151
|
Shinskey JL, Chan CHM, Coleman R, Moxom L, Yamamoto E. Preschoolers’ nonsymbolic arithmetic with large sets: Is addition more accurate than subtraction? J Exp Child Psychol 2009; 103:409-20. [PMID: 19285685 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2009.01.012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/31/2008] [Revised: 12/03/2008] [Accepted: 01/27/2009] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Affiliation(s)
- Jeanne L Shinskey
- Department of Psychology, Royal Holloway, University of London, Surrey, UK.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
152
|
Jenks KM, de Moor J, van Lieshout ECDM. Arithmetic difficulties in children with cerebral palsy are related to executive function and working memory. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2009; 50:824-33. [PMID: 19486225 DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.02031.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 63] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Although it is believed that children with cerebral palsy are at high risk for learning difficulties and arithmetic difficulties in particular, few studies have investigated this issue. METHODS Arithmetic ability was longitudinally assessed in children with cerebral palsy in special (n = 41) and mainstream education (n = 16) and controls in mainstream education (n = 16). Second grade executive function and working memory scores were used to predict third grade arithmetic accuracy and response time. RESULTS Children with cerebral palsy in special education were less accurate and slower than their peers on all arithmetic tests, even after controlling for IQ, whereas children with cerebral palsy in mainstream education performed as well as controls. Although the performance gap became smaller over time, it did not disappear. Children with cerebral palsy in special education showed evidence of executive function and working memory deficits in shifting, updating, visuospatial sketchpad and phonological loop (for digits, not words) whereas children with cerebral palsy in mainstream education only had a deficit in visuospatial sketchpad. Hierarchical regression revealed that, after controlling for intelligence, components of executive function and working memory explained large proportions of unique variance in arithmetic accuracy and response time and these variables were sufficient to explain group differences in simple, but not complex, arithmetic. CONCLUSIONS Children with cerebral palsy are at risk for specific executive function and working memory deficits that, when present, increase the risk for arithmetic difficulties in these children.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kathleen M Jenks
- Behavioral Science Institute, Radboud University Nijmegen, P.O. Box 9104, Nijmegen 6500 HE, The Netherlands.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
153
|
Sarnecka BW, Lee MD. Levels of number knowledge during early childhood. J Exp Child Psychol 2009; 103:325-37. [PMID: 19345956 PMCID: PMC3127737 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2009.02.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 82] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/06/2008] [Revised: 02/06/2009] [Accepted: 02/27/2009] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
Researchers have long disagreed about whether number concepts are essentially continuous (unchanging) or discontinuous over development. Among those who take the discontinuity position, there is disagreement about how development proceeds. The current study addressed these questions with new quantitative analyses of children's incorrect responses on the Give-N task. Using data from 280 children, ages 2 to 4 years, this study showed that most wrong answers were simply guesses, not counting or estimation errors. Their mean was unrelated to the target number, and they were lower-bounded by the numbers children actually knew. In addition, children learned the number-word meanings one at a time and in order; they treated the number words as mutually exclusive; and once they figured out the cardinal principle of counting, they generalized this principle to the rest of their count list. Findings support the 'discontinuity' account of number development in general and the 'knower-levels' account in particular.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Barbara W Sarnecka
- Department of Cognitive Sciences, University of California, Irvine, CA 92697, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
154
|
Barth H, Starr A, Sullivan J. Children's mappings of large number words to numerosities. COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT 2009. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2009.04.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 45] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
|
155
|
Dynamic representations underlying symbolic and nonsymbolic calculation: Evidence from the operational momentum effect. Atten Percept Psychophys 2009; 71:803-21. [DOI: 10.3758/app.71.4.803] [Citation(s) in RCA: 127] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
|
156
|
Jenks KM, van Lieshout ECDM, de Moor J. The relationship between medical impairments and arithmetic development in children with cerebral palsy. J Child Neurol 2009; 24:528-35. [PMID: 19406754 DOI: 10.1177/0883073809335009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
