueno A, Yoshida K, Sakai S, Mugikura S, Takahashi S, Mori E. Differential activation of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex between male and female givers of social reputation. Neurosci Res 2015; 103:27-33. [PMID: 26235682 DOI: 10.1016/j.neures.2015.07.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/24/2015] [Revised: 07/20/2015] [Accepted: 07/24/2015] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
Abstract
Accumulating evidence has shown the profound influence of social reputation on human behavior and has implicated the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) in representing subjective values induced by social interaction. However, little is known regarding how the vmPFC encodes subjective pleasantness induced by social reputation received from others. We used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to investigate how the vmPFC in males and females encodes the subjective pleasantness of social reputation received from the same gender and from the opposite gender. Behavioral data showed that positive reputation was perceived to be more pleasant than negative reputation. Intriguingly, both male and female subjects showed greater differences in the pleasantness scores between the positive reputation condition and the negative reputation condition from females than between positive and negative reputations from males. Imaging data revealed that the left vmPFC specifically contributed to the processing of positive reputation. The activity patterns of the vmPFC corresponded to the gender differences in behavior during the processing of social reputation. These results indicate that the vmPFC plays a role in representing the subjective value of positive social reputation and that this region might be a final computational site in a stream of value-based decision-making processes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Iori Kawasaki
- Department of Behavioral Neurology and Cognitive Neuroscience, Tohoku University Graduate School of Medicine, 2-1 Seiryo-machi, Aoba-ku, Sendai 980-8575, Japan
| | - Ayahito Ito
- Department of Behavioral Neurology and Cognitive Neuroscience, Tohoku University Graduate School of Medicine, 2-1 Seiryo-machi, Aoba-ku, Sendai 980-8575, Japan; The Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (JSPS), Kojimachi Business Center Building, 5-3-1 Kojimachi, Chiyoda-ku, Tokyo 102-0083, Japan.
| | - Toshikatsu Fujii
- Department of Behavioral Neurology and Cognitive Neuroscience, Tohoku University Graduate School of Medicine, 2-1 Seiryo-machi, Aoba-ku, Sendai 980-8575, Japan; Kansei Fukushi Research Institute, Tohoku Fukushi University, 6-149-1 Kunimigaoka, Aoba-ku, Sendai 989-3201, Japan
| | - Aya Ueno
- The Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (JSPS), Kojimachi Business Center Building, 5-3-1 Kojimachi, Chiyoda-ku, Tokyo 102-0083, Japan; Division of Systems Neuroscience, Tohoku University Graduate School of Life Sciences, 2-1-1 Katahira, Aoba-ku, Sendai 980-8577, Japan
| | - Kazuki Yoshida
- Division of Health Sciences, Graduate School of Health Sciences, Hokkaido University, N12-W5, Kita-ku, Sapporo 060-0812, Japan
| | - Shinya Sakai
- Department of Functioning and Disability, Faculty of Health Science, Hokkaido University, N12-W5, Kita-ku, Sapporo 060-0812, Japan
| | - Shunji Mugikura
- Department of Diagnostic Radiology, Tohoku University Graduate School of Medicine, 1-1 Seiryo-machi, Aoba-ku, Sendai 980-8575, Japan
| | - Shoki Takahashi
- Department of Diagnostic Radiology, Tohoku University Graduate School of Medicine, 1-1 Seiryo-machi, Aoba-ku, Sendai 980-8575, Japan
| | - Etsuro Mori
- Department of Behavioral Neurology and Cognitive Neuroscience, Tohoku University Graduate School of Medicine, 2-1 Seiryo-machi, Aoba-ku, Sendai 980-8575, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
103
|
Powers KE, Chavez RS, Heatherton TF. Individual differences in response of dorsomedial prefrontal cortex predict daily social behavior. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2015. [PMID: 26206505 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsv096] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/14/2022] Open
Abstract
The capacity to accurately infer the thoughts and intentions of other people is critical for effective social interaction, and neural activity in dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (dmPFC) has long been linked with the extent to which people engage in mental state attribution. In this study, we combined functional neuroimaging and experience sampling methodologies to test the predictive value of this neural response for daily social behaviors. We found that individuals who displayed greater activity in dmPFC when viewing social scenes spent more time around other people on a daily basis. These findings suggest a specific role for the neural mechanisms that support the capacity to mentalize in guiding individuals toward situations containing valuable social outcomes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Katherine E Powers
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, Dartmouth College, Hanover, NH, USA, Department of Psychology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA and
| | - Robert S Chavez
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, Dartmouth College, Hanover, NH, USA, Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH, USA
| | - Todd F Heatherton
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, Dartmouth College, Hanover, NH, USA
| |
Collapse
|
104
|
Perceived communicative context and emotional content amplify visual word processing in the fusiform gyrus. J Neurosci 2015; 35:6010-9. [PMID: 25878274 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.3346-14.2015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 51] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
The personal significance of a language statement depends on its communicative context. However, this is rarely taken into account in neuroscience studies. Here, we investigate how the implied source of single word statements alters their cortical processing. Participants' brain event-related potentials were recorded in response to identical word streams consisting of positive, negative, and neutral trait adjectives stated to either represent personal trait feedback from a human or to be randomly generated by a computer. Results showed a strong impact of perceived sender. Regardless of content, the notion of receiving feedback from a human enhanced all components, starting with the P2 and encompassing early posterior negativity (EPN), P3, and the late positive potential (LPP). Moreover, negative feedback by the "human sender" elicited a larger EPN, whereas positive feedback generally induced a larger LPP. Source estimations revealed differences between "senders" in visual areas, particularly the bilateral fusiform gyri. Likewise, emotional content enhanced activity in these areas. These results specify how even implied sender identity changes the processing of single words in seemingly realistic communicative settings, amplifying their processing in the visual brain. This suggests that the concept of motivated attention extends from stimulus significance to simultaneous appraisal of contextual relevance. Finally, consistent with distinct stages of emotional processing, at least in contexts perceived as social, humans are initially alerted to negative content, but later process what is perceived as positive feedback more intensely.
Collapse
|
105
|
Meyer ML, Williams KD, Eisenberger NI. Why Social Pain Can Live on: Different Neural Mechanisms Are Associated with Reliving Social and Physical Pain. PLoS One 2015; 10:e0128294. [PMID: 26061877 PMCID: PMC4465485 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0128294] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/16/2014] [Accepted: 04/27/2015] [Indexed: 01/12/2023] Open
Abstract
Although social and physical pain recruit overlapping neural activity in regions associated with the affective component of pain, the two pains can diverge in their phenomenology. Most notably, feelings of social pain can be re-experienced or “relived,” even when the painful episode has long passed, whereas feelings of physical pain cannot be easily relived once the painful episode subsides. Here, we observed that reliving social (vs. physical) pain led to greater self-reported re-experienced pain and greater activity in affective pain regions (dorsal anterior cingulate cortex and anterior insula). Moreover, the degree of relived pain correlated positively with affective pain system activity. In contrast, reliving physical (vs. social) pain led to greater activity in the sensory-discriminative pain system (primary and secondary somatosensory cortex and posterior insula), which did not correlate with relived pain. Preferential engagement of these different pain mechanisms may reflect the use of different top-down neurocognitive pathways to elicit the pain. Social pain reliving recruited dorsomedial prefrontal cortex, often associated with mental state processing, which functionally correlated with affective pain system responses. In contrast, physical pain reliving recruited inferior frontal gyrus, known to be involved in body state processing, which functionally correlated with activation in the sensory pain system. These results update the physical-social pain overlap hypothesis: while overlapping mechanisms support live social and physical pain, distinct mechanisms guide internally-generated pain.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Meghan L. Meyer
- Psychology Department, University of California Los Angeles, Los Angeles, California, United States of America
- * E-mail:
| | - Kipling D. Williams
- Department of Psychology, Purdue University, West Lafayette, Indiana, United States of America
| | - Naomi I. Eisenberger
- Psychology Department, University of California Los Angeles, Los Angeles, California, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
106
|
Thagard P, Wood JV. Eighty phenomena about the self: representation, evaluation, regulation, and change. Front Psychol 2015; 6:334. [PMID: 25870574 PMCID: PMC4375917 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00334] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/19/2014] [Accepted: 03/09/2015] [Indexed: 01/13/2023] Open
Abstract
We propose a new approach for examining self-related aspects and phenomena. The approach includes (1) a taxonomy and (2) an emphasis on multiple levels of mechanisms. The taxonomy categorizes approximately eighty self-related phenomena according to three primary functions involving the self: representing, effecting, and changing. The representing self encompasses the ways in which people depict themselves, either to themselves or to others (e.g., self-concepts, self-presentation). The effecting self concerns ways in which people facilitate or limit their own traits and behaviors (e.g., self-enhancement, self-regulation). The changing self is less time-limited than the effecting self; it concerns phenomena that involve lasting alterations in how people represent and control themselves (e.g., self-expansion, self-development). Each self-related phenomenon within these three categories may be examined at four levels of interacting mechanisms (social, individual, neural, and molecular). We illustrate our approach by focusing on seven self-related phenomena.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Paul Thagard
- Department of Philosophy, University of Waterloo , Waterloo, ON, Canada
| | - Joanne V Wood
- Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo , Waterloo, ON, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
107
|
Kawamoto T, Ura M, Nittono H. Intrapersonal and interpersonal processes of social exclusion. Front Neurosci 2015; 9:62. [PMID: 25798081 PMCID: PMC4351632 DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2015.00062] [Citation(s) in RCA: 31] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/28/2014] [Accepted: 02/12/2015] [Indexed: 01/24/2023] Open
Abstract
People have a fundamental need to belong with others. Social exclusion impairs this need and has various effects on cognition, affect, and the behavior of excluded individuals. We have previously reported that activity in the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) and right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (rVLPFC) could be a neurocognitive index of social exclusion (Kawamoto et al., 2012). In this article, we provide an integrative framework for understanding occurrences during and after social exclusion, by reviewing neuroimaging, electrophysiological, and behavioral studies of dACC and rVLPFC, within the framework of intrapersonal and interpersonal processes of social exclusion. As a result, we have indicated directions for future studies to further clarify the phenomenon of social exclusion from the following perspectives: (1) constructional elements of social exclusion, (2) detection sensitivity and interpretation bias in social exclusion, (3) development of new methods to assess the reactivity to social exclusion, and (4) sources of social exclusion.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Taishi Kawamoto
- Japan Society for the Promotion of Science Tokyo, Japan ; Faculty of Integrated Arts and Sciences, Hiroshima University Higashi-Hiroshima, Japan
| | - Mitsuhiro Ura
- Department of Psychology, Otemon-Gakuin University Ibaraki, Japan
| | - Hiroshi Nittono
- Faculty of Integrated Arts and Sciences, Hiroshima University Higashi-Hiroshima, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
108
|
It still hurts: altered endogenous opioid activity in the brain during social rejection and acceptance in major depressive disorder. Mol Psychiatry 2015; 20:193-200. [PMID: 25600108 PMCID: PMC4469367 DOI: 10.1038/mp.2014.185] [Citation(s) in RCA: 136] [Impact Index Per Article: 13.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/15/2014] [Revised: 10/10/2014] [Accepted: 11/17/2014] [Indexed: 01/24/2023]
Abstract
The μ-opioid receptor (MOR) system, well known for dampening physical pain, is also hypothesized to dampen 'social pain.' We used positron emission tomography scanning with the selective MOR radioligand [(11)C]carfentanil to test the hypothesis that MOR system activation (reflecting endogenous opioid release) in response to social rejection and acceptance is altered in medication-free patients diagnosed with current major depressive disorder (MDD, n=17) compared with healthy controls (HCs, n=18). During rejection, MDD patients showed reduced endogenous opioid release in brain regions regulating stress, mood and motivation, and slower emotional recovery compared with HCs. During acceptance, only HCs showed increased social motivation, which was positively correlated with endogenous opioid release in the nucleus accumbens, a reward structure. Altered endogenous opioid activity in MDD may hinder emotional recovery from negative social interactions and decrease pleasure derived from positive interactions. Both effects may reinforce depression, trigger relapse and contribute to poor treatment outcomes.
