151
|
Jacquey L, Fagard J, O’Regan K, Esseily R. Développement du savoir-faire corporel durant la première année de vie du bébé. ENFANCE 2020. [DOI: 10.3917/enf2.202.0175] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/14/2022]
|
152
|
Köster M, Kayhan E, Langeloh M, Hoehl S. Making Sense of the World: Infant Learning From a Predictive Processing Perspective. PERSPECTIVES ON PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE 2020; 15:562-571. [PMID: 32167407 PMCID: PMC7243078 DOI: 10.1177/1745691619895071] [Citation(s) in RCA: 45] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/12/2023]
Abstract
For human infants, the first years after birth are a period of intense exploration-getting to understand their own competencies in interaction with a complex physical and social environment. In contemporary neuroscience, the predictive-processing framework has been proposed as a general working principle of the human brain, the optimization of predictions about the consequences of one's own actions, and sensory inputs from the environment. However, the predictive-processing framework has rarely been applied to infancy research. We argue that a predictive-processing framework may provide a unifying perspective on several phenomena of infant development and learning that may seem unrelated at first sight. These phenomena include statistical learning principles, infants' motor and proprioceptive learning, and infants' basic understanding of their physical and social environment. We discuss how a predictive-processing perspective can advance the understanding of infants' early learning processes in theory, research, and application.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Moritz Köster
- Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences
- Faculty of Education and Psychology, Freie Universität Berlin
- Department of Psychology, Graduate School of Letters, Kyoto University
| | - Ezgi Kayhan
- Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences
- Department of Psychology, University of Potsdam
| | - Miriam Langeloh
- Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences
- Department of Psychology, Heidelberg University
| | - Stefanie Hoehl
- Department of Developmental and Educational Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, University of Vienna
| |
Collapse
|
153
|
Brod G, Breitwieser J, Hasselhorn M, Bunge SA. Being proven wrong elicits learning in children – but only in those with higher executive function skills. Dev Sci 2020; 23:e12916. [DOI: 10.1111/desc.12916] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/08/2018] [Revised: 09/01/2019] [Accepted: 10/11/2019] [Indexed: 12/29/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Garvin Brod
- Center for Individual Development and Adaptive Education of Children at Risk (IDeA) Frankfurt Germany
- DIPF
- Leibniz Institute for Research and Information in Education Frankfurt Germany
- Department of Psychology Goethe University Frankfurt Germany
| | - Jasmin Breitwieser
- Center for Individual Development and Adaptive Education of Children at Risk (IDeA) Frankfurt Germany
- DIPF
- Leibniz Institute for Research and Information in Education Frankfurt Germany
| | - Marcus Hasselhorn
- Center for Individual Development and Adaptive Education of Children at Risk (IDeA) Frankfurt Germany
- DIPF
- Leibniz Institute for Research and Information in Education Frankfurt Germany
- Department of Psychology Goethe University Frankfurt Germany
| | - Silvia A. Bunge
- Department of Psychology & Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute University of California, Berkeley Berkeley CA USA
| |
Collapse
|
154
|
Shavlik M, Bauer JR, Booth AE. Children's Preference for Causal Information in Storybooks. Front Psychol 2020; 11:666. [PMID: 32351428 PMCID: PMC7174774 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00666] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/29/2019] [Accepted: 03/19/2020] [Indexed: 11/26/2022] Open
Abstract
Fostering early literacy depends in part on engaging and inspiring children’s early interest in reading. Enriching the causal content of children’s books may be one way to do so, as causal information has been empirically shown to capture children’s attention. To more directly test whether children’s book preferences might be driven by causal content, we created pairs of expository books closely matched for content and complexity, but with differing amounts of causal information embedded therein. Three and 4 years old participants (n = 48) were read both books and their interests and preferences were evaluated. When asked to choose, children preferred the highly causal over the minimally causal books. Results are discussed in terms of broader implications for creating books that optimally engage young children, as well as guiding book selections parents and educators make in their endeavors to promote interest in reading and early literacy.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Margaret Shavlik
- Department of Psychology and Human Development, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN, United States
| | - Jessie Raye Bauer
- Department of Psychology, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX, United States
| | - Amy E Booth
- Department of Psychology and Human Development, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN, United States
| |
Collapse
|
155
|
Weisberg DS, Hopkins EJ. Preschoolers' extension and export of information from realistic and fantastical stories. INFANT AND CHILD DEVELOPMENT 2020. [DOI: 10.1002/icd.2182] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Deena Skolnick Weisberg
- Department of Psychological and Brain SciencesVillanova University Villanova Pennsylvania USA
| | - Emily J. Hopkins
- Department of PsychologyUniversity of Scranton Scranton Pennsylvania USA
| |
Collapse
|
156
|
Lai W, Rácz P, Roberts G. Experience With a Linguistic Variant Affects the Acquisition of Its Sociolinguistic Meaning: An Alien-Language-Learning Experiment. Cogn Sci 2020; 44:e12832. [PMID: 32246526 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.12832] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/25/2019] [Revised: 02/24/2020] [Accepted: 03/02/2020] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
Abstract
How do speakers learn the social meaning of different linguistic variants, and what factors influence how likely a particular social-linguistic association is to be learned? It has been argued that the social meaning of more salient variants should be learned faster, and that learners' pre-existing experience of a variant will influence its salience. In this paper, we report two artificial-language-learning experiments investigating this. Each experiment involved two language-learning stages followed by a test. The first stage introduced the artificial language and trained participants in it, while the second stage added a simple social context using images of cartoon aliens. The first learning stage was intended to establish participants' experience with the artificial language in general and with the distribution of linguistic variants in particular. The second stage, in which linguistic stimuli were accompanied by images of particular aliens, was intended to simulate the acquisition of linguistic variants in a social context. In our first experiment, we manipulated whether a particular linguistic variant, associated with one species of alien in the second learning phase, had been encountered in the first learning phase. In the second experiment, we manipulated whether the variant had been encountered in the same grammatical context. In both cases we predicted that the unexpectedness of a new variant or a new grammatical context for an old variant would increase the variant's salience and facilitate the learning of its social meaning. This is what we found, although in the second experiment, the effect was driven by better learners. Our results suggest that unexpectedness increases the salience of variants and makes their social distribution easier to learn, deepening our understanding of the role of individual language experience in the acquisition of sociolinguistic meaning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Wei Lai