Arithmetic ability was tested in children with cerebral palsy without severe intellectual impairment (verbal IQ >or= 70) attending special (n = 41) or mainstream education (n = 16) as well as control children in mainstream education (n = 16) throughout first and second grade. Children with cerebral palsy in special education did not appear to have fully automatized arithmetic facts by the end of second grade. Their lower accuracy and consistently slower (verbal) response times raise important concerns for their future arithmetic development. Differences in arithmetic performance between children with cerebral palsy in special or mainstream education were not related to localization of cerebral palsy or to gross motor impairment. Rather, lower accuracy and slower verbal responses were related to differences in nonverbal intelligence and the presence of epilepsy. Left-hand impairment was related to slower verbal responses but not to lower accuracy.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kathleen M Jenks
- Behavioral Science Institute, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
157
|
Abstract
Mathematicians frequently evoke their "intuition" when they are able to quickly and automatically solve a problem, with little introspection into their insight. Cognitive neuroscience research shows that mathematical intuition is a valid concept that can be studied in the laboratory in reduced paradigms, and that relates to the availability of "core knowledge" associated with evolutionarily ancient and specialized cerebral subsystems. As an illustration, I discuss the case of elementary arithmetic. Intuitions of numbers and their elementary transformations by addition and subtraction are present in all human cultures. They relate to a brain system, located in the intraparietal sulcus of both hemispheres, which extracts numerosity of sets and, in educated adults, maps back and forth between numerical symbols and the corresponding quantities. This system is available to animal species and to preverbal human infants. Its neuronal organization is increasingly being uncovered, leading to a precise mathematical theory of how we perform tasks of number comparison or number naming. The next challenge will be to understand how education changes our core intuitions of number.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Stanislas Dehaene
- INSERM, Cognitive Neuro-imaging Unit, IFR 49, Gif sur Yvette, France.
| |
Collapse
|
158
|
vanMarle K, Wynn K. Infants' auditory enumeration: evidence for analog magnitudes in the small number range. Cognition 2009; 111:302-16. [PMID: 19324330 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.01.011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 40] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/26/2007] [Revised: 01/13/2009] [Accepted: 01/30/2009] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
Vigorous debate surrounds the issue of whether infants use different representational mechanisms to discriminate small and large numbers. We report evidence for ratio-dependent performance in infants' discrimination of small numbers of auditory events, suggesting that infants can use analog magnitudes to represent small values, at least in the auditory domain. Seven-month-old infants in the present study reliably discriminated two from four tones (a 1:2 ratio) in Experiment 1, when melodic and continuous temporal properties of the sequences were controlled, but failed to discriminate two from three tones (a 2:3 ratio) under the same conditions in Experiment 2. A third experiment ruled out the possibility that infants in Experiment 1 were responding to greater melodic variety in the four-tone sequences. The discrimination function obtained here is the same as that found for infants' discrimination of large numbers of visual and auditory items at a similar age, as well as for that obtained for similar-aged infants' duration discriminations, and thus adds to a growing body of evidence suggesting that human infants may share with adults and nonhuman animals a mechanism for representing quantities as "noisy" mental magnitudes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kristy vanMarle
- Department of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, CT 06520, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
159
|
Abstract
Many experiments with infants suggest that they possess quantitative abilities, and many experimentalists believe that these abilities set the stage for later mathematics: natural numbers and arithmetic. However, the connection between these early and later skills is far from obvious. We evaluate two possible routes to mathematics and argue that neither is sufficient: (1) We first sketch what we think is the most likely model for infant abilities in this domain, and we examine proposals for extrapolating the natural number concept from these beginnings. Proposals for arriving at natural number by (empirical) induction presuppose the mathematical concepts they seek to explain. Moreover, standard experimental tests for children's understanding of number terms do not necessarily tap these concepts. (2) True concepts of number do appear, however, when children are able to understand generalizations over all numbers; for example, the principle of additive commutativity (a+b=b+a). Theories of how children learn such principles usually rely on a process of mapping from physical object groupings. But both experimental results and theoretical considerations imply that direct mapping is insufficient for acquiring these principles. We suggest instead that children may arrive at natural numbers and arithmetic in a more top-down way, by constructing mathematical schemas.