Collapse
|
109
|
Eisenberger NI. Social Pain and the Brain: Controversies, Questions, and Where to Go from Here. Annu Rev Psychol 2015; 66:601-29. [DOI: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115146] [Citation(s) in RCA: 185] [Impact Index Per Article: 18.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Naomi I. Eisenberger
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, California 90095-1563;
| |
Collapse
|
110
|
Muscatell KA, Dedovic K, Slavich GM, Jarcho MR, Breen EC, Bower JE, Irwin MR, Eisenberger NI. Greater amygdala activity and dorsomedial prefrontal-amygdala coupling are associated with enhanced inflammatory responses to stress. Brain Behav Immun 2015; 43:46-53. [PMID: 25016200 PMCID: PMC4368432 DOI: 10.1016/j.bbi.2014.06.201] [Citation(s) in RCA: 162] [Impact Index Per Article: 16.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/15/2014] [Revised: 06/20/2014] [Accepted: 06/29/2014] [Indexed: 12/27/2022] Open
Abstract
Psychological stress is implicated in the etiology of many common chronic diseases and mental health disorders. Recent research suggests that inflammation may be a key biological mediator linking stress and health. Nevertheless, the neurocognitive pathways underlying stress-related increases in inflammatory activity are largely unknown. The present study thus examined associations between neural and inflammatory responses to an acute laboratory-based social stressor. Healthy female participants (n=31) were exposed to a brief episode of stress while they underwent an fMRI scan. Blood samples were taken before and after the stressor, and plasma was assayed for markers of inflammatory activity. Exposure to the stressor was associated with significant increases in feelings of social evaluation and rejection, and with increases in levels of inflammation. Analyses linking the neural and inflammatory data revealed that heightened neural activity in the amygdala in response to the stressor was associated with greater increases in inflammation. Functional connectivity analyses indicated that individuals who showed stronger coupling between the amygdala and the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (DMPFC) also showed a heightened inflammatory response to the stressor. Interestingly, activity in a different set of neural regions was related to increases in feelings of social rejection. These data show that greater amygdala activity in response to a stressor, as well as tighter coupling between the amygdala and the DMPFC, are associated with greater increases in inflammatory activity. Results from this study begin to identify neural mechanisms that might link stress with increased risk for inflammation-related disorders such as cardiovascular disease and depression.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Keely A. Muscatell
- Robert Wood Johnson Foundation Health and Society Scholars Program, University of California, San Francisco and University of California, Berkeley, San Francisco, CA, USA,Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, USA
| | - Katarina Dedovic
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, USA,Department of Psychiatry, Research Centre of the Douglas Mental Health University Institute, Montreal, Quebec, Canada
| | - George M. Slavich
- Department of Psychiatry & Biobehavioral Sciences and Cousins Center for Psychoneuroimmunology, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, USA
| | - Michael R. Jarcho
- Department of Psychiatry & Biobehavioral Sciences and Cousins Center for Psychoneuroimmunology, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, USA,Department of Neuroscience, Loras College, Dubuque, IA, USA
| | - Elizabeth C. Breen
- Department of Psychiatry & Biobehavioral Sciences and Cousins Center for Psychoneuroimmunology, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, USA
| | - Julienne E. Bower
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, USA,Department of Psychiatry & Biobehavioral Sciences and Cousins Center for Psychoneuroimmunology, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, USA
| | - Michael R. Irwin
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, USA,Department of Psychiatry & Biobehavioral Sciences and Cousins Center for Psychoneuroimmunology, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, USA
| | - Naomi I. Eisenberger
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, USA,Corresponding author. Address: UCLA Psychology Department, Box 951563, 1285 Franz Hall, Los Angeles, CA 90095-1563, USA. (N.I. Eisenberger)
| |
Collapse
|
111
|
Kong F, Ding K, Yang Z, Dang X, Hu S, Song Y, Liu J. Examining gray matter structures associated with individual differences in global life satisfaction in a large sample of young adults. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2014; 10:952-60. [PMID: 25406366 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsu144] [Citation(s) in RCA: 45] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/29/2014] [Accepted: 11/12/2014] [Indexed: 12/30/2022] Open
Abstract
Although much attention has been directed towards life satisfaction that refers to an individual's general cognitive evaluations of his or her life as a whole, little is known about the neural basis underlying global life satisfaction. In this study, we used voxel-based morphometry to investigate the structural neural correlates of life satisfaction in a large sample of young healthy adults (n = 299). We showed that individuals' life satisfaction was positively correlated with the regional gray matter volume (rGMV) in the right parahippocampal gyrus (PHG), and negatively correlated with the rGMV in the left precuneus and left ventromedial prefrontal cortex. This pattern of results remained significant even after controlling for the effect of general positive and negative affect, suggesting a unique structural correlates of life satisfaction. Furthermore, we found that self-esteem partially mediated the association between the PHG volume and life satisfaction as well as that between the precuneus volume and global life satisfaction. Taken together, we provide the first evidence for the structural neural basis of life satisfaction, and highlight that self-esteem might play a crucial role in cultivating an individual's life satisfaction.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Feng Kong
- State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning & IDG/McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Center for Collaboration and Innovation in Brain and Learning Sciences, and School of Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning & IDG/McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Center for Collaboration and Innovation in Brain and Learning Sciences, and School of Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China
| | - Ke Ding
- State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning & IDG/McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Center for Collaboration and Innovation in Brain and Learning Sciences, and School of Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning & IDG/McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Center for Collaboration and Innovation in Brain and Learning Sciences, and School of Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China
| | - Zetian Yang
- State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning & IDG/McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Center for Collaboration and Innovation in Brain and Learning Sciences, and School of Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning & IDG/McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Center for Collaboration and Innovation in Brain and Learning Sciences, and School of Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China
| | - Xiaobin Dang
- State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning & IDG/McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Center for Collaboration and Innovation in Brain and Learning Sciences, and School of Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning & IDG/McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Center for Collaboration and Innovation in Brain and Learning Sciences, and School of Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China
| | - Siyuan Hu
- State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning & IDG/McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Center for Collaboration and Innovation in Brain and Learning Sciences, and School of Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China
| | - Yiying Song
- State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning & IDG/McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Center for Collaboration and Innovation in Brain and Learning Sciences, and School of Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning & IDG/McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Center for Collaboration and Innovation in Brain and Learning Sciences, and School of Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China
| | - Jia Liu
- State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning & IDG/McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Center for Collaboration and Innovation in Brain and Learning Sciences, and School of Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China
| |
Collapse
|
112
|
Schindler S, Wegrzyn M, Steppacher I, Kissler J. It's all in your head - how anticipating evaluation affects the processing of emotional trait adjectives. Front Psychol 2014; 5:1292. [PMID: 25426095 PMCID: PMC4227471 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01292] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/28/2014] [Accepted: 10/24/2014] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Language has an intrinsically evaluative and communicative function. Words can serve to describe emotional traits and states in others and communicate evaluations. Using electroencephalography (EEG), we investigate how the cerebral processing of emotional trait adjectives is modulated by their perceived communicative sender in anticipation of an evaluation. 16 students were videotaped while they described themselves. They were told that a stranger would evaluate their personality based on this recording by endorsing trait adjectives. In a control condition a computer program supposedly randomly selected the adjectives. Actually, both conditions were random. A larger parietal N1 was found for adjectives in the supposedly human-generated condition. This indicates that more visual attention is allocated to the presented adjectives when putatively interacting with a human. Between 400 and 700 ms a fronto-central main effect of emotion was found. Positive, and in tendency also negative adjectives, led to a larger late positive potential (LPP) compared to neutral adjectives. A centro-parietal interaction in the LPP-window was due to larger LPP amplitudes for negative compared to neutral adjectives within the ‘human sender’ condition. Larger LPP amplitudes are related to stimulus elaboration and memory consolidation. Participants responded more to emotional content particularly when presented in a meaningful ‘human’ context. This was first observed in the early posterior negativity window (210–260 ms). But the significant interaction between sender and emotion reached only trend-level on post hoc tests. Our results specify differential effects of even implied communicative partners on emotional language processing. They show that anticipating evaluation by a communicative partner alone is sufficient to increase the relevance of particularly emotional adjectives, given a seemingly realistic interactive setting.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sebastian Schindler