- Department of Linguistics, University of Pennsylvania
| | - Péter Rácz
- Cognitive Development Center, Central European University
| | | |
Collapse
|
157
|
Callanan MA, Legare CH, Sobel DM, Jaeger GJ, Letourneau S, McHugh SR, Willard A, Brinkman A, Finiasz Z, Rubio E, Barnett A, Gose R, Martin JL, Meisner R, Watson J. Exploration, Explanation, and Parent-Child Interaction in Museums. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev 2020; 85:7-137. [PMID: 32175600 PMCID: PMC10676013 DOI: 10.1111/mono.12412] [Citation(s) in RCA: 57] [Impact Index Per Article: 11.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
Young children develop causal knowledge through everyday family conversations and activities. Children's museums are an informative setting for studying the social context of causal learning because family members engage together in everyday scientific thinking as they play in museums. In this multisite collaborative project, we investigate children's developing causal thinking in the context of family interaction at museum exhibits. We focus on explaining and exploring as two fundamental collaborative processes in parent-child interaction, investigating how families explain and explore in open-ended collaboration at gear exhibits in three children's museums in Providence, RI, San Jose, CA, and Austin, TX. Our main research questions examined (a) how open-ended family exploration and explanation relate to one another to form a dynamic for children's learning; (b) how that dynamic differs for families using different interaction styles, and relates to contextual factors such as families' science background, and (c) how that dynamic predicts children's independent causal thinking when given more structured tasks. We summarize findings on exploring, explaining, and parent-child interaction (PCI) styles. We then present findings on how these measures related to one another, and finally how that dynamic predicts children's causal thinking. In studying children's exploring we described two types of behaviors of importance for causal thinking: (a) Systematic Exploration: Connecting gears to form a gear machine followed by spinning the gear machine. (b) Resolute Behavior: Problem-solving behaviors, in which children attempted to connect or spin a particular set of gears, hit an obstacle, and then persisted to succeed (as opposed to moving on to another behavior). Older children engaged in both behaviors more than younger children, and the proportion of these behaviors were correlated with one another. Parents and children talked to each other while interacting with the exhibits. We coded causal language, as well as other types of utterances. Parents' causal language predicted children's causal language, independent of age. The proportion of parents' causal language also predicted the proportion of children's systematic exploration. Resolute behavior on the part of children did not correlate with parents' causal language, but did correlate with children's own talk about actions and the exhibit. We next considered who set goals for the play in a more holistic measure of parent-child interaction style, identifying dyads as parent-directed, child-directed, or jointly-directed in their interaction with one another. Children in different parent-child interaction styles engaged in different amounts of systematic exploration and had parents who engaged in different amounts of causal language. Resolute behavior and the language related to children engaging in such troubleshooting, seemed more consistent across the three parent-child interaction styles. Using general linear mixed modeling, we considered relations within sequences of action and talk. We found that the timing of parents' causal language was crucial to whether children engaged in systematic exploration. Parents' causal talk was a predictor of children's systematic exploration only if it occurred prior to the act of spinning the gears (while children were building gear machines). We did not observe an effect of causal language when it occurred concurrently with or after children's spinning. Similarly, children's talk about their actions and the exhibit predicted their resolute behavior, but only when the talk occurred while the child was encountering the problem. No effects were found for models where the talk happened concurrently or after resolving the problem. Finally, we considered how explaining and exploring related to children's causal thinking. We analyzed measures of children's causal thinking about gears and a free play measure with a novel set of gears. Principal component analysis revealed a latent factor of causal thinking in these measures. Structural equation modeling examined how parents' background in science related to children's systematic exploration, parents' causal language, and parent-child interaction style, and then how those factors predicted children's causal thinking. In a full model, with children's age and gender included, children's systematic exploration related to children's causal thinking. Overall, these data demonstrate that children's systematic exploration and parents' causal explanation are best studied in relation to one another, because both contributed to children's learning while playing at a museum exhibit. Children engaged in systematic exploration, which supported their causal thinking. Parents' causal talk supported children's exploration when it was presented at certain times during the interaction. In contrast, children's persistence in problem solving was less sensitive to parents' talk or interaction style, and more related to children's own language, which may act as a form of self-explanation. We discuss the findings in light of ongoing approaches to promote the benefit of parent-child interaction during play for children's learning and problem solving. We also examine the implications of these findings for formal and informal learning settings, and for theoretical integration of constructivist and sociocultural approaches in the study of children's causal thinking.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - David M Sobel
- Department of Cognitive, Linguistic, and Psychological Sciences, Brown University
| | | | | | - Sam R McHugh
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Santa Cruz
| | | | | | - Zoe Finiasz
- Department of Human Development, Cornell University
| | - Erika Rubio
- School of Education, University of Southern California
| | | | - Robin Gose
- MOXI, The Wolf Museum of Exploration and Innovation
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
158
|
Interpersonal Neural Entrainment during Early Social Interaction. Trends Cogn Sci 2020; 24:329-342. [PMID: 32160569 DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2020.01.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 91] [Impact Index Per Article: 18.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/30/2019] [Revised: 01/19/2020] [Accepted: 01/21/2020] [Indexed: 12/14/2022]
Abstract
Currently, we understand much about how children's brains attend to and learn from information presented while they are alone, viewing a screen - but less about how interpersonal social influences are substantiated in the brain. Here, we consider research that examines how social behaviors affect not one, but both partners in a dyad. We review studies that measured interpersonal neural entrainment during early social interaction, considering two ways of measuring entrainment: concurrent entrainment (e.g., 'when A is high, B is high' - also known as synchrony) and sequential entrainment ('changes in A forward-predict changes in B'). We discuss possible causes of interpersonal neural entrainment, and consider whether it is merely an epiphenomenon, or whether it plays an independent, mechanistic role in early attention and learning.