Collapse
|
160
|
Li J, Liu J, Liang J, Zhang H, Zhao J, Huber DE, Rieth CA, Lee K, Tian J, Shi G. A distributed neural system for top-down face processing. Neurosci Lett 2009; 451:6-10. [PMID: 19121364 PMCID: PMC2634849 DOI: 10.1016/j.neulet.2008.12.039] [Citation(s) in RCA: 40] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/11/2008] [Revised: 12/17/2008] [Accepted: 12/19/2008] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
Evidence suggests that the neural system associated with face processing is a distributed cortical network containing both bottom-up and top-down mechanisms. While bottom-up face processing has been the focus of many studies, the neural areas involved in the top-down face processing have not been extensively investigated due to difficulty in isolating top-down influences from the bottom-up response engendered by presentation of a face. In the present study, we used a novel experimental method to induce illusory face-detection. This method allowed for directly examining the neural systems involved in top-down face processing while minimizing the influence of bottom-up perceptual input. A distributed cortical network of top-down face processing was identified by analyzing the functional connectivity patterns of the right fusiform face area (FFA). This distributed cortical network model for face processing includes both "core" and "extended" face processing areas. It also includes left anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), bilateral orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), left premotor cortex, and left inferior parietal cortex. These findings suggest that top-down face processing contains not only regions for analyzing the visual appearance of faces, but also those involved in processing low spatial frequency (LSF) information, decision-making, and working memory.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jun Li
- Life Science Research Center, School of Electronic Engineering, Xidian University, Xi’an, Shaanxi 710071, China
| | - Jiangang Liu
- School of Computer and Information Technology, Beijing Jiaotong University, Beijing 1000441, China
| | - Jimin Liang
- Life Science Research Center, School of Electronic Engineering, Xidian University, Xi’an, Shaanxi 710071, China
| | - Hongchuan Zhang
- Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory, Northwestern University, Illinois, USA
| | - Jizheng Zhao
- Life Science Research Center, School of Electronic Engineering, Xidian University, Xi’an, Shaanxi 710071, China
| | - David E. Huber
- Department of Psychology, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California, USA
| | - Cory A. Rieth
- Department of Psychology, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California, USA
| | - Kang Lee
- Institute of Child Study, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
| | - Jie Tian
- Life Science Research Center, School of Electronic Engineering, Xidian University, Xi’an, Shaanxi 710071, China
- Institute of Automation, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100190, China
| | - Guangming Shi
- Life Science Research Center, School of Electronic Engineering, Xidian University, Xi’an, Shaanxi 710071, China
| |
Collapse
|
161
|
Emmerton J, Renner JC. Local rather than global processing of visual arrays in numerosity discrimination by pigeons (Columba livia). Anim Cogn 2009; 12:511-26. [DOI: 10.1007/s10071-009-0212-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/10/2008] [Revised: 01/09/2009] [Accepted: 01/09/2009] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
|
162
|
Cantlon JF, Cordes S, Libertus ME, Brannon EM. Comment on "Log or linear? Distinct intuitions of the number scale in Western and Amazonian indigene cultures". Science 2009; 323:38; author reply 38. [PMID: 19119201 PMCID: PMC3393850 DOI: 10.1126/science.1164878] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/01/2023]
Abstract
Dehaene et al. (Reports, 30 May 2008, p. 1217) argued that native speakers of Mundurucu, a language without a linguistic numerical system, inherently represent numerical values as a logarithmically spaced spatial continuum. However, their data do not rule out the alternative conclusion that Mundurucu speakers encode numbers linearly with scalar variability and psychologically construct space-number mappings by analogy.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jessica F Cantlon
- Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, NC 27708, USA.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
163
|
Intuitive numbers guide decisions. JUDGMENT AND DECISION MAKING 2008. [DOI: 10.1017/s1930297500001571] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/04/2023]
Abstract
AbstractMeasuring reaction times to number comparisons is thought to reveal a processing stage in elementary numerical cognition linked to internal, imprecise representations of number magnitudes. These intuitive representations of the mental number line have been demonstrated across species and human development but have been little explored in decision making. This paper develops and tests hypotheses about the influence of such evolutionarily ancient, intuitive numbers on human decisions. We demonstrate that individuals with more precise mental-number-line representations are higher in numeracy (number skills) consistent with previous research with children. Individuals with more precise representations (compared to those with less precise representations) also were more likely to choose larger, later amounts over smaller, immediate amounts, particularly with a larger proportional difference between the two monetary outcomes. In addition, they were more likely to choose an option with a larger proportional but smaller absolute difference compared to those with less precise representations. These results are consistent with intuitive number representations underlying: a) perceived differences between numbers, b) the extent to which proportional differences are weighed in decisions, and, ultimately, c) the valuation of decision options. Human decision processes involving numbers important to health and financial matters may be rooted in elementary, biological processes shared with other species.