- Department of Psychology, Affective Neuropsychology, University of Bielefeld Bielefeld, Germany ; Center of Excellence Cognitive Interaction Technology, University of Bielefeld Bielefeld, Germany
| | - Martin Wegrzyn
- Department of Psychology, Affective Neuropsychology, University of Bielefeld Bielefeld, Germany ; Center of Excellence Cognitive Interaction Technology, University of Bielefeld Bielefeld, Germany
| | - Inga Steppacher
- Department of Psychology, Affective Neuropsychology, University of Bielefeld Bielefeld, Germany
| | - Johanna Kissler
- Department of Psychology, Affective Neuropsychology, University of Bielefeld Bielefeld, Germany ; Center of Excellence Cognitive Interaction Technology, University of Bielefeld Bielefeld, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
113
|
Iffland B, Sansen LM, Catani C, Neuner F. Rapid heartbeat, but dry palms: reactions of heart rate and skin conductance levels to social rejection. Front Psychol 2014; 5:956. [PMID: 25221535 PMCID: PMC4148623 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00956] [Citation(s) in RCA: 35] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/24/2014] [Accepted: 08/11/2014] [Indexed: 12/30/2022] Open
Abstract
Background: Social rejection elicits negative mood, emotional distress, and neural activity in networks that are associated with physical pain. However, studies assessing physiological reactions to social rejection are rare and results of these studies were found to be ambiguous. Therefore, the present study aimed to examine and specify physiological effects of social rejection. Methods: Participants (n = 50) were assigned to either a social exclusion or inclusion condition of a virtual ball-tossing game (Cyberball). Immediate and delayed physiological [skin conductance level (SCL) and heart rate] reactions were recorded. In addition, subjects reported levels of affect, emotional states, and fundamental needs. Results: Subjects who were socially rejected showed increased heart rates. However, social rejection had no effect on subjects' SCLs. Both conditions showed heightened arousal on this measurement. Furthermore, psychological consequences of social rejection indicated the validity of the paradigm. Conclusions: Our results reveal that social rejection evokes an immediate physiological reaction. Accelerated heart rates indicate that behavior activation rather than inhibition is associated with socially threatening events. In addition, results revealed gender-specific response patterns suggesting that sample characteristics such as differences in gender may account for ambiguous findings of physiological reactions to social rejection.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Benjamin Iffland
- Department of Psychology, Bielefeld University Bielefeld, Germany
| | - Lisa M Sansen
- Department of Psychology, Bielefeld University Bielefeld, Germany ; Christoph-Dornier-Stiftung für Klinische Psychologie Bielefeld, Germany
| | - Claudia Catani
- Department of Psychology, Bielefeld University Bielefeld, Germany
| | - Frank Neuner
- Department of Psychology, Bielefeld University Bielefeld, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
114
|
Rotge JY, Lemogne C, Hinfray S, Huguet P, Grynszpan O, Tartour E, George N, Fossati P. A meta-analysis of the anterior cingulate contribution to social pain. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2014; 10:19-27. [PMID: 25140048 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsu110] [Citation(s) in RCA: 156] [Impact Index Per Article: 14.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/08/2023] Open
Abstract
Many functional magnetic resonance imaging studies have explored the neural correlates of social pain that results from social threat, exclusion, rejection, loss or negative evaluation. Although activations have consistently been reported within the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), it remains unclear which ACC subdivision is particularly involved. To provide a quantitative estimation of the specific involvement of ACC subdivisions in social pain, we conducted a voxel-based meta-analysis. The literature search identified 46 articles that included 940 subjects, the majority of which used the cyberball task. Significant likelihoods of activation were found in both the ventral and dorsal ACC for both social pain elicitation and self-reported distress during social pain. Self-reported distress involved more specifically the subgenual and pregenual ACC than social pain-related contrasts. The cyberball task involved the anterior midcingulate cortex to a lesser extent than other experimental tasks. During social pain, children exhibited subgenual activations to a greater extent than adults. Finally, the ventro-dorsal gradient of ACC activations in cyberball studies was related to the length of exclusion phases. The present meta-analysis contributes to a better understanding of the role of ACC subdivisions in social pain, and it could be of particular importance for guiding future studies of social pain and its neural underpinnings.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jean-Yves Rotge
- INSERM UMR 894, Centre Psychiatrie et Neurosciences, Paris, France, Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité, Faculté de Médecine, Paris, France, AP-HP, Hôpitaux Universitaires Paris Ouest, Service Universitaire de Psychiatrie de l'Adulte et du Sujet Agé, Paris, France, Aix-Marseille Université, Marseille, France, CNRS UMR 7290, Laboratoire de Psychologie Cognitive, Marseille, France, Université Pierre et Marie Curie, Institut des Systèmes Intelligents et de Robotique (ISIR), CNRS UMR 7222, Paris, France, Assistance Publique-Hôpitaux de Paris, Service d'Immunologie Biologique, Hôpital Européen Georges Pompidou, Paris, France, Inserm U 1127, CNRS UMR 7225, Sorbonne Universités, UPMC Univ Paris 06, UMR S 1127, Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle, ICM, Social and Affective Neuroscience (SAN) Laboratory, F-75013 Paris, France, and AP-HP, Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, Department of Psychiatry, F-75013 Paris, France INSERM UMR 894, Centre Psychiatrie et Neurosciences, Paris, France, Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité, Faculté de Médecine, Paris, France, AP-HP, Hôpitaux Universitaires Paris Ouest, Service Universitaire de Psychiatrie de l'Adulte et du Sujet Agé, Paris, France, Aix-Marseille Université, Marseille, France, CNRS UMR 7290, Laboratoire de Psychologie Cognitive, Marseille, France, Université Pierre et Marie Curie, Institut des Systèmes Intelligents et de Robotique (ISIR), CNRS UMR 7222, Paris, France, Assistance Publique-Hôpitaux de Paris, Service d'Immunologie Biologique, Hôpital Européen Georges Pompidou, Paris, France, Inserm U 1127, CNRS UMR 7225, Sorbonne Universités, UPMC Univ Paris 06, UMR S 1127, Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle, ICM, Social and Affective Neuroscience (SAN) Laboratory, F-75013 Paris, France, and AP-HP, Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, Department of Psychiatry, F-75013 Paris, France
| | - Cedric Lemogne
- INSERM UMR 894, Centre Psychiatrie et Neurosciences, Paris, France, Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité, Faculté de Médecine, Paris, France, AP-HP, Hôpitaux Universitaires Paris Ouest, Service Universitaire de Psychiatrie de l'Adulte et du Sujet Agé, Paris, France, Aix-Marseille Université, Marseille, France, CNRS UMR 7290, Laboratoire de Psychologie Cognitive, Marseille, France, Université Pierre et Marie Curie, Institut des Systèmes Intelligents et de Robotique (ISIR), CNRS UMR 7222, Paris, France, Assistance Publique-Hôpitaux de Paris, Service d'Immunologie Biologique, Hôpital Européen Georges Pompidou, Paris, France, Inserm U 1127, CNRS UMR 7225, Sorbonne Universités, UPMC Univ Paris 06, UMR S 1127, Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle, ICM, Social and Affective Neuroscience (SAN) Laboratory, F-75013 Paris, France, and AP-HP, Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, Department of Psychiatry, F-75013 Paris, France INSERM UMR 894, Centre Psychiatrie et Neurosciences, Paris, France, Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité, Faculté de Médecine, Paris, France, AP-HP, Hôpitaux Universitaires Paris Ouest, Service Universitaire de Psychiatrie de l'Adulte et du Sujet Agé, Paris, France, Aix-Marseille Université, Marseille, France, CNRS UMR 7290, Laboratoire de Psychologie Cognitive, Marseille, France, Université Pierre et Marie Curie, Institut des Systèmes Intelligents et de Robotique (ISIR), CNRS UMR 7222, Paris, France, Assistance Publique-Hôpitaux de Paris, Service d'Immunologie Biologique, Hôpital Européen Georges Pompidou, Paris, France, Inserm U 1127, CNRS UMR 7225, Sorbonne Universités, UPMC Univ Paris 06, UMR S 1127, Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle, ICM, Social and Affective Neuroscience (SAN) Laboratory, F-75013 Paris, France, and AP-HP, Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, Department of Psychiatry, F-75013 Paris, France INSERM UMR 894, Centre Psychiatrie et Neurosciences, Paris, France, Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Pari
| | - Sophie Hinfray
- INSERM UMR 894, Centre Psychiatrie et Neurosciences, Paris, France, Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité, Faculté de Médecine, Paris, France, AP-HP, Hôpitaux Universitaires Paris Ouest, Service Universitaire de Psychiatrie de l'Adulte et du Sujet Agé, Paris, France, Aix-Marseille Université, Marseille, France, CNRS UMR 7290, Laboratoire de Psychologie Cognitive, Marseille, France, Université Pierre et Marie Curie, Institut des Systèmes Intelligents et de Robotique (ISIR), CNRS UMR 7222, Paris, France, Assistance Publique-Hôpitaux de Paris, Service d'Immunologie Biologique, Hôpital Européen Georges Pompidou, Paris, France, Inserm U 1127, CNRS UMR 7225, Sorbonne Universités, UPMC Univ Paris 06, UMR S 1127, Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle, ICM, Social and Affective Neuroscience (SAN) Laboratory, F-75013 Paris, France, and AP-HP, Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, Department of Psychiatry, F-75013 Paris, France INSERM UMR 894, Centre Psychiatrie et Neurosciences, Paris, France, Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité, Faculté de Médecine, Paris, France, AP-HP, Hôpitaux Universitaires Paris Ouest, Service Universitaire de Psychiatrie de l'Adulte et du Sujet Agé, Paris, France, Aix-Marseille Université, Marseille, France, CNRS UMR 7290, Laboratoire de Psychologie Cognitive, Marseille, France, Université Pierre et Marie Curie, Institut des Systèmes Intelligents et de Robotique (ISIR), CNRS UMR 7222, Paris, France, Assistance Publique-Hôpitaux de Paris, Service d'Immunologie Biologique, Hôpital Européen Georges Pompidou, Paris, France, Inserm U 1127, CNRS UMR 7225, Sorbonne Universités, UPMC Univ Paris 06, UMR S 1127, Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle, ICM, Social and Affective Neuroscience (SAN) Laboratory, F-75013 Paris, France, and AP-HP, Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, Department of Psychiatry, F-75013 Paris, France
| | - Pascal Huguet