Collapse
|
159
|
Butler LP. The Empirical Child? A Framework for Investigating the Development of Scientific Habits of Mind. CHILD DEVELOPMENT PERSPECTIVES 2020. [DOI: 10.1111/cdep.12354] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
|
160
|
Abstract
Recent work has argued that curiosity can improve learning. However, these studies also leave open the possibility that being on the verge of knowing can itself induce curiosity. We investigate how prior knowledge relates to curiosity and subsequent learning using a trivia question task. Curiosity in our task is best predicted by a learner's estimate of their current knowledge, more so than an objective measure of what they actually know. Learning is best predicted by both curiosity and an objective measure of knowledge. These results suggest that while curiosity is correlated with knowledge, there is only a small boost in learning from being curious. The implication is that the mechanisms that drive curiosity are not identical to those that drive learning outcomes.
Collapse
|
161
|
Abstract
Human culture is unique among animals in its complexity, variability, and cumulative quality. This article describes the development and diversity of cumulative cultural learning. Children inhabit cultural ecologies that consist of group-specific knowledge, practices, and technologies that are inherited and modified over generations. The learning processes that enable cultural acquisition and transmission are universal but are sufficiently flexible to accommodate the highly diverse cultural repertoires of human populations. Children learn culture in several complementary ways, including through exploration, observation, participation, imitation, and instruction. These methods of learning vary in frequency and kind within and between populations due to variation in socialization values and practices associated with specific educational institutions, skill sets, and knowledge systems. The processes by which children acquire and transmit the cumulative culture of their communities provide unique insight into the evolution and ontogeny of human cognition and culture.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Cristine H Legare
- Department of Psychology, The University of Texas at Austin, Austin, Texas 78712, USA
| |
Collapse
|
162
|
Berger A, Shmueli M, Lisson S, Ben-Shachar MS, Lindinger NM, Lewis CE, Dodge NC, Molteno CD, Meintjes EM, Jacobson JL, Jacobson SW. Deficits in arithmetic error detection in infants with prenatal alcohol exposure: An ERP study. Dev Cogn Neurosci 2019; 40:100722. [PMID: 31733524 PMCID: PMC6942494 DOI: 10.1016/j.dcn.2019.100722] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/07/2019] [Revised: 08/12/2019] [Accepted: 10/16/2019] [Indexed: 12/04/2022] Open
Abstract
Prenatal alcohol exposure (PAE) is associated with a range of physical, cognitive, and behavioral problems, particularly in arithmetic. We report ERP data collected from 32 infants (mean age = 6.8 mo; SD = 0.6; range = 6.1–8.1; 16 typically developing [TD]; 16 prenatally alcohol-exposed) during a task designed to assess error detection. Evidence of error monitoring at this early age suggests that precursors of the onset of executive control can already be detected in infancy. As predicted, the ERPs of the TD infants, time-locked to the presentation of the solution to simple arithmetic equations, showed greater negative activity for the incorrect solution condition at middle-frontal scalp areas. Spectral analysis indicated specificity to the 6–7 Hz frequency range. By contrast, the alcohol-exposed infants did not show the increased middle-frontal negativity seen in the TD group nor the increased power in the 6–7 Hz frequency, suggesting a marked developmental delay in error detection and/or early impairment in information processing of small quantities. Overall, our research demonstrates that (a) the brain network involved in error detection can be identified and highly specified in TD young infants, and (b) this effect is replicable and can be utilized for studying developmental psychopathology at very early ages.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Andrea Berger
- Department of Psychology and Zlotowski Center for Neuroscience, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beer Sheva, Israel.
| | - Michael Shmueli
- Department of Psychology and Zlotowski Center for Neuroscience, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beer Sheva, Israel
| | - Svetlana Lisson
- Department of Psychology and Zlotowski Center for Neuroscience, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beer Sheva, Israel
| | - Mattan S Ben-Shachar
- Department of Psychology and Zlotowski Center for Neuroscience, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beer Sheva, Israel
| | - Nadine M Lindinger
- Department of Human Biology, University of Cape Town, Faculty of Health Sciences, Cape Town, South Africa
| | - Catherine E Lewis
- Department of Human Biology, University of Cape Town, Faculty of Health Sciences, Cape Town, South Africa
| | - Neil C Dodge
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neurosciences, Wayne State University School of Medicine, Detroit, MI, United States
| | - Christopher D Molteno
- Department of Psychiatry and Mental Health, University of Cape Town, Faculty of Health Sciences, Cape Town, South Africa
| | - Ernesta M Meintjes
- Department of Human Biology, University of Cape Town, Faculty of Health Sciences, Cape Town, South Africa; MRC/UCT Medical Imaging Research Unit, University of Cape Town, Division of Biomedical Engineering, Faculty of Health Sciences, Cape Town, South Africa
| | - Joseph L Jacobson
- Department of Human Biology, University of Cape Town, Faculty of Health Sciences, Cape Town, South Africa; Department of Psychiatry and Mental Health, University of Cape Town, Faculty of Health Sciences, Cape Town, South Africa
| | - Sandra W Jacobson
- Department of Human Biology, University of Cape Town, Faculty of Health Sciences, Cape Town, South Africa; Department of Psychiatry and Mental Health, University of Cape Town, Faculty of Health Sciences, Cape Town, South Africa
| |
Collapse
|
163
|
Medina C, Sobel DM. Caregiver-child interaction influences causal learning and engagement during structured play. J Exp Child Psychol 2019; 189:104678. [PMID: 31635828 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2019.104678] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/13/2018] [Revised: 07/14/2019] [Accepted: 07/31/2019] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
The current study investigated the relation between the ways in which caregivers and children interact in a learning environment and children's learning outcomes and engagement with the learning task. We assessed caregiver-child interaction in a structured play environment in which 3- and 4-year-olds and a caregiver were tasked with learning a causal system. Children whose caregivers were more directive during their interaction learned the causal system the best and better than children whose caregivers were more hands off and allowed children to engage in unstructured exploration. These two groups of children explored for the same amount of time, indicating similar levels of engagement with the task. Children whose caregivers were more guiding, but not directive, played significantly longer than either of the other groups, suggesting deeper engagement. We discuss these findings in relation to how children engage in causal learning and how caregivers might contribute to children's learning and engagement with the learning process.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Carol Medina
- Department of Psychology, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL 60637, USA.