Collapse
|
164
|
Delays without mistakes: response time and error distributions in dual-task. PLoS One 2008; 3:e3196. [PMID: 18787706 PMCID: PMC2527526 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0003196] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/20/2008] [Accepted: 08/19/2008] [Indexed: 01/29/2023] Open
Abstract
Background When two tasks are presented within a short interval, a delay in the execution of the second task has been systematically observed. Psychological theorizing has argued that while sensory and motor operations can proceed in parallel, the coordination between these modules establishes a processing bottleneck. This model predicts that the timing but not the characteristics (duration, precision, variability…) of each processing stage are affected by interference. Thus, a critical test to this hypothesis is to explore whether the qualitiy of the decision is unaffected by a concurrent task. Methodology/Principal Findings In number comparison–as in most decision comparison tasks with a scalar measure of the evidence–the extent to which two stimuli can be discriminated is determined by their ratio, referred as the Weber fraction. We investigated performance in a rapid succession of two non-symbolic comparison tasks (number comparison and tone discrimination) in which error rates in both tasks could be manipulated parametrically from chance to almost perfect. We observed that dual-task interference has a massive effect on RT but does not affect the error rates, or the distribution of errors as a function of the evidence. Conclusions/Significance Our results imply that while the decision process itself is delayed during multiple task execution, its workings are unaffected by task interference, providing strong evidence in favor of a sequential model of task execution.
Collapse
|
165
|
Iuculano T, Tang J, Hall CWB, Butterworth B. Core information processing deficits in developmental dyscalculia and low numeracy. Dev Sci 2008; 11:669-80. [PMID: 18801122 DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00716.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 166] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Teresa Iuculano
- Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London, UK
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
166
|
Canobi KH, Bethune NE. Number words in young children’s conceptual and procedural knowledge of addition, subtraction and inversion. Cognition 2008; 108:675-86. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.05.011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/14/2007] [Revised: 05/07/2008] [Accepted: 05/19/2008] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
|
167
|
Jordan KE, Maclean EL, Brannon EM. Monkeys match and tally quantities across senses. Cognition 2008; 108:617-25. [PMID: 18571636 PMCID: PMC3641156 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.05.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 67] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/15/2007] [Revised: 03/12/2008] [Accepted: 05/02/2008] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
We report here that monkeys can actively match the number of sounds they hear to the number of shapes they see and present the first evidence that monkeys sum over sounds and sights. In Experiment 1, two monkeys were trained to choose a simultaneous array of 1-9 squares that numerically matched a sample sequence of shapes or sounds. Monkeys numerically matched across (audio-visual) and within (visual-visual) modalities with equal accuracy and transferred to novel numerical values. In Experiment 2, monkeys presented with sample sequences of randomly ordered shapes or tones were able to choose an array of 2-9 squares that was the numerical sum of the shapes and sounds in the sample sequence. In both experiments, accuracy and reaction time depended on the ratio between the correct numerical match and incorrect choice. These findings suggest monkeys and humans share an abstract numerical code that can be divorced from the modality in which stimuli are first experienced.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kerry E Jordan
- Department of Psychology, Utah State University, Logan, UT 84322, USA.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
168
|
Barth H, Beckmann L, Spelke ES. Nonsymbolic, approximate arithmetic in children: abstract addition prior to instruction. Dev Psychol 2008; 44:1466-77. [PMID: 18793077 PMCID: PMC3489021 DOI: 10.1037/a0013046] [Citation(s) in RCA: 67] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
Do children draw upon abstract representations of number when they perform approximate arithmetic operations? In this study, kindergarten children viewed animations suggesting addition of a sequence of sounds to an array of dots, and they compared the sum to a second dot array that differed from the sum by 1 of 3 ratios. Children performed this task successfully with all the signatures of adults' nonsymbolic number representations: accuracy modulated by the ratio of the sum and the comparison quantity, equal performance for within- and cross-modality tasks and for addition and comparison tasks, and performance superior to that of a matched subtraction task. The findings provide clear evidence for nonsymbolic numerical operations on abstract numerical quantities in children who have not yet been taught formal arithmetic.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hilary Barth
- Department of Psychology, Wesleyan University, Middletown, CT 06459, USA.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
169
|
Gebuis T, Cohen Kadosh R, de Haan E, Henik A. Automatic quantity processing in 5-year olds and adults. Cogn Process 2008; 10:133-42. [PMID: 18607652 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-008-0219-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 61] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/07/2008] [Revised: 05/26/2008] [Accepted: 05/27/2008] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Titia Gebuis
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Helmholtz Institute, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
170
|
Gilmore CK, Spelke ES. Children's understanding of the relationship between addition and subtraction. Cognition 2008; 107:932-45. [PMID: 18281029 PMCID: PMC2705957 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.12.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/27/2006] [Revised: 12/07/2007] [Accepted: 12/24/2007] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