- INSERM UMR 894, Centre Psychiatrie et Neurosciences, Paris, France, Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité, Faculté de Médecine, Paris, France, AP-HP, Hôpitaux Universitaires Paris Ouest, Service Universitaire de Psychiatrie de l'Adulte et du Sujet Agé, Paris, France, Aix-Marseille Université, Marseille, France, CNRS UMR 7290, Laboratoire de Psychologie Cognitive, Marseille, France, Université Pierre et Marie Curie, Institut des Systèmes Intelligents et de Robotique (ISIR), CNRS UMR 7222, Paris, France, Assistance Publique-Hôpitaux de Paris, Service d'Immunologie Biologique, Hôpital Européen Georges Pompidou, Paris, France, Inserm U 1127, CNRS UMR 7225, Sorbonne Universités, UPMC Univ Paris 06, UMR S 1127, Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle, ICM, Social and Affective Neuroscience (SAN) Laboratory, F-75013 Paris, France, and AP-HP, Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, Department of Psychiatry, F-75013 Paris, France INSERM UMR 894, Centre Psychiatrie et Neurosciences, Paris, France, Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité, Faculté de Médecine, Paris, France, AP-HP, Hôpitaux Universitaires Paris Ouest, Service Universitaire de Psychiatrie de l'Adulte et du Sujet Agé, Paris, France, Aix-Marseille Université, Marseille, France, CNRS UMR 7290, Laboratoire de Psychologie Cognitive, Marseille, France, Université Pierre et Marie Curie, Institut des Systèmes Intelligents et de Robotique (ISIR), CNRS UMR 7222, Paris, France, Assistance Publique-Hôpitaux de Paris, Service d'Immunologie Biologique, Hôpital Européen Georges Pompidou, Paris, France, Inserm U 1127, CNRS UMR 7225, Sorbonne Universités, UPMC Univ Paris 06, UMR S 1127, Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle, ICM, Social and Affective Neuroscience (SAN) Laboratory, F-75013 Paris, France, and AP-HP, Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, Department of Psychiatry, F-75013 Paris, France
| | - Ouriel Grynszpan
- INSERM UMR 894, Centre Psychiatrie et Neurosciences, Paris, France, Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité, Faculté de Médecine, Paris, France, AP-HP, Hôpitaux Universitaires Paris Ouest, Service Universitaire de Psychiatrie de l'Adulte et du Sujet Agé, Paris, France, Aix-Marseille Université, Marseille, France, CNRS UMR 7290, Laboratoire de Psychologie Cognitive, Marseille, France, Université Pierre et Marie Curie, Institut des Systèmes Intelligents et de Robotique (ISIR), CNRS UMR 7222, Paris, France, Assistance Publique-Hôpitaux de Paris, Service d'Immunologie Biologique, Hôpital Européen Georges Pompidou, Paris, France, Inserm U 1127, CNRS UMR 7225, Sorbonne Universités, UPMC Univ Paris 06, UMR S 1127, Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle, ICM, Social and Affective Neuroscience (SAN) Laboratory, F-75013 Paris, France, and AP-HP, Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, Department of Psychiatry, F-75013 Paris, France
| | - Eric Tartour
- INSERM UMR 894, Centre Psychiatrie et Neurosciences, Paris, France, Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité, Faculté de Médecine, Paris, France, AP-HP, Hôpitaux Universitaires Paris Ouest, Service Universitaire de Psychiatrie de l'Adulte et du Sujet Agé, Paris, France, Aix-Marseille Université, Marseille, France, CNRS UMR 7290, Laboratoire de Psychologie Cognitive, Marseille, France, Université Pierre et Marie Curie, Institut des Systèmes Intelligents et de Robotique (ISIR), CNRS UMR 7222, Paris, France, Assistance Publique-Hôpitaux de Paris, Service d'Immunologie Biologique, Hôpital Européen Georges Pompidou, Paris, France, Inserm U 1127, CNRS UMR 7225, Sorbonne Universités, UPMC Univ Paris 06, UMR S 1127, Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle, ICM, Social and Affective Neuroscience (SAN) Laboratory, F-75013 Paris, France, and AP-HP, Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, Department of Psychiatry, F-75013 Paris, France INSERM UMR 894, Centre Psychiatrie et Neurosciences, Paris, France, Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité, Faculté de Médecine, Paris, France, AP-HP, Hôpitaux Universitaires Paris Ouest, Service Universitaire de Psychiatrie de l'Adulte et du Sujet Agé, Paris, France, Aix-Marseille Université, Marseille, France, CNRS UMR 7290, Laboratoire de Psychologie Cognitive, Marseille, France, Université Pierre et Marie Curie, Institut des Systèmes Intelligents et de Robotique (ISIR), CNRS UMR 7222, Paris, France, Assistance Publique-Hôpitaux de Paris, Service d'Immunologie Biologique, Hôpital Européen Georges Pompidou, Paris, France, Inserm U 1127, CNRS UMR 7225, Sorbonne Universités, UPMC Univ Paris 06, UMR S 1127, Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle, ICM, Social and Affective Neuroscience (SAN) Laboratory, F-75013 Paris, France, and AP-HP, Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, Department of Psychiatry, F-75013 Paris, France
| | - Nathalie George
- INSERM UMR 894, Centre Psychiatrie et Neurosciences, Paris, France, Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité, Faculté de Médecine, Paris, France, AP-HP, Hôpitaux Universitaires Paris Ouest, Service Universitaire de Psychiatrie de l'Adulte et du Sujet Agé, Paris, France, Aix-Marseille Université, Marseille, France, CNRS UMR 7290, Laboratoire de Psychologie Cognitive, Marseille, France, Université Pierre et Marie Curie, Institut des Systèmes Intelligents et de Robotique (ISIR), CNRS UMR 7222, Paris, France, Assistance Publique-Hôpitaux de Paris, Service d'Immunologie Biologique, Hôpital Européen Georges Pompidou, Paris, France, Inserm U 1127, CNRS UMR 7225, Sorbonne Universités, UPMC Univ Paris 06, UMR S 1127, Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle, ICM, Social and Affective Neuroscience (SAN) Laboratory, F-75013 Paris, France, and AP-HP, Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, Department of Psychiatry, F-75013 Paris, France
| | - Philippe Fossati
- INSERM UMR 894, Centre Psychiatrie et Neurosciences, Paris, France, Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité, Faculté de Médecine, Paris, France, AP-HP, Hôpitaux Universitaires Paris Ouest, Service Universitaire de Psychiatrie de l'Adulte et du Sujet Agé, Paris, France, Aix-Marseille Université, Marseille, France, CNRS UMR 7290, Laboratoire de Psychologie Cognitive, Marseille, France, Université Pierre et Marie Curie, Institut des Systèmes Intelligents et de Robotique (ISIR), CNRS UMR 7222, Paris, France, Assistance Publique-Hôpitaux de Paris, Service d'Immunologie Biologique, Hôpital Européen Georges Pompidou, Paris, France, Inserm U 1127, CNRS UMR 7225, Sorbonne Universités, UPMC Univ Paris 06, UMR S 1127, Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle, ICM, Social and Affective Neuroscience (SAN) Laboratory, F-75013 Paris, France, and AP-HP, Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, Department of Psychiatry, F-75013 Paris, France INSERM UMR 894, Centre Psychiatrie et Neurosciences, Paris, France, Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité, Faculté de Médecine, Paris, France, AP-HP, Hôpitaux Universitaires Paris Ouest, Service Universitaire de Psychiatrie de l'Adulte et du Sujet Agé, Paris, France, Aix-Marseille Université, Marseille, France, CNRS UMR 7290, Laboratoire de Psychologie Cognitive, Marseille, France, Université Pierre et Marie Curie, Institut des Systèmes Intelligents et de Robotique (ISIR), CNRS UMR 7222, Paris, France, Assistance Publique-Hôpitaux de Paris, Service d'Immunologie Biologique, Hôpital Européen Georges Pompidou, Paris, France, Inserm U 1127, CNRS UMR 7225, Sorbonne Universités, UPMC Univ Paris 06, UMR S 1127, Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle, ICM, Social and Affective Neuroscience (SAN) Laboratory, F-75013 Paris, France, and AP-HP, Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, Department of Psychiatry, F-75013 Paris, France
| |
Collapse
|
115
|
Wu L, Cai H, Gu R, Luo YLL, Zhang J, Yang J, Shi Y, Ding L. Neural manifestations of implicit self-esteem: an ERP study. PLoS One 2014; 9:e101837. [PMID: 25006966 PMCID: PMC4090159 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0101837] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/06/2014] [Accepted: 06/11/2014] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Behavioral research has established that humans implicitly tend to hold a positive view toward themselves. In this study, we employed the event-related potential (ERP) technique to explore neural manifestations of positive implicit self-esteem using the Go/Nogo association task (GNAT). Participants generated a response (Go) or withheld a response (Nogo) to self or others words and good or bad attributes. Behavioral data showed that participants responded faster to the self paired with good than the self paired with bad, whereas the opposite proved true for others, reflecting the positive nature of implicit self-esteem. ERP results showed an augmented N200 over the frontal areas in Nogo responses relative to Go responses. Moreover, the positive implicit self-positivity bias delayed the onset time of the N200 wave difference between Nogo and Go trials, suggesting that positive implicit self-esteem is manifested on neural activity about 270 ms after the presentation of self-relevant stimuli. These findings provide neural evidence for the positivity and automaticity of implicit self-esteem.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lili Wu
- Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China
| | - Huajian Cai
- Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China
- * E-mail:
| | - Ruolei Gu
- Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China
| | - Yu L. L. Luo
- Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China
| | - Jianxin Zhang
- Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China
| | - Jing Yang
- Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China
| | - Yuanyuan Shi
- Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China
| | - Lei Ding
- School of Electrical and Computer Engineering, University of Oklahoma, Norman, Oklahoma, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
116
|
Chester DS, Pond RS, DeWall CN. Alexithymia is associated with blunted anterior cingulate response to social rejection: implications for daily rejection. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2014; 10:517-22. [PMID: 24894765 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsu082] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/10/2013] [Accepted: 05/31/2014] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Social rejection elicits distress through the brain's alarm system, the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC). The distress of rejection facilitates subsequent inclusion. As a result, traits that blunt this dACC response to social rejection might then threaten group membership, leading to further subsequent rejection. Alexithymia, the inability to identify and describe affective states, is associated with social impairment and reduced dACC activity under conditions of negative affect. Thus, we expected that alexithymia would relate to less dACC activation during rejection and that this blunted response would explain an association between alexithymia and greater rejection in everyday life. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging and daily diaries, we found that subclinical individual differences in the core feature of alexithymia, difficulty identifying affect, was associated with a blunted dACC response to social rejection. Deficits in affect identification were also associated with greater daily rejection and that this effect was mediated and suppressed by dACC activation to rejection. Our findings emphasize the crucial role of the dACC in response to social rejection and extend the literature on alexithymia's ability to dampen neural responses and contribute to poor social functioning. The suppressing role of the dACC suggests future directions for clinical interventions on those with affective disorders.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- David S Chester