| | - David M Sobel
- Department of Cognitive, Linguistic, and Psychological Sciences, Brown University, Providence, RI 02912, USA
| |
Collapse
|
164
|
Bridgers S, Jara-Ettinger J, Gweon H. Young children consider the expected utility of others' learning to decide what to teach. Nat Hum Behav 2019; 4:144-152. [PMID: 31611659 DOI: 10.1038/s41562-019-0748-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 45] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/07/2018] [Accepted: 08/29/2019] [Indexed: 01/08/2023]
Abstract
Direct instruction facilitates learning without the costs of exploration, yet teachers must be selective because not everything can nor needs to be taught. How do we decide what to teach and what to leave for learners to discover? Here we investigate the cognitive underpinnings of the human ability to prioritize what to teach. We present a computational model that decides what to teach by maximizing the learner's expected utility of learning from instruction and from exploration, and we show that children (aged 5-7 years) make decisions that are consistent with the model's predictions (that is, minimizing the learner's costs and maximizing the rewards). Children flexibly considered either the learner's utility or their own, depending on the context, and even considered costs they had not personally experienced, to decide what to teach. These results suggest that utility-based reasoning may play an important role in curating cultural knowledge by supporting selective transmission of high-utility information.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sophie Bridgers
- Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA.
| | | | - Hyowon Gweon
- Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
165
|
Köster M, Langeloh M, Hoehl S. Visually Entrained Theta Oscillations Increase for Unexpected Events in the Infant Brain. Psychol Sci 2019; 30:1656-1663. [PMID: 31603724 PMCID: PMC6843601 DOI: 10.1177/0956797619876260] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
Infants form basic expectations about their physical and social environment, as indicated by their attention toward events that violate their expectations. Yet little is known about the neuronal processing of unexpected events in the infant brain. Here, we used rhythmic visual brain stimulation in 9-month-olds (N = 38) to elicit oscillations of the theta (4 Hz) and the alpha (6 Hz) rhythms while presenting events with unexpected or expected outcomes. We found that visually entrained theta oscillations sharply increased for unexpected outcomes, in contrast to expected outcomes, in the scalp-recorded electroencephalogram. Visually entrained alpha oscillations did not differ between conditions. The processing of unexpected events at the theta rhythm may reflect learning processes such as the refinement of infants’ basic representations. Visual brain-stimulation techniques provide new ways to investigate the functional relevance of neuronal oscillatory dynamics in early brain development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Moritz Köster
- Institute of Psychology, Free University Berlin.,Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Miriam Langeloh
- Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany.,Department of Psychology, Heidelberg University
| | - Stefanie Hoehl
- Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany.,Faculty of Psychology, University of Vienna
| |
Collapse
|
166
|
Meng X, Nakawake Y, Nitta H, Hashiya K, Moriguchi Y. Space and rank: infants expect agents in higher position to be socially dominant. Proc Biol Sci 2019; 286:20191674. [PMID: 31594505 DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2019.1674] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/12/2023] Open
Abstract
Social hierarchies exist throughout the animal kingdom, including among humans. Our daily interactions inevitably reflect social dominance relationships between individuals. How do we mentally represent such concepts? Studies show that social dominance is represented as vertical space (i.e. high = dominant) by adults and preschool children, suggesting a space-dominance representational link in social cognition. However, little is known about its early development. Here, we present experimental evidence that 12- to 16-month-old infants expect agents presented in a higher spatial position to be more socially dominant than agents in a lower spatial position. After infants repeatedly watched the higher and lower agents being presented simultaneously, they looked longer at the screen when the lower agent subsequently outcompeted the higher agent in securing a reward object, suggesting that this outcome violated their higher-is-dominant expectation. We first manipulated agents' positions by presenting them on a podium (experiment 1). Then we presented the agents on a double-decker stand to make their spatial positions directly above or below each other (experiment 2), and we replicated the results (experiment 3). This research demonstrates that infants expect spatially higher-positioned agents to be socially dominant, suggesting deep roots of the space-dominance link in ontogeny.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Xianwei Meng
- Graduate School of Education, Kyoto University, Kyoto, Japan.,Japan Society for the Promotion of Science, Tokyo, Japan.,Faculty of Human-Environment Studies, Kyushu University, Fukuoka, Japan.,Centre for Baby Science, Doshisha University, Kyoto, Japan
| | - Yo Nakawake
- Institute of Cognitive and Evolutionary Anthropology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK.,Faculty of Human-Environment Studies, Kyushu University, Fukuoka, Japan
| | - Hiroshi Nitta
- Japan Society for the Promotion of Science, Tokyo, Japan.,Graduate School of Human-Environment Studies, Kyushu University, Fukuoka, Japan
| | - Kazuhide Hashiya
- Faculty of Human-Environment Studies, Kyushu University, Fukuoka, Japan
| | | |
Collapse
|
167
|
Lamnina M, Chase CC. Developing a thirst for knowledge: How uncertainty in the classroom influences curiosity, affect, learning, and transfer. CONTEMPORARY EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY 2019. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2019.101785] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
|
168
|
Maloy J, Fries L, Laski F, Ramirez G. Seductive Details in the Flipped Classroom: The Impact of Interesting but Educationally Irrelevant Information on Student Learning and Motivation. CBE LIFE SCIENCES EDUCATION 2019; 18:ar42. [PMID: 31469621 PMCID: PMC6755318 DOI: 10.1187/cbe.19-01-0004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/13/2023]
Abstract
In this study, we assessed the impact of providing students with short video clips highlighting the relevance of material they are learning in the genetics classroom to their everyday lives. These interesting but non-learning objective oriented clips, referred to as "seductive details," have been studied extensively in laboratory contexts. In laboratory studies, seductive details have been shown to actually decrease learning, leading some to recommend that any information not directly pertaining to academic learning outcomes be removed from education materials. We aimed to uncover effects of seductive details in an actual college course, in a manner divorced from the confounding variation introduced by instructor-level differences in personality and lecture styles. Our results show that, in a flipped-classroom environment, seductive details do not harm students' content attainment, interest, or perceived learning, but they are memorable. Students with high background knowledge of genetics reported greater learning after watching videos containing seductive details than students who watched equivalent videos without seductive details, but there was no difference in quiz scores between the groups. These results contradict some of the major effects observed throughout decades of studies conducted in artificial psychology laboratory environments and highlight possible affective benefits of instructors using seductive details.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jeffrey Maloy