In learning mathematics, children must master fundamental logical relationships, including the inverse relationship between addition and subtraction. At the start of elementary school, children lack generalized understanding of this relationship in the context of exact arithmetic problems: they fail to judge, for example, that 12+9-9 yields 12. Here, we investigate whether preschool children's approximate number knowledge nevertheless supports understanding of this relationship. Five-year-old children were more accurate on approximate large-number arithmetic problems that involved an inverse transformation than those that did not, when problems were presented in either non-symbolic or symbolic form. In contrast they showed no advantage for problems involving an inverse transformation when exact arithmetic was involved. Prior to formal schooling, children therefore show generalized understanding of at least one logical principle of arithmetic. The teaching of mathematics may be enhanced by building on this understanding.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Camilla K Gilmore
- Laboratory for Developmental Studies, Department of Psychology, Harvard University, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
171
|
Ebersbach M, Van Dooren W, Van den Noortgate W, Resing WC. Understanding linear and exponential growth: Searching for the roots in 6- to 9-year-olds. COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT 2008. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2008.01.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
|
172
|
Abstract
This study investigates the ability of 6-month-old infants to attend to the continuous properties of a set of discrete entities. Infants were habituated to dot arrays that were constant in cumulative surface area yet varied in number for small (< 4) or large (> 3) sets. Results revealed that infants detected a 4-fold (but not 3-fold) change in area, regardless of set size. These results are in marked contrast to demonstrations that infants of the same age successfully discriminate a 2- or 3-fold change in number, providing strong counterevidence to the claim that infants use solely nonnumerical, continuous extent variables when discriminating sets. These findings also shed light on the processes involved in tracking continuous variables in infants.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sara Cordes
- Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, Duke University, Box 90999, Durham, NC 27708, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
173
|
Izard V, Dehaene S. Calibrating the mental number line. Cognition 2008; 106:1221-47. [PMID: 17678639 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.06.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 215] [Impact Index Per Article: 12.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/21/2006] [Revised: 05/30/2007] [Accepted: 06/01/2007] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
Human adults are thought to possess two dissociable systems to represent numbers: an approximate quantity system akin to a mental number line, and a verbal system capable of representing numbers exactly. Here, we study the interface between these two systems using an estimation task. Observers were asked to estimate the approximate numerosity of dot arrays. We show that, in the absence of calibration, estimates are largely inaccurate: responses increase monotonically with numerosity, but underestimate the actual numerosity. However, insertion of a few inducer trials, in which participants are explicitly (and sometimes misleadingly) told that a given display contains 30 dots, is sufficient to calibrate their estimates on the whole range of stimuli. Based on these empirical results, we develop a model of the mapping between the numerical symbols and the representations of numerosity on the number line.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Véronique Izard
- Department of Psychology, Harvard University, 33 Kirkland Street, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
174
|
Abstract
Adult humans possess a sophisticated repertoire of mathematical faculties. Many of these capacities are rooted in symbolic language and are therefore unlikely to be shared with nonhuman animals. However, a subset of these skills is shared with other animals, and this set is considered a cognitive vestige of our common evolutionary history. Current evidence indicates that humans and nonhuman animals share a core set of abilities for representing and comparing approximate numerosities nonverbally; however, it remains unclear whether nonhuman animals can perform approximate mental arithmetic. Here we show that monkeys can mentally add the numerical values of two sets of objects and choose a visual array that roughly corresponds to the arithmetic sum of these two sets. Furthermore, monkeys' performance during these calculations adheres to the same pattern as humans tested on the same nonverbal addition task. Our data demonstrate that nonverbal arithmetic is not unique to humans but is instead part of an evolutionarily primitive system for mathematical thinking shared by monkeys. Adult humans possess mathematical abilities that are unmatched by any other member of the animal kingdom. Yet, there is increasing evidence that the ability to enumerate sets of objects nonverbally is a capacity that humans share with other animal species. That is, like humans, nonhuman animals possess the ability to estimate and compare numerical values nonverbally. We asked whether humans and nonhuman animals also share a capacity for nonverbal arithmetic. We tested monkeys and college students on a nonverbal arithmetic task in which they had to add the numerical values of two sets of dots together and choose a stimulus from two options that reflected the arithmetic sum of the two sets. Our results indicate that monkeys perform approximate mental addition in a manner that is remarkably similar to the performance of the college students. These findings support the argument that humans and nonhuman primates share a cognitive system for nonverbal arithmetic, which likely reflects an evolutionary link in their cognitive abilities. Monkeys have an ability to represent numerical values even though they lack linguistic abilities. The authors show that monkeys can also perform addition on numerical values and that they perform similarly to college students who are asked to add without counting.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jessica F Cantlon
- Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, North Carolina, United States of America.