- Department of Psychology, University of Kentucky, Lexington, KYUSA 40506 and Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina, Wilmington, NC 28403, USA
| | - Richard S Pond
- Department of Psychology, University of Kentucky, Lexington, KYUSA 40506 and Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina, Wilmington, NC 28403, USA
| | - C Nathan DeWall
- Department of Psychology, University of Kentucky, Lexington, KYUSA 40506 and Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina, Wilmington, NC 28403, USA
| |
Collapse
|
117
|
Chavez RS, Heatherton TF. Multimodal frontostriatal connectivity underlies individual differences in self-esteem. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2014; 10:364-70. [PMID: 24795440 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsu063] [Citation(s) in RCA: 57] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
A heightened sense of self-esteem is associated with a reduced risk for several types of affective and psychiatric disorders, including depression, anxiety and eating disorders. However, little is known about how brain systems integrate self-referential processing and positive evaluation to give rise to these feelings. To address this, we combined diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to test how frontostriatal connectivity reflects long-term trait and short-term state aspects of self-esteem. Using DTI, we found individual variability in white matter structural integrity between the medial prefrontal cortex and the ventral striatum was related to trait measures of self-esteem, reflecting long-term stability of self-esteem maintenance. Using fMRI, we found that functional connectivity of these regions during positive self-evaluation was related to current feelings of self-esteem, reflecting short-term state self-esteem. These results provide convergent anatomical and functional evidence that self-esteem is related to the connectivity of frontostriatal circuits and suggest that feelings of self-worth may emerge from neural systems integrating information about the self with positive affect and reward. This information could potentially inform the etiology of diminished self-esteem underlying multiple psychiatric conditions and inform future studies of evaluative self-referential processing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Robert S Chavez
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Dartmouth College, Hanover, NH, USA
| | - Todd F Heatherton
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Dartmouth College, Hanover, NH, USA
| |
Collapse
|
118
|
Leitner JB, Hehman E, Jones JM, Forbes CE. Self-enhancement influences medial frontal cortex alpha power to social rejection feedback. J Cogn Neurosci 2014; 26:2330-41. [PMID: 24738770 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_00645] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
Abstract
Although previous research has demonstrated that individuals are motivated to self-enhance, the neurocognitive mechanisms and temporal dynamics of self-enhancement are poorly understood. The current research examined whether self-enhancing motivations affect the perceptual processing of social feedback. Participants who varied in self-enhancement motivations received accept and reject feedback while EEG was recorded. Following this task, we measured perceptions of feedback by asking participants to estimate the number of times they were rejected. Source localization and time-frequency analyses revealed that alpha power in the medial frontal cortex (MFC) completely mediated the relationship between self-enhancement motivations and rejection estimates. Specifically, greater self-enhancement motivations predicted decreased MFC alpha power to reject compared to accept feedback, which predicted decreased rejection estimates. These findings suggest that self-enhancement motivations decrease perception of social rejection by influencing how the MFC processes social feedback.
Collapse
|
119
|
Kawamoto T, Nittono H, Ura M. Social exclusion induces early-stage perceptual and behavioral changes in response to social cues. Soc Neurosci 2014; 9:174-85. [PMID: 24499456 DOI: 10.1080/17470919.2014.883325] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
Abstract
Social exclusion is so aversive that it causes broad cognitive and behavioral changes to regulate the individual's belonging status. The present study examined whether such changes also occur at early neural or automatic behavioral levels in response to social cues. Event-related brain potentials (ERPs) and facial electromyograms (EMGs) were recorded during a task in which participants viewed smiling, disgusted, and neutral faces after experiencing social exclusion or inclusion. Social exclusion was manipulated using a simple ball-tossing game (Cyberball), and need threat was assessed after the game. We found that zygomaticus major muscle activity, which reflects facial mimicry, was larger in response to smiling faces after exclusion than after inclusion. In addition, P1 amplitude, which reflects visual attention, was larger for disgusted faces than for neutral faces following social exclusion. N170 amplitude, which reflects structural encoding of the face, was correlated with heightened need threat. These findings demonstrate that social exclusion induces immediate and rapid changes in attention, perception, and automatic behavior. These findings reflect the rapid and primary regulation of belonging.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Taishi Kawamoto
- a Graduate School of Integrated Arts and Sciences , Hiroshima University , Higashi-Hiroshima 739-8521 , Japan
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
120
|
Morelli SA, Torre JB, Eisenberger NI. The neural bases of feeling understood and not understood. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2014; 9:1890-6. [PMID: 24396002 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nst191] [Citation(s) in RCA: 50] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Past research suggests that feeling understood enhances both personal and social well-being. However, little research has examined the neurobiological bases of feeling understood and not understood. We addressed these gaps by experimentally inducing felt understanding and not understanding as participants underwent functional magnetic resonance imaging. The results demonstrated that feeling understood activated neural regions previously associated with reward and social connection (i.e. ventral striatum and middle insula), while not feeling understood activated neural regions previously associated with negative affect (i.e. anterior insula). Both feeling understood and not feeling understood activated different components of the mentalizing system (feeling understood: precuneus and temporoparietal junction; not feeling understood: dorsomedial prefrontal cortex). Neural responses were associated with subsequent feelings of social connection and disconnection and were modulated by individual differences in rejection sensitivity. Thus, this study provides insight into the psychological processes underlying feeling understood (or not) and may suggest new avenues for targeted interventions that amplify the benefits of feeling understood or buffer individuals from the harmful consequences of not feeling understood.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sylvia A Morelli
- Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305, USA and Department of Psychology, University of California Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA 90095-1563, USA
| | - Jared B Torre
- Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305, USA and Department of Psychology, University of California Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA 90095-1563, USA
| | - Naomi I Eisenberger
- Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305, USA and Department of Psychology, University of California Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA 90095-1563, USA
| |
Collapse
|
121
|
Hostinar CE, Sullivan RM, Gunnar MR. Psychobiological mechanisms underlying the social buffering of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical axis: a review of animal models and human studies across development. Psychol Bull 2014; 140:256-282. [PMID: 23607429 PMCID: PMC3844011 DOI: 10.1037/a0032671 10.1037/a0032671] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/28/2023]
Abstract
Discovering the stress-buffering effects of social relationships has been one of the major findings in psychobiology in the last century. However, an understanding of the underlying neurobiological and psychological mechanisms of this buffering is only beginning to emerge. An important avenue of this research concerns the neurocircuitry that can regulate the activity of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical (HPA) axis. The present review is a translational effort aimed at integrating animal models and human studies of the social regulation of the HPA axis from infancy to adulthood, specifically focusing on the process that has been named social buffering. This process has been noted across species and consists of a dampened HPA axis stress response to threat or challenge that occurs with the presence or assistance of a conspecific. We describe aspects of the relevant underlying neurobiology when enough information exists and expose major gaps in our understanding across all domains of the literatures we aimed to integrate. We provide a working conceptual model focused on the role of oxytocinergic systems and prefrontal neural networks as 2 of the putative biological mediators of this process, and propose that the role of early experiences is critical in shaping later social buffering effects. This synthesis points to both general future directions and specific experiments that need to be conducted to build a more comprehensive model of the HPA social buffering effect across the life span that incorporates multiple levels of analysis: neuroendocrine, behavioral, and social.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Regina M Sullivan
- Emotional Brain Institute, Nathan S. Kline Institute, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, New York University Langone Medical Center
| | | |
Collapse
|
122
|