- Department of Life Sciences Core Education, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA 90095
- *Address correspondence to: Jeffrey Maloy ()
| | - Laura Fries
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA 90095
| | - Frank Laski
- Department of Life Sciences Core Education, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA 90095
- Department of Molecular, Cell, and Developmental Biology, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA 90095
| | - Gerardo Ramirez
- Department of Educational Psychology, Ball State University, Muncie, IN 47306
| |
Collapse
|
169
|
Meng X, Ishii T, Sugimoto K, Song R, Moriguchi Y, Watanabe K. Smiling enemies: Young children better recall mean individuals who smile. J Exp Child Psychol 2019; 188:104672. [PMID: 31430567 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2019.104672] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/10/2019] [Revised: 07/19/2019] [Accepted: 07/23/2019] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
Abstract
Remembering whether a person is cooperative is essential in social interactions. It has been shown that adults have better memory of a person who showed an incongruence between emotional expression and expected behavior (e.g., smiling while stealing). To examine whether children would show similar emotional incongruity effects, we examined 70 children aged 5 or 6 years. They obtained coins that could be exchanged later for rewards (stickers) by answering quiz questions. Then, they participated in the coin collection game where individual persons with smiling or angry expressions appeared one at a time on a computer monitor. These same individuals then either gave coins to or took coins away from the children, leading to congruent (smiling giver and angry taker) and incongruent (smiling taker and angry giver) conditions. After the game, children needed to choose between two faces to indicate which one previously appeared in the game. Participants recognized faces better under the incongruent conditions. In particular, the smiling taker was recognized significantly better than the angry taker, whereas no difference was observed for the smiling and angry givers. Evidently, 5- and 6-year-olds better remember individuals whose facial expression or appearance is incongruent with their expected behavior.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Xianwei Meng
- Graduate School of Education, Kyoto University, Kyoto 606-8501, Japan; Japan Society for the Promotion of Science, Tokyo 102-0083, Japan; Center for Baby Science, Doshisha University, Kyoto 619-0225, Japan.
| | - Tatsunori Ishii
- Faculty of Science and Engineering, Waseda University, Tokyo, 169-8555, Japan
| | - Kairi Sugimoto
- Faculty of Science and Engineering, Waseda University, Tokyo, 169-8555, Japan
| | - Ruiting Song
- Wildlife Research Center, Kyoto University, Kyoto 606-8501, Japan
| | - Yusuke Moriguchi
- Graduate School of Education, Kyoto University, Kyoto 606-8501, Japan
| | - Katsumi Watanabe
- Faculty of Science and Engineering, Waseda University, Tokyo, 169-8555, Japan; Art & Design, University of New South Wales, Sydney, NSW 2021, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
170
|
|
171
|
Ruggeri A, Swaboda N, Sim ZL, Gopnik A. Shake it baby, but only when needed: Preschoolers adapt their exploratory strategies to the information structure of the task. Cognition 2019; 193:104013. [PMID: 31280062 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2019.104013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/08/2018] [Revised: 06/20/2019] [Accepted: 06/21/2019] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
Abstract
Previous research has suggested that active engagement with the world drives children's remarkable learning capabilities. We investigated whether preschoolers are "ecological learners," that is, whether they are able to select those active learning strategies that are most informative in a given task. Children had to choose which of two exploratory actions (open vs. shake) to perform to find an egg shaker hidden in one of four small boxes, contained in two larger boxes. Prior to this game, children either learnt that the egg was equally likely to be found in any of the four small boxes (Uniform condition), or that it was most likely to be found in one particular small box (Skewed condition). Results of Study 1 show that 3- and 4-year-olds successfully tailored their exploratory actions to the different likelihood-distributions: They were more likely to shake first in the Uniform compared to the Skewed condition. Five-year-olds were equally likely to shake first, irrespective of condition, even when incentivized to shake only when needed (Study 2a). However, when the relevance of the frequency training for the hiding game was highlighted (Study 2b and Study 2c), the 5-year-olds showed the same behavioural pattern as the younger preschoolers in Study 1. We suggest that ecological learning may be a key mechanism underlying children's effectiveness in active learning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Azzurra Ruggeri
- Max Planck Research Group iSearch - Information Search, Ecological and Active Learning Research with Children, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Lentzeallee 94, 14195 Berlin, Germany; School of Education, Technical University Munich, Arcisstraße 21, 80333 Munich, Germany.
| | - Nora Swaboda
- Max Planck Research Group iSearch - Information Search, Ecological and Active Learning Research with Children, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Lentzeallee 94, 14195 Berlin, Germany
| | - Zi Lin Sim
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley, CA 94720-1650, USA
| | - Alison Gopnik
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley, CA 94720-1650, USA
| |
Collapse
|
172
|
Busch JTA, Legare CH. Using data to solve problems: Children reason flexibly in response to different kinds of evidence. J Exp Child Psychol 2019; 183:172-188. [PMID: 30875548 PMCID: PMC10675997 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2019.01.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/19/2018] [Revised: 01/05/2019] [Accepted: 01/11/2019] [Indexed: 11/21/2022]
Abstract
This study examined children's (5- to 9-year-olds, N = 363) abilities to use information seeking and explanation to solve problems using conclusive or inconclusive (i.e., consistent, inconsistent, or ambiguous) evidence. Results demonstrated that inconsistent and ambiguous evidence, not consistent evidence, motivate more requests for information than conclusive evidence. In addition, children's explanations were flexible in response to evidence; explanations based on transitive inference were more likely to be associated with an accurate conclusion than other explanation types. Children's requests for additional information in response to inconclusive evidence increased with age, as did their problem-solving accuracy. The data demonstrate that children's capacity to use information seeking and explanation develop in tandem as tools for problem solving.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Justin T A Busch
- Department of Psychology, The University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX 78712, USA.