| | | |
Collapse
|
175
|
Moving along the number line: Operational momentum in nonsymbolic arithmetic. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2007; 69:1324-33. [PMID: 18078224 DOI: 10.3758/bf03192949] [Citation(s) in RCA: 159] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
|
176
|
|
177
|
Cappelletti M, Barth H, Fregni F, Spelke ES, Pascual-Leone A. rTMS over the intraparietal sulcus disrupts numerosity processing. Exp Brain Res 2007; 179:631-42. [PMID: 17216413 PMCID: PMC2567820 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-006-0820-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 71] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/09/2006] [Accepted: 11/26/2006] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
Abstract
It has been widely argued that the intraparietal sulcus (IPS) is involved in tasks that evoke representations of numerical magnitude, among other cognitive functions. However, the causal role of this parietal region in processing symbolic and non-symbolic numerosity has not been established. The current study used repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (rTMS) to the left and right IPS to investigate the effects of temporary deactivations of these regions on the capacity to represent symbolic (Arabic numbers) and non-symbolic (arrays of dots) numerosities. We found that comparisons of both symbolic and non-symbolic numerosities were impaired after rTMS to the left IPS but enhanced by rTMS to the right IPS. A signature effect of numerical distance was also found: greater impairment (or lesser facilitation) when comparing numerosities of similar magnitude. The reverse pattern of impairment and enhancement was found in a control task that required judging an analogue stimulus property (ellipse orientation) but no numerosity judgements. No rTMS effects for the numerosity tasks were found when stimulating an area adjacent but distinct from the IPS, the left and right angular gyrus. These data suggest that left IPS is critical for processing symbolic and non-symbolic numerosity; this processing may thus depend on common neural mechanisms, which are distinct from mechanisms supporting the processing of analogue stimulus properties.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marinella Cappelletti
- Center for Non-Invasive Brain Stimulation, Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center and Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA 02215, USA.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
178
|
Gilmore CK, McCarthy SE, Spelke ES. Symbolic arithmetic knowledge without instruction. Nature 2007; 447:589-91. [PMID: 17538620 DOI: 10.1038/nature05850] [Citation(s) in RCA: 196] [Impact Index Per Article: 10.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/18/2006] [Accepted: 04/18/2007] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
Symbolic arithmetic is fundamental to science, technology and economics, but its acquisition by children typically requires years of effort, instruction and drill. When adults perform mental arithmetic, they activate nonsymbolic, approximate number representations, and their performance suffers if this nonsymbolic system is impaired. Nonsymbolic number representations also allow adults, children, and even infants to add or subtract pairs of dot arrays and to compare the resulting sum or difference to a third array, provided that only approximate accuracy is required. Here we report that young children, who have mastered verbal counting and are on the threshold of arithmetic instruction, can build on their nonsymbolic number system to perform symbolic addition and subtraction. Children across a broad socio-economic spectrum solved symbolic problems involving approximate addition or subtraction of large numbers, both in a laboratory test and in a school setting. Aspects of symbolic arithmetic therefore lie within the reach of children who have learned no algorithms for manipulating numerical symbols. Our findings help to delimit the sources of children's difficulties learning symbolic arithmetic, and they suggest ways to enhance children's engagement with formal mathematics.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Camilla K Gilmore
- Learning Sciences Research Institute, University of Nottingham, Wollaton Road, Nottingham NG8 1BB, UK.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
179
|
Cordes S, King AP, Gallistel CR. Time left in the mouse. Behav Processes 2007; 74:142-51. [PMID: 17101237 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2006.10.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/28/2006] [Revised: 09/30/2006] [Accepted: 10/02/2006] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
Evidence suggests that the online combination of non-verbal magnitudes (durations, numerosities) is central to learning in both human and non-human animals [Gallistel, C.R., 1990. The Organization of Learning. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA]. The molecular basis of these computations, however, is an open question at this point. The current study provides the first direct test of temporal subtraction in a species in which the genetic code is available. In two experiments, mice were run in an adaptation of Gibbon and Church's [Gibbon, J., Church, R.M., 1981. Time left: linear versus logarithmic subjective time. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 7, 87-107] time left paradigm in order to characterize typical responding in this task. Both experiments suggest that mice engaged in online subtraction of temporal values, although the generalization of a learned response rule to novel stimulus values resulted in slightly less systematic responding. Potential explanations for this pattern of results are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sara Cordes
- Duke University, Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, 572 Research Dr., GSRB2, Box 91050, Durham, NC 27708-91050, United States.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
180
|
Abstract
Human cognition is founded, in part, on four systems for representing objects, actions, number, and space. It may be based, as well, on a fifth system for representing social partners. Each system has deep roots in human phylogeny and ontogeny, and it guides and shapes the mental lives of adults. Converging research on human infants, non-human primates, children and adults in diverse cultures can aid both understanding of these systems and attempts to overcome their limits.