MacDonald K, Berlow R, Thomas ML. Attachment, affective temperament, and personality disorders: a study of their relationships in psychiatric outpatients. J Affect Disord 2013; 151:932-41. [PMID: 24054918 DOI: 10.1016/j.jad.2013.07.040] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/18/2012] [Revised: 02/25/2013] [Accepted: 07/31/2013] [Indexed: 12/14/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND As the result of extensive translational and cross-disciplinary research, attachment theory is now a construct with significant neuropsychiatric traction. The correlation of attachment with other influential conceptual models (i.e. temperament and personality) is therefore of interest. Consequently, we explored how two attachment dimensions (attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance) correlated with measures of temperament and personality in 357 psychiatric outpatients. METHODS We performed a retrospective review of four questionnaires (the Experiences in Close Relationship scale (ECR-R), Temperament and Character inventory (TCI), Temperament Evaluation of the Memphis, Pisa, Paris and San Diego questionnaire (TEMPS-A), and Personality Self-Portrait Questionnaire (PSQ)). Frequency measures and correlations were examined, as was the predictive value of attachment security for a personality disorder (PD). RESULTS Significant, robust correlations were found between attachment anxiety and (1) several negative affective temperaments (dysthymic and cyclothymic); (2) several indices of personality pathology (low self-directedness (TCI), DSM-IV paranoid, borderline, histrionic, avoidant and dependent personality traits). Attachment avoidance had fewer large correlations. In an exploratory model, the negative predictive value of attachment security for a PD was 86%. LIMITATIONS Subjects were a relatively homogeneous subset of ambulatory psychiatric outpatients. PD diagnoses were via self-report. CONCLUSIONS Clinically, these findings highlight the significant overlap between attachment, affective temperament, and personality and support the value of attachment as a screen for PDs. More broadly, given our growing understanding of the neurobiology of attachment (i.e. links with the oxytocin system), these results raise interesting questions about underlying biological systems and psychiatric treatment.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kai MacDonald
- University of California Medical Center, Department of Psychiatry, USA.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
123
|
Howlett JR, Paulus MP. Decision-Making Dysfunctions of Counterfactuals in Depression: Who Might I have Been? Front Psychiatry 2013; 4:143. [PMID: 24265620 PMCID: PMC3820979 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00143] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/23/2013] [Accepted: 10/14/2013] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Cognitive neuroscience enables us now to decompose major depressive disorder into dysfunctional component processes and relate these processes to specific neural substrates. This approach can be used to illuminate the biological basis of altered psychological processes in depression, including abnormal decision-making. One important decision-related process is counterfactual thinking, or the comparison of reality to hypothetical alternatives. Evidence suggests that individuals with depression experience exaggerated emotional responses due to focusing on counterfactual decision outcomes in general and regret, i.e., the emotion associated with focus on an alternative superior outcome, in particular. Regret is linked to self-esteem in that it involves the evaluation of an individual's own decisions. Alterations of self-esteem, in turn, are a hallmark of depression. The literature on the behavioral and neural processes underlying counterfactual thinking, self-esteem, and depression is selectively reviewed. A model is proposed in which unstable self-representation in depression is more strongly perturbed when a different choice would have produced a better outcome, leading to increased feelings of regret. This approach may help unify diverse aspects of depression, can generate testable predictions, and may suggest new treatment avenues targeting distorted counterfactual cognitions, attentional biases toward superior counterfactual outcomes, or increased affective response to regretted outcomes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jonathon R. Howlett
- Laboratory of Biological Dynamics and Theoretical Medicine, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA
- Department of Psychiatry, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA
- Veterans Affairs San Diego Healthcare System, San Diego, CA, USA
| | - Martin P. Paulus
- Laboratory of Biological Dynamics and Theoretical Medicine, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA
- Department of Psychiatry, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA
- Veterans Affairs San Diego Healthcare System, San Diego, CA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
124
|
Jankowiak-Siuda K, Zajkowski W. A neural model of mechanisms of empathy deficits in narcissism. Med Sci Monit 2013; 19:934-41. [PMID: 24189465 PMCID: PMC3829700 DOI: 10.12659/msm.889593] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/21/2022] Open
Abstract
From a multidimensional perspective, empathy is a process that includes affective sharing and imagining and understanding the emotions of others. The primary brain structures involved in mediating the components of empathy are the anterior insula (AI), the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), and specific regions of the medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC). The AI and ACC are the main nodes in the salience network (SN), which selects and coordinates the information flow from the intero- and exteroreceptors. AI might play a role as a crucial hub – a dynamic switch between 2 separate networks of cognitive processing: the central executive network (CEN), which is concerned with effective task execution, and the default mode network (DMN), which is involved with self-reflective processes. Given various classifications, a deficit in empathy may be considered a central dysfunctional trait in narcissism. A recent fMRI study suggests that deficit in empathy is due to a dysfunction in the right AI. Based on the acquired data, we propose a theoretical model of imbalanced SN functioning in narcissism in which the dysfunctional AI hub is responsible for constant DMN activation, which, in turn, centers one’s attention on the self. This might hinder the ability to affectively share and understand the emotions of others. This review paper on neural mechanisms of empathy deficits in narcissism aims to inspire and direct future research in this area.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kamila Jankowiak-Siuda
- Department of Experimental Neuropsychology, University of Social Sciences and Humanities, Warsaw, Poland
| | | |
Collapse
|
125
|
Gray matter abnormalities in patients with narcissistic personality disorder. J Psychiatr Res 2013; 47:1363-9. [PMID: 23777939 DOI: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.05.017] [Citation(s) in RCA: 44] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/07/2013] [Revised: 04/04/2013] [Accepted: 05/17/2013] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Despite the relevance of narcissistic personality disorder (NPD) in clinical settings, there is currently no empirical data available regarding the neurobiological correlates of NPD. In the present study, we performed a voxel-based morphometric analysis to provide initial insight into local abnormalities of gray matter (GM) volume. METHODS Structural brain images were obtained from patients with NPD (n = 17) and a sample of healthy controls (n = 17) matched regarding age, gender, handedness, and intelligence. Groups were compared with regard to global brain tissue volumes and local abnormalities of GM volume. Regions-of-interest analyses were calculated for the anterior insula. RESULTS Relative to the control group, NPD patients had smaller GM volume in the left anterior insula. Independent of group, GM volume in the left anterior insula was positively related to self-reported emotional empathy. Complementary whole-brain analyses yielded smaller GM volume in fronto-paralimbic brain regions comprising the rostral and median cingulate cortex as well as dorsolateral and medial parts of the prefrontal cortex. CONCLUSION Here we provide the first empirical evidence for structural abnormalities in fronto-paralimbic brain regions of patients with NPD. The results are discussed in the context of NPD patients' restricted ability for emotional empathy.
Collapse
|
126
|
Dedovic K, Duchesne A, Engert V, Lue SD, Andrews J, Efanov SI, Beaudry T, Pruessner JC. Psychological, endocrine and neural responses to social evaluation in subclinical depression. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2013; 9:1632-44. [PMID: 24078020 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nst151] [Citation(s) in RCA: 32] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/19/2023] Open
Abstract
This study aimed to identify vulnerability patterns in psychological, physiological and neural responses to mild psychosocial challenge in a population that is at a direct risk of developing depression, but who has not as yet succumbed to the full clinical syndrome. A group of healthy and a group of subclinically depressed participants underwent a modified Montreal Imaging Stress task (MIST), a mild neuroimaging psychosocial task and completed state self-esteem and mood measures. Cortisol levels were assessed throughout the session. All participants showed a decrease in performance self-esteem levels following the MIST. Yet, the decline in performance self-esteem levels was associated with increased levels of anxiety and confusion in the healthy group, but increased levels of depression in the subclinical group, following the MIST. The subclinical group showed overall lower cortisol levels compared with the healthy group. The degree of change in activity in the subgenual anterior cingulate cortex in response to negative evaluation was associated with increased levels of depression in the whole sample. Findings suggest that even in response to a mild psychosocial challenge, those individuals vulnerable to depression already show important maladaptive response patterns at psychological and neural levels. The findings point to important targets for future interventions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Katarina Dedovic
- Social and Affective Neuroscience Laboratory, Psychology Department, University of California, Los Angeles, CA 90095, USA, Douglas Mental Health University Institute, Integrated Program in Neuroscience, Department of Psychiatry, McGill University, Montreal, QC H4H 1R2, Canada, Maxplanck Institute, 04103 Leipzig, Germany, American School of professional Psychology, Washington, DC 22209, USA, and McGill Centre for Studies in Aging, Faculty of Medicine, McGill University, Montreal, QC H4H 1R2, Canada
| | - Annie Duchesne
- Social and Affective Neuroscience Laboratory, Psychology Department, University of California, Los Angeles, CA 90095, USA, Douglas Mental Health University Institute, Integrated Program in Neuroscience, Department of Psychiatry, McGill University, Montreal, QC H4H 1R2, Canada, Maxplanck Institute, 04103 Leipzig, Germany, American School of professional Psychology, Washington, DC 22209, USA, and McGill Centre for Studies in Aging, Faculty of Medicine, McGill University, Montreal, QC H4H 1R2, Canada
| | - Veronika Engert
- Social and Affective Neuroscience Laboratory, Psychology Department, University of California, Los Angeles, CA 90095, USA, Douglas Mental Health University Institute, Integrated Program in Neuroscience, Department of Psychiatry, McGill University, Montreal, QC H4H 1R2, Canada, Maxplanck Institute, 04103 Leipzig, Germany, American School of professional Psychology, Washington, DC 22209, USA, and McGill Centre for Studies in Aging, Faculty of Medicine, McGill University, Montreal, QC H4H 1R2, Canada
| | - Sonja Damika Lue
- Social and Affective Neuroscience Laboratory, Psychology Department, University of California, Los Angeles, CA 90095, USA, Douglas Mental Health University Institute, Integrated Program in Neuroscience, Department of Psychiatry, McGill University, Montreal, QC H4H 1R2, Canada, Maxplanck Institute, 04103 Leipzig, Germany, American School of professional Psychology, Washington, DC 22209, USA, and McGill Centre for Studies in Aging, Faculty of Medicine, McGill University, Montreal, QC H4H 1R2, Canada