| | - Cristine H Legare
- Department of Psychology, The University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX 78712, USA
| |
Collapse
|
173
|
Wang SH. Regularity detection and explanation-based learning jointly support learning about physical events in early infancy. Cogn Psychol 2019; 113:101219. [PMID: 31200209 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2019.05.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/03/2018] [Revised: 04/26/2019] [Accepted: 05/09/2019] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
Abstract
The present research considers statistical learning (SL) and explanation-based learning (EBL) as joint mechanisms to support the development of physical knowledge. Infants watched teaching events in which a cover was lowered over an object and released, with outcomes that violated object principles. The object became fully hidden under a cover that was much shorter, and it remained partly visible under a cover that was much taller. Next, infants watched two test events identical to the teaching events except that one of the events was modified to present a plausible outcome and thus deviated from teaching. Infants at 3.5 months readily detected the regularity in the teaching events and noticed the change in the modified test event, whereas 6.5-month-olds did not. The pattern of response was reversed (1) when 3.5-month-olds were primed to notice the violation of object principles in the teaching events, which interfered with EBL and led infants to miss the change in the modified test event; and (2) when 6.5-month-olds were provided ways to remove the violation from the teaching events, which enabled EBL and led infants to notice the change in the modified test event. Together, the results shed light on young infants' approach to learning about physical events-one that integrates SL for pattern detection and EBL for causal coherence of the rule being learned.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Su-Hua Wang
- Department of Psychology, University of California, 1156 High Street, Santa Cruz, CA 95064, United States.
| |
Collapse
|
174
|
Wu R. Learning What to Learn Across the Life Span: From Objects to Real-World Skills. CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE 2019. [DOI: 10.1177/0963721419847994] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
One of the most difficult and important problems that all learners face across the life span is learning what to learn. Understanding what to learn is difficult when both relevant and irrelevant information compete for attention. In these situations, the learner can rely on cues in the environment, as well as prior knowledge. However, these sources of information sometimes conflict, and the learner has to prioritize some sources over others. Determining what to learn is important because learning relevant information helps the learner achieve goals, whereas learning irrelevant information can waste time and energy. A new theoretical approach posits that adaptation is relevant for all age groups because the environment is dynamic, suggesting that learning what to learn is a problem relevant across the life span instead of only during infancy and childhood. In this article, I review new research demonstrating the importance and ways of learning what to learn across the life span, from objects to real-world skills, before highlighting some unresolved issues for future research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rachel Wu
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Riverside
| |
Collapse
|
175
|
Garcés M, Finkel L. Emotional Theory of Rationality. Front Integr Neurosci 2019; 13:11. [PMID: 31024267 PMCID: PMC6463757 DOI: 10.3389/fnint.2019.00011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/09/2018] [Accepted: 03/13/2019] [Indexed: 11/16/2022] Open
Abstract
In recent decades, the existence of a close relationship between emotional phenomena and rational processes has certainly been established, yet there is still no unified definition or effective model to describe them. To advance our understanding of the mechanisms governing the behavior of living beings, we must integrate multiple theories, experiments, and models from both fields. In this article we propose a new theoretical framework that allows integrating and understanding the emotion-cognition duality, from a functional point of view. Based on evolutionary principles, our reasoning adds to the definition and understanding of emotion, justifying its origin, explaining its mission and dynamics, and linking it to higher cognitive processes, mainly with attention, cognition, decision-making, and consciousness. According to our theory, emotions are the mechanism for brain function optimization, aside from the contingency and stimuli prioritization system. As a result of this approach, we have developed a dynamic systems-level model capable of providing plausible explanations for certain psychological and behavioral phenomena and establishing a new framework for the scientific definition of some fundamental psychological terms.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mario Garcés
- Department of Emotion, Cognition and Behavior Research, DAXNATUR S.L., Majadahonda, Spain
| | - Lucila Finkel
- Department of Sociology, Methodology and Theory, Universidad Complutense de Madrid, Madrid, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
176
|
Cheng C, Kaldy Z, Blaser E. Focused attention predicts visual working memory performance in 13-month-old infants: A pupillometric study. Dev Cogn Neurosci 2019; 36:100616. [PMID: 30769261 PMCID: PMC6555424 DOI: 10.1016/j.dcn.2019.100616] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/16/2018] [Revised: 01/15/2019] [Accepted: 01/15/2019] [Indexed: 11/26/2022] Open
Abstract
Attention turns looking, into seeing. Yet, little developmental research has examined the interface of attention and visual working memory (VWM), where what is seen is maintained for use in ongoing visual tasks. Using the task-evoked pupil response - a sensitive, real-time, involuntary measure of focused attention that has been shown to correlate with VWM performance in adults and older children - we examined the relationship between focused attention and VWM in 13-month-olds. We used a Delayed Match Retrieval paradigm, to test infants' VWM for object-location bindings - what went where - while recording anticipatory gaze responses and pupil dilation. We found that infants with greater focused attention during memory encoding showed significantly better memory performance. As well, trials that ended in a correct response had significantly greater pupil response during memory encoding than incorrect trials. Taken together, this shows that pupillometry can be used as a measure of focused attention in infants, and a means to identify those individuals, or moments, where cognitive effort is maximized.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chen Cheng
- University of Massachusetts Boston, United States
| | - Zsuzsa Kaldy
- University of Massachusetts Boston, United States
| | - Erik Blaser
- University of Massachusetts Boston, United States.
| |
Collapse
|
177
|
Willard AK, Busch JTA, Cullum KA, Letourneau SM, Sobel DM, Callanan M, Legare CH. Explain This, Explore That: A Study of Parent-Child Interaction in a Children's Museum. Child Dev 2019; 90:e598-e617. [PMID: 30866040 PMCID: PMC6850333 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.13232] [Citation(s) in RCA: 44] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
Parents visiting a gear exhibit at a children's museum were instructed to encourage their children (N = 65; ages 4-6) to explain, explore, or engage as usual. Instructions led to different patterns of play at the exhibit: Encouragement to explain led to greater discussion of gear mechanisms, whereas encouragement to explore led to more time connecting gears. In the explain condition, parents' questions predicted their children's discussion and further testing of gears. Questions also predicted the amount of time children spent on a follow-up task. Parents' exploration predicted an increase in exploration by their children. These data indicate that minimal interventions impact parent-child interaction at a museum exhibit and that prompts to explore or explain uniquely influence parent and child behavior.