Collapse
|
181
|
Sakkalis V, Zervakis M, Micheloyannis S. Significant EEG Features Involved in Mathematical Reasoning: Evidence from Wavelet Analysis. Brain Topogr 2006; 19:53-60. [PMID: 16988882 DOI: 10.1007/s10548-006-0012-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
Using electroencephalographic (EEG) signals and a novel methodology based on wavelet measures in the time-scale domain, we evaluated cortex reactions during mathematical thinking. Our purpose was to extract more precise information from the cortex reactions during this cognitive task. Initially, the brain areas (lobes) of significant activation during the task are extracted using time-averaged wavelet power spectrum estimation. Then, a refinement step makes use of statistical significance-based criteria for comparing wavelet power spectra between the task and the rest condition. EEG signals are recorded from 15 young normal volunteers using 30 scalp electrodes as participants performed one difficult arithmetic task and the results are compared with a rest situation. The results are in accordance with similar previous studies, showing activations of frontal and central regions. Compared with the alternative spectral-based techniques, the method we propose achieves higher task discrimination on the same dataset and provides additional detail-signal information to evaluate cortical reactivity during local cortical activation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Vangelis Sakkalis
- Department of Electronic and Computer Engineering, Technical University of Crete, Heraklion 71110, Greece.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
182
|
Ward C, Smuts BB. Quantity-based judgments in the domestic dog (Canis lupus familiaris). Anim Cogn 2006; 10:71-80. [PMID: 16941158 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-006-0042-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 119] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/27/2006] [Revised: 06/02/2006] [Accepted: 07/15/2006] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
Abstract
We examined the ability of domestic dogs to choose the larger versus smaller quantity of food in two experiments. In experiment 1, we investigated the ability of 29 dogs (results from 18 dogs were used in the data analysis) to discriminate between two quantities of food presented in eight different combinations. Choices were simultaneously presented and visually available at the time of choice. Overall, subjects chose the larger quantity more often than the smaller quantity, but they found numerically close comparisons more difficult. In experiment 2, we tested two dogs from experiment 1 under three conditions. In condition 1, we used similar methods from experiment 1 and tested the dogs multiple times on the eight combinations from experiment 1 plus one additional combination. In conditions 2 and 3, the food was visually unavailable to the subjects at the time of choice, but in condition 2, food choices were viewed simultaneously before being made visually unavailable, and in condition 3, they were viewed successively. In these last two conditions, and especially in condition 3, the dogs had to keep track of quantities mentally in order to choose optimally. Subjects still chose the larger quantity more often than the smaller quantity when the food was not simultaneously visible at the time of choice. Olfactory cues and inadvertent cuing by the experimenter were excluded as mechanisms for choosing larger quantities. The results suggest that, like apes tested on similar tasks, some dogs can form internal representations and make mental comparisons of quantity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Camille Ward
- Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, 1012 East Hall, 530 Church Street, Ann Arbor, MI 48109-1043, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
183
|
Jordan KE, Brannon EM. A common representational system governed by Weber's law: nonverbal numerical similarity judgments in 6-year-olds and rhesus macaques. J Exp Child Psychol 2006; 95:215-29. [PMID: 16808924 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2006.05.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 58] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/31/2006] [Revised: 05/15/2006] [Accepted: 05/18/2006] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
This study compared nonverbal numerical processing in 6-year-olds with that in nonhuman animals using a numerical bisection task. In the study, 16 children were trained on a delayed match-to-sample paradigm to match exemplars of two anchor numerosities. Children were then required to indicate whether a sample intermediate to the anchor values was closer to the small anchor value or the large anchor value. For two sets of anchor values with the same ratio, the probability of choosing the larger anchor value increased systematically with sample number, and the psychometric functions superimposed when plotted on a logarithmic scale. The psychometric functions produced by the children also superimposed with the psychometric functions produced by rhesus monkeys in an analogous previous experiment. These examples of superimposition demonstrate that nonverbal number representations, even in children who have acquired the verbal counting system, are modulated by Weber's law.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kerry E Jordan
- Center for Cognitive Neuroscience and Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Duke University, Durham, NC 27708, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