| | - Julie Andrews
- Social and Affective Neuroscience Laboratory, Psychology Department, University of California, Los Angeles, CA 90095, USA, Douglas Mental Health University Institute, Integrated Program in Neuroscience, Department of Psychiatry, McGill University, Montreal, QC H4H 1R2, Canada, Maxplanck Institute, 04103 Leipzig, Germany, American School of professional Psychology, Washington, DC 22209, USA, and McGill Centre for Studies in Aging, Faculty of Medicine, McGill University, Montreal, QC H4H 1R2, Canada
| | - Simona I Efanov
- Social and Affective Neuroscience Laboratory, Psychology Department, University of California, Los Angeles, CA 90095, USA, Douglas Mental Health University Institute, Integrated Program in Neuroscience, Department of Psychiatry, McGill University, Montreal, QC H4H 1R2, Canada, Maxplanck Institute, 04103 Leipzig, Germany, American School of professional Psychology, Washington, DC 22209, USA, and McGill Centre for Studies in Aging, Faculty of Medicine, McGill University, Montreal, QC H4H 1R2, Canada
| | - Thomas Beaudry
- Social and Affective Neuroscience Laboratory, Psychology Department, University of California, Los Angeles, CA 90095, USA, Douglas Mental Health University Institute, Integrated Program in Neuroscience, Department of Psychiatry, McGill University, Montreal, QC H4H 1R2, Canada, Maxplanck Institute, 04103 Leipzig, Germany, American School of professional Psychology, Washington, DC 22209, USA, and McGill Centre for Studies in Aging, Faculty of Medicine, McGill University, Montreal, QC H4H 1R2, Canada
| | - Jens C Pruessner
- Social and Affective Neuroscience Laboratory, Psychology Department, University of California, Los Angeles, CA 90095, USA, Douglas Mental Health University Institute, Integrated Program in Neuroscience, Department of Psychiatry, McGill University, Montreal, QC H4H 1R2, Canada, Maxplanck Institute, 04103 Leipzig, Germany, American School of professional Psychology, Washington, DC 22209, USA, and McGill Centre for Studies in Aging, Faculty of Medicine, McGill University, Montreal, QC H4H 1R2, Canada Social and Affective Neuroscience Laboratory, Psychology Department, University of California, Los Angeles, CA 90095, USA, Douglas Mental Health University Institute, Integrated Program in Neuroscience, Department of Psychiatry, McGill University, Montreal, QC H4H 1R2, Canada, Maxplanck Institute, 04103 Leipzig, Germany, American School of professional Psychology, Washington, DC 22209, USA, and McGill Centre for Studies in Aging, Faculty of Medicine, McGill University, Montreal, QC H4H 1R2, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
127
|
Kawamichi H, Sasaki AT, Matsunaga M, Yoshihara K, Takahashi HK, Tanabe HC, Sadato N. Medial prefrontal cortex activation is commonly invoked by reputation of self and romantic partners. PLoS One 2013; 8:e74958. [PMID: 24086409 PMCID: PMC3781163 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0074958] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/27/2013] [Accepted: 08/08/2013] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
The reputation of others influences partner selection in human cooperative behaviors through verbal reputation representation. Although the way in which humans represent the verbal reputations of others is a pivotal issue for social neuroscience, the neural correlates underlying the representation of verbal reputations of others are unclear. Humans primarily depend on self-evaluation when assessing reputation of self. Likewise, humans might primarily depend on self-evaluation of others when representing their reputation. As interaction promotes the formation of more nuanced, individualized impressions of an interaction partner, humans tend to form self-evaluations of persons with whom they are intimate in their daily life. Thus, we hypothesized that the representation of reputation of others is modulated by intimacy due to one's own evaluation formation of that person. To test this hypothesis, we conducted a functional magnetic resonance imaging experiment with 11 pairs of romantic partners while they viewed an evaluation of a target person (self, partner [intimate other], or stranger [non-intimate other]), made by other evaluators. When compared with strangers, viewing evaluations of self and partner activated overlapping regions in the medial prefrontal cortex. Verbal reputation of self-specific activation was found in the precuneus, which represents self-related processing. The data suggest that midline structures represent reputation of self. In addition, intimacy-modulated activation in the medial prefrontal cortex suggests that the verbal reputation of intimate others is represented similarly to reputation of self. These results suggest that the reputation representation in the medial prefrontal cortex is engaged by verbal reputation of self and intimate others stemming from both own and other evaluators' judgments.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hiroaki Kawamichi
- Department of Cerebral Research, National Institute for Physiological Sciences, Okazaki, Aichi, Japan
- * E-mail:
| | - Akihiro T. Sasaki
- Department of Physiology, Osaka City University Graduate School of Medicine, Osaka, Osaka, Japan
- Pathophysiological and Health Science Team, RIKEN Center for Life Science Technologies, Kobe, Hyogo, Japan
| | - Masahiro Matsunaga
- Department of Health and Psychosocial Medicine, Aichi Medical University School of Medicine, Nagakute, Aichi, Japan
| | - Kazufumi Yoshihara
- Department of Psychosomatic Medicine, Graduate School of Medical Sciences, Kyushu University, Fukuoka, Fukuoka, Japan
| | - Haruka K. Takahashi
- Department of Cerebral Research, National Institute for Physiological Sciences, Okazaki, Aichi, Japan
- Department of Physiological Sciences, The Graduate University for Advanced Studies (Sokendai), Okazaki, Aichi, Japan
| | - Hiroki C. Tanabe
- Department of Cerebral Research, National Institute for Physiological Sciences, Okazaki, Aichi, Japan
- Department of Social and Human Environment, Graduate School of Environmental Studies, Nagoya University, Nagoya, Aichi, Japan
| | - Norihiro Sadato
- Department of Cerebral Research, National Institute for Physiological Sciences, Okazaki, Aichi, Japan
- Department of Physiological Sciences, The Graduate University for Advanced Studies (Sokendai), Okazaki, Aichi, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
128
|
Hostinar CE, Sullivan RM, Gunnar MR. Psychobiological mechanisms underlying the social buffering of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical axis: a review of animal models and human studies across development. Psychol Bull 2013; 140:256-82. [PMID: 23607429 DOI: 10.1037/a0032671] [Citation(s) in RCA: 472] [Impact Index Per Article: 39.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/25/2022]
Abstract
Discovering the stress-buffering effects of social relationships has been one of the major findings in psychobiology in the last century. However, an understanding of the underlying neurobiological and psychological mechanisms of this buffering is only beginning to emerge. An important avenue of this research concerns the neurocircuitry that can regulate the activity of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical (HPA) axis. The present review is a translational effort aimed at integrating animal models and human studies of the social regulation of the HPA axis from infancy to adulthood, specifically focusing on the process that has been named social buffering. This process has been noted across species and consists of a dampened HPA axis stress response to threat or challenge that occurs with the presence or assistance of a conspecific. We describe aspects of the relevant underlying neurobiology when enough information exists and expose major gaps in our understanding across all domains of the literatures we aimed to integrate. We provide a working conceptual model focused on the role of oxytocinergic systems and prefrontal neural networks as 2 of the putative biological mediators of this process, and propose that the role of early experiences is critical in shaping later social buffering effects. This synthesis points to both general future directions and specific experiments that need to be conducted to build a more comprehensive model of the HPA social buffering effect across the life span that incorporates multiple levels of analysis: neuroendocrine, behavioral, and social.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Regina M Sullivan
- Emotional Brain Institute, Nathan S. Kline Institute, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, New York University Langone Medical Center
| | | |
Collapse
|
129
|
Hughes BL, Beer JS. Protecting the Self: The Effect of Social-evaluative Threat on Neural Representations of Self. J Cogn Neurosci 2013; 25:613-22. [DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_00343] [Citation(s) in RCA: 68] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/18/2022]
Abstract
Abstract
One of the most robust ways that people protect themselves from social-evaluative threat is by emphasizing the desirability of their personal characteristics, yet the neural underpinnings of this fundamental process are unknown. The current fMRI study addresses this question by examining self-evaluations of desirability (in comparison with other people) as a response to threat. Participants judged how much personality traits described themselves in comparison with their average peer. These judgments were preceded by threatening or nonthreatening social-evaluative feedback. Self-evaluations made in response to threat significantly increased activation in a number of regions including the OFC, medial pFC, lateral pFC, amygdala, and insula. Individual differences in the extent to which threat increased desirability were significantly correlated with medial OFC activity. This is the first study to examine the neural associations of a fundamental self-protection strategy: responding to threat by emphasizing the self's desirability. Although neural research has separately examined self-evaluation processes from the regulation of social-evaluative threat, little is known about the interplay between the two. The findings build on this previous research by showing that regions, often associated with self-evaluation, are modulated by the degree to which people respond to threat by emphasizing their own desirability.
Collapse
|
130
|
Eisenberger NI. Social ties and health: a social neuroscience perspective. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2013; 23:407-13. [PMID: 23395461 DOI: 10.1016/j.conb.2013.01.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 39] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/20/2012] [Revised: 01/07/2013] [Accepted: 01/13/2013] [Indexed: 12/30/2022]
Abstract
Research over the last several decades has shown that the health of the body is intimately tied to the strength of our social connections, but why? This article reviews evidence from affective and social neuroscience suggesting that, because of the importance of social ties for mammalian survival, threats to social connection are processed by some of the same neural regions that process basic threats to survival and consequently trigger physiological threat responses that have negative health implications. Likewise, social support is processed by some of the same neural regions that process safety or protection from basic threats and inhibit these same health-relevant physiological threat responses.