Collapse
|
178
|
Horne Z, Muradoglu M, Cimpian A. Explanation as a Cognitive Process. Trends Cogn Sci 2019; 23:187-199. [DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.12.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/05/2018] [Revised: 12/09/2018] [Accepted: 12/11/2018] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
|
179
|
Stavans M, Lin Y, Wu D, Baillargeon R. Catastrophic individuation failures in infancy: A new model and predictions. Psychol Rev 2019; 126:196-225. [PMID: 30550314 PMCID: PMC6600085 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000136] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Comparison of infant findings from the physical-reasoning and object-individuation literatures reveals a contradictory picture. On the one hand, physical-reasoning results indicate that young infants can use featural information to guide their actions on objects and to detect interaction violations (when objects interact in ways that are not physically possible) as well as change violations (when objects spontaneously undergo featural changes that are not physically possible). On the other hand, object-individuation results indicate that young infants typically cannot use featural information to detect individuation violations (when the number of objects revealed at the end of an event is less than the number of objects introduced during the event). In this article, we attempt to reconcile these two bodies of research. In a new model of early individuation, we propose that two systems help infants individuate objects in physical events: the object-file and physical-reasoning systems. Under certain conditions, disagreements between the systems result in catastrophic individuation failures, leading infants to hold no expectation at all about how many objects are present. We report experiments with 9- to 11-month-old infants (N = 216) that tested predictions from the model. After two objects emerged in alternation from behind a screen, infants detected no violation when the screen was lowered to reveal no object. Similarly, after two objects emerged in alternation from inside a box, which was then shaken, infants detected no violation when the box remained silent, as though empty. We end with new directions, suggested by our model, for research on early object representations. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Maayan Stavans
- Psychology Department, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat-Gan 5290002, Israel
| | - Yi Lin
- Psychology Department, University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign, IL 61820, USA
| | - Di Wu
- Psychology Department, Cedarville University, Cedarville, OH 45314, USA
| | - Renée Baillargeon
- Psychology Department, University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign, IL 61820, USA
| |
Collapse
|
180
|
Kayhan E, Hunnius S, O'Reilly JX, Bekkering H. Infants differentially update their internal models of a dynamic environment. Cognition 2019; 186:139-146. [PMID: 30780046 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2019.02.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/04/2017] [Revised: 01/28/2019] [Accepted: 02/04/2019] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
Unexpected events provide us with opportunities for learning about what to expect from the world around us. Using a saccadic-planning paradigm, we investigated whether and how infants and adults represent the statistics of a changing environment (i.e. build an internal model of the environment). Participants observed differently colored bees that appeared at an unexpected location every few trials. The color cues indicated whether the subsequent bees would appear at this new location (i.e. update trials) or at the same location as previously (i.e. no-update trials). Infants learned the predictive value of the color cues and updated their internal models when necessary. Unlike infants, adults had a tendency to update their models each time they observed a change in the structure. We argue that infants are open to learning from current evidence due to being less influenced by their prior knowledge. This is an advantageous learning strategy to form accurate representations in dynamic environments, which is fundamental for successful adaptation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- E Kayhan
- Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany; Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behavior, Radboud University Nijmegen, the Netherlands.
| | - S Hunnius
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behavior, Radboud University Nijmegen, the Netherlands
| | - J X O'Reilly
- University of Oxford, Oxford Centre for Functional MRI of the Brain, Nuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, John Radcliffe Hospital, Oxford, UK
| | - H Bekkering
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behavior, Radboud University Nijmegen, the Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
181
|
Ratner HH, Foley MA, Lesnick CS. Kindergarten children's event memory: the role of action prediction in remembering. Cogn Process 2019; 20:227-241. [PMID: 30739253 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-018-00900-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/20/2018] [Accepted: 12/28/2018] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
Abstract
In two studies, kindergarteners participated in a series of staged events immediately preceded by pre-event interactions that were designed to identify factors relevant to improving recall. The events were based on preschool science-related activities and the experimental pre-event involved predicting actions to occur during a target event, manipulating types of cues available to support these predictive inferences. Action prediction did improve free recall, and effects may have influenced attentional processes evoked by actions generated and enacted. Although children effectively used outcome cues to predict actions, a one-to-one relation between pre-event action prediction patterns and recall did not occur. In combination with other findings, this result may suggest that increased attention during the target event may have supported the pre-event effect rather than integration of information between the pre-event and target event. Early childhood teachers engaging children in science activities should provide explicit cues to enhance usefulness of preparatory activities for recall.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hilary Horn Ratner
- Merrill Palmer Skillman Institute, Wayne State University, 71 E. Ferry Avenue, Detroit, MI, 48207, USA.