184
|
Cantlon JF, Brannon EM, Carter EJ, Pelphrey KA. Functional imaging of numerical processing in adults and 4-y-old children. PLoS Biol 2006; 4:e125. [PMID: 16594732 PMCID: PMC1431577 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0040125] [Citation(s) in RCA: 342] [Impact Index Per Article: 18.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/06/2005] [Accepted: 02/16/2006] [Indexed: 01/29/2023] Open
Abstract
Adult humans, infants, pre-school children, and non-human animals appear to share a system of approximate numerical processing for non-symbolic stimuli such as arrays of dots or sequences of tones. Behavioral studies of adult humans implicate a link between these non-symbolic numerical abilities and symbolic numerical processing (e.g., similar distance effects in accuracy and reaction-time for arrays of dots and Arabic numerals). However, neuroimaging studies have remained inconclusive on the neural basis of this link. The intraparietal sulcus (IPS) is known to respond selectively to symbolic numerical stimuli such as Arabic numerals. Recent studies, however, have arrived at conflicting conclusions regarding the role of the IPS in processing non-symbolic, numerosity arrays in adulthood, and very little is known about the brain basis of numerical processing early in development. Addressing the question of whether there is an early-developing neural basis for abstract numerical processing is essential for understanding the cognitive origins of our uniquely human capacity for math and science. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) at 4-Tesla and an event-related fMRI adaptation paradigm, we found that adults showed a greater IPS response to visual arrays that deviated from standard stimuli in their number of elements, than to stimuli that deviated in local element shape. These results support previous claims that there is a neurophysiological link between non-symbolic and symbolic numerical processing in adulthood. In parallel, we tested 4-y-old children with the same fMRI adaptation paradigm as adults to determine whether the neural locus of non-symbolic numerical activity in adults shows continuity in function over development. We found that the IPS responded to numerical deviants similarly in 4-y-old children and adults. To our knowledge, this is the first evidence that the neural locus of adult numerical cognition takes form early in development, prior to sophisticated symbolic numerical experience. More broadly, this is also, to our knowledge, the first cognitive fMRI study to test healthy children as young as 4 y, providing new insights into the neurophysiology of human cognitive development. This functional imaging study provides evidence for a neurobiological link between early non-symbolic numerical abilities of 4 year-old children and the more symbolic numerical processing of adults.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jessica F Cantlon
- 1Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Duke University, Durham, North Carolina, United States of America
- 2Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, North Carolina, United States of America
| | - Elizabeth M Brannon
- 1Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Duke University, Durham, North Carolina, United States of America
- 2Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, North Carolina, United States of America
| | - Elizabeth J Carter
- 1Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Duke University, Durham, North Carolina, United States of America
| | - Kevin A Pelphrey
- 1Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Duke University, Durham, North Carolina, United States of America
- 3Brain Imaging and Analysis Center, Duke University, Durham, North Carolina, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
185
|
Jordan KE, Brannon EM. Weber's Law influences numerical representations in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Anim Cogn 2006; 9:159-72. [PMID: 16575587 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-006-0017-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 66] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/30/2005] [Revised: 01/30/2006] [Accepted: 02/01/2006] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
We present the results of two experiments that probe the ability of rhesus macaques to match visual arrays based on number. Three monkeys were first trained on a delayed match-to-sample paradigm (DMTS) to match stimuli on the basis of number and ignore continuous dimensions such as element size, cumulative surface area, and density. Monkeys were then tested in a numerical bisection experiment that required them to indicate whether a sample numerosity was closer to a small or large anchor value. Results indicated that, for two sets of anchor values with the same ratio, the probability of choosing the larger anchor value systematically increased with the sample number and the psychometric functions superimposed. A second experiment employed a numerical DMTS task in which the choice values contained an exact numerical match to the sample and a distracter that varied in number. Both accuracy and reaction time were modulated by the ratio between the correct numerical match and the distracter, as predicted by Weber's Law.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kerry E Jordan
- Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, Duke University, Box 90999, Durham, NC 27708, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|