Collapse
|
131
|
Abstract
Receiving social feedback such as praise or blame for one's character traits is a key component of everyday human interactions. It has been proposed that humans are positively biased when integrating social feedback into their self-concept. However, a mechanistic description of how humans process self-relevant feedback is lacking. Here, participants received feedback from peers after a real-life interaction. Participants processed feedback in a positively biased way, i.e., they changed their self-evaluations more toward desirable than toward undesirable feedback. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging we investigated two feedback components. First, the reward-related component correlated with activity in ventral striatum and in anterior cingulate cortex/medial prefrontal cortex (ACC/MPFC). Second, the comparison-related component correlated with activity in the mentalizing network, including the MPFC, the temporoparietal junction, the superior temporal sulcus, the temporal pole, and the inferior frontal gyrus. This comparison-related activity within the mentalizing system has a parsimonious interpretation, i.e., activity correlated with the differences between participants' own evaluation and feedback. Importantly, activity within the MPFC that integrated reward-related and comparison-related components predicted the self-related positive updating bias across participants offering a mechanistic account of positively biased feedback processing. Thus, theories on both reward and mentalizing are important for a better understanding of how social information is integrated into the human self-concept.
Collapse
|
132
|
Muscatell KA, Eisenberger NI. A Social Neuroscience Perspective on Stress and Health. SOCIAL AND PERSONALITY PSYCHOLOGY COMPASS 2012; 6:890-904. [PMID: 23227112 DOI: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2012.00467.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 63] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/30/2022]
Abstract
Psychological stress is a major risk factor for the development and progression of a number of diseases, including cardiovascular disease, cancer, arthritis, and major depression. A growing body of research suggests that long-term, stress-induced activation of the sympathetic nervous system (SNS) and the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis may lead to increases in inflammation, which is known to play a key role in the pathophysiology of a variety of diseases. Furthermore, the burgeoning fields of social neuroscience and health neuroscience have begun to identify the neurocognitive mechanisms by which stress may lead to these physiological changes. Here we review the literature examining the neurocognitive correlates of stress-induced SNS, HPA, and inflammatory responses. Specifically, we summarize the results of neuroimaging studies that have examined the neural correlates of stress-related increases in SNS, HPA, and inflammatory activity. A set of neural systems involved in threat processing, safety processing, and social cognition are suggested as key contributors to stress-related changes in physiology. We conclude by offering suggestions for future research in the exciting new field of health neuroscience.
Collapse
|
133
|
Kawamoto T, Onoda K, Nakashima K, Nittono H, Yamaguchi S, Ura M. Is dorsal anterior cingulate cortex activation in response to social exclusion due to expectancy violation? An fMRI study. FRONTIERS IN EVOLUTIONARY NEUROSCIENCE 2012; 4:11. [PMID: 22866035 PMCID: PMC3406317 DOI: 10.3389/fnevo.2012.00011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 53] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/10/2012] [Accepted: 07/11/2012] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
People are typically quite sensitive about being accepted or excluded by others. Previous studies have suggested that the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) is a key brain region involved in the detection of social exclusion. However, this region has also been shown to be sensitive to non-social expectancy violations. We often expect other people to follow an unwritten rule in which they include us as they would expect to be included, such that social exclusion likely involves some degree of expectancy violation. The present event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study sought to separate the effects of expectancy violation from those of social exclusion, such that we employed an “overinclusion” condition in which a player was unexpectedly overincluded in the game by the other players. With this modification, we found that the dACC and right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (rVLPFC) were activated by exclusion, relative to overinclusion. In addition, we identified a negative correlation between exclusion-evoked brain activity and self-rated social pain in the rVLPFC, but not in the dACC. These findings suggest that the rVLPFC is critical for regulating social pain, whereas the dACC plays an important role in the detection of exclusion. The neurobiological basis of social exclusion is different from that of mere expectancy violation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Taishi Kawamoto
- Department of Behavioral Sciences, Graduate School of Integrated Arts and Sciences, Hiroshima University Higashi-Hiroshima, Japan
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
134
|
Gender differences of brain activity in the conflicts based on implicit self-esteem. PLoS One 2012; 7:e37901. [PMID: 22666409 PMCID: PMC3364282 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0037901] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/22/2011] [Accepted: 04/27/2012] [Indexed: 12/12/2022] Open
Abstract
There are gender differences in global and domain-specific self-esteem and the incidence of some psychiatric disorders related to self-esteem, suggesting that there are gender differences in the neural basis underlying one's own self-esteem. We investigated gender differences in the brain activity while subjects (14 males and 12 females) performed an implicit self-esteem task, using fMRI. While ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) was significantly activated in females, medial and dorsomedial PFC (dmPFC) were activated in males in the incongruent condition (self = negative) compared with the congruent condition (self = positive). Additionally, scores on the explicit self-esteem test were negatively correlated with vmPFC activity in females and positively correlated with dmPFC activity in males. Furthermore, the functional relationships among the regions found by direct gender comparisons were discussed based on the somatic-marker model. These showed that, compared to males, females more firmly store even the incongruent associations as part of their schematic self-knowledge, and such associations automatically activate the neural networks for emotional response and control, in which vmPFC plays a central role. This may explain female cognitive/behavioral traits; females have more tendency to ruminate more often than males, which sometimes results in a prolonged negative affect.
Collapse
|
135
|
The pain of social disconnection: examining the shared neural underpinnings of physical and social pain. Nat Rev Neurosci 2012; 13:421-34. [PMID: 22551663 DOI: 10.1038/nrn3231] [Citation(s) in RCA: 448] [Impact Index Per Article: 34.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/14/2022]
|
136
|
Eisenberger NI, Cole SW. Social neuroscience and health: neurophysiological mechanisms linking social ties with physical health. Nat Neurosci 2012. [DOI: 10.1038/nn.3086 nn.3086 [pii]] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/30/2022]
|
137
|
Eisenberger NI, Cole SW. Social neuroscience and health: neurophysiological mechanisms linking social ties with physical health. Nat Neurosci 2012; 15:669-74. [DOI: 10.1038/nn.3086] [Citation(s) in RCA: 298] [Impact Index Per Article: 22.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/20/2022]
|
138
|
Vandellen M, Knowles ML, Krusemark E, Sabet RF, Campbell WK, McDowell JE, Clementz BA. Trait Self-esteem Moderates Decreases in Self-control Following Rejection: An Information-processing Account. EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF PERSONALITY 2012; 26:123-132. [PMID: 22611304 DOI: 10.1002/per.1845] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
Abstract
In the current paper, the authors posit that trait self-esteem moderates the relationship between social rejection and decrements in self-control, propose an information-processing account of trait self-esteem's moderating influence and discuss three tests of this theory. The authors measured trait self-esteem, experimentally manipulated social rejection and assessed subsequent self-control in Studies 1 and 2. Additionally, Study 3 framed a self-control task as diagnostic of social skills to examine motivational influences. Together, the results reveal that rejection impairs self-control, but only among low self-esteem individuals. Moreover, this decrement in self-control only emerged when the task had no social implications-suggesting that low self-esteem individuals exert effort on tasks of social value and are otherwise preoccupied with belonging needs when completing nonsocial tasks.
Collapse
|
139
|
Yang J, Dedovic K, Chen W, Zhang Q. Self-esteem modulates dorsal anterior cingulate cortical response in self-referential processing. Neuropsychologia 2012; 50:1267-70. [PMID: 22391476 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.02.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/11/2011] [Revised: 02/06/2012] [Accepted: 02/13/2012] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
Self-esteem can be defined as evaluations that individuals make about their worth as human beings. These evaluations are in part based on how we evaluate ourselves on our abilities, values, opinions, etc. compared with others or our past or ideal self; and they are also influenced by a thought that what others may think about us. Studies to date investigating the neural mechanisms underlying individual differences in self-esteem have focused mostly on the latter process (i.e. on how self-esteem is associated with neural correlates of processing feedback from others). However, given that people spend a lot of time thinking about themselves and evaluating their worth, we aimed to investigate neural mechanism underlying the association between levels of self-esteem and processing of self-relevant information. Seventeen participants completed a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) scan during which they were asked to evaluate whether a given statement is true about them (Self), an acquaintance of theirs (Other), or about general knowledge (Semantic). A whole brain correlational analysis revealed a significant negative correlation between levels of self-esteem and changes in activation of dorsal anterior cingulate gyrus (dACC, BA32) in response to evaluating self-relevant information (Self versus Other contrast). This result extends previous findings implicating this region in the association between processing evaluative feedback and levels of self-esteem and suggests that activity in this region is affected by self-esteem levels also when individuals are engaged in self-referencing and self-evaluation. Future studies should investigate whether these associations are affected differently based on valence of self-evaluations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Juan Yang
- Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality, Ministry of Education, Southwest University, Chongqing 400715, China.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
140
|
Abstract
Experiences of social rejection or loss have been described as some of the most "painful" experiences that we, as humans, face and perhaps for good reason. Because of our prolonged period of immaturity, the social attachment system may have co-opted the pain system, borrowing the pain signal to prevent the detrimental consequences of social separation. This review summarizes a program of research that has explored the idea that experiences of physical pain and social pain rely on shared neural substrates. First, evidence showing that social pain activates pain-related neural regions is reviewed. Then, studies exploring some of the expected consequences of such a physical pain-social pain overlap are summarized. These studies demonstrate that a) individuals who are more sensitive to one kind of pain are also more sensitive to the other and b) factors that increase or decrease one kind of pain alter the other in a similar manner. Finally, what these shared neural substrates mean for our understanding of socially painful experience is discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Naomi I Eisenberger
- Department of Psychology, 1285 Franz Hall, University of California, Los Angeles, CA 90095-1563, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
141
|
This time with motivation: The implications of social neuroscience for research on motivated self- and other-perception (and vice versa). MOTIVATION AND EMOTION 2011. [DOI: 10.1007/s11031-011-9259-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
|