| | - Mary Ann Foley
- Skidmore College, 50 Sherwood Trail, Saratoga Springs, NY, 12866, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
182
|
Ullman S, Dorfman N, Harari D. A model for discovering 'containment' relations. Cognition 2019; 183:67-81. [PMID: 30419508 PMCID: PMC6331663 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.11.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/26/2018] [Revised: 10/28/2018] [Accepted: 11/02/2018] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
Rapid developments in the fields of learning and object recognition have been obtained by successfully developing and using methods for learning from a large number of labeled image examples. However, such current methods cannot explain infants' learning of new concepts based on their visual experience, in particular, the ability to learn complex concepts without external guidance, as well as the natural order in which related concepts are acquired. A remarkable example of early visual learning is the category of 'containers' and the notion of 'containment'. Surprisingly, this is one of the earliest spatial relations to be learned, starting already around 3 month of age, and preceding other common relations (e.g., 'support', 'in-between'). In this work we present a model, which explains infants' capacity of learning 'containment' and related concepts by 'just looking', together with their empirical development trajectory. Learning occurs in the model fast and without external guidance, relying only on perceptual processes that are present in the first months of life. Instead of labeled training examples, the system provides its own internal supervision to guide the learning process. We show how the detection of so-called 'paradoxical occlusion' provides natural internal supervision, which guides the system to gradually acquire a range of useful containment-related concepts. Similar mechanisms of using implicit internal supervision can have broad application in other cognitive domains as well as artificial intelligent systems, because they alleviate the need for supplying extensive external supervision, and because they can guide the learning process to extract concepts that are meaningful to the observer, even if they are not by themselves obvious, or salient in the input.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Shimon Ullman
- Weizmann Institute of Science, Department of Computer Science and Applied Mathematics, 234 Herzl Street, Rehovot 7610001, Israel
| | - Nimrod Dorfman
- Weizmann Institute of Science, Department of Computer Science and Applied Mathematics, 234 Herzl Street, Rehovot 7610001, Israel
| | - Daniel Harari
- Weizmann Institute of Science, Department of Computer Science and Applied Mathematics, 234 Herzl Street, Rehovot 7610001, Israel.
| |
Collapse
|
183
|
Abstract
Motor development and psychological development are fundamentally related, but researchers typically consider them separately. In this review, we present four key features of infant motor development and show that motor skill acquisition both requires and reflects basic psychological functions. ( a) Motor development is embodied: Opportunities for action depend on the current status of the body. ( b) Motor development is embedded: Variations in the environment create and constrain possibilities for action. ( c) Motor development is enculturated: Social and cultural influences shape motor behaviors. ( d) Motor development is enabling: New motor skills create new opportunities for exploration and learning that instigate cascades of development across diverse psychological domains. For each of these key features, we show that changes in infants' bodies, environments, and experiences entail behavioral flexibility and are thus essential to psychology. Moreover, we suggest that motor development is an ideal model system for the study of psychological development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Karen E Adolph
- Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, New York 10003, USA;
| | - Justine E Hoch
- Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, New York 10003, USA;
| |
Collapse
|
184
|
Denisova K. Neurobiology, not artifacts: Challenges and guidelines for imaging the high risk infant. Neuroimage 2019; 185:624-640. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.07.023] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/01/2017] [Revised: 07/07/2018] [Accepted: 07/10/2018] [Indexed: 12/21/2022] Open
|
185
|
Stahl AE, Feigenson L. Violations of Core Knowledge Shape Early Learning. Top Cogn Sci 2019; 11:136-153. [PMID: 30369059 PMCID: PMC6360129 DOI: 10.1111/tops.12389] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/29/2016] [Revised: 09/17/2018] [Accepted: 09/17/2018] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
Research on cognitive development has revealed that even the youngest minds detect and respond to events that adults find surprising. These surprise responses suggest that infants have a basic set of "core" expectations about the world that are shared with adults and other species. However, little work has asked what purpose these surprise responses serve. Here we discuss recent evidence that violations of core knowledge offer special opportunities for learning. Infants and young children make predictions about the world on the basis of their core knowledge of objects, quantities, and social entities. We argue that when these predictions fail to match the observed data, infants and children experience an enhanced drive to seek and retain new information. This impact of surprise on learning is not equipotent. Instead, it is directed to entities that are relevant to the surprise itself; this drive propels children-even infants-to form and test new hypotheses about surprising aspects of the world. We briefly consider similarities and differences between these recent findings with infants and children, on the one hand, and findings on prediction errors in humans and non-human animals, on the other. These comparisons raise open questions that require continued inquiry, but suggest that considering phenomena across species, ages, kinds of surprise, and types of learning will ultimately help to clarify how surprise shapes thought.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Lisa Feigenson
- Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, Johns Hopkins University
| |
Collapse
|
186
|
Munnich EL, Foster MI, Keane MT. Editors' Introduction and Review: An Appraisal of Surprise: Tracing the Threads That Stitch It Together. Top Cogn Sci 2018; 11:37-49. [PMID: 30580495 DOI: 10.1111/tops.12402] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/03/2018] [Revised: 11/12/2018] [Accepted: 11/12/2018] [Indexed: 01/25/2023]
Abstract
Though the scientific study of surprise dates back to Darwin (), there was an upsurge in interest beginning in the 1960s and 70s, and this has continued to the present. Recent developments have shed much light on the cognitive mechanisms and consequences of surprise, but research has often been siloed within sub-areas of Cognitive Science. A central challenge for research on surprise is, therefore, to connect various research programs around their overlapping foci. This issue has its roots in a symposium on surprise, entitled "Triangulating Surprise: Expectations, Uncertainty, and Making Sense," at the 36th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (Quebec City, July 2014). Building on the interdisciplinary conversations that started at the symposium, this issue aims to draw attention to some promising empirical and modeling results and their theoretical implications. The present paper sets the stage for the issue by presenting a historical summary, discussing contrasting definitions of surprise, and then by tracing major threads that run through both this issue and the larger literature on surprise. Our aim is to develop broader, shared understandings of the main insights, theories, and findings regarding surprise, with a view to supporting future integration and progress.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Meadhbh I Foster
- School of Computer Science and Informatics, University College Dublin
| | - Mark T Keane
- School of Computer Science and Informatics, University College Dublin
| |
Collapse
|
187
|
Saffran JR. Statistical learning as a window into developmental disabilities. J Neurodev Disord 2018; 10:35. [PMID: 30541453 PMCID: PMC6292000 DOI: 10.1186/s11689-018-9252-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/15/2017] [Accepted: 11/14/2018] [Indexed: 01/30/2023] Open
Abstract
Until recently, most behavioral studies of children with intellectual and developmental disabilities (IDD) have used standardized assessments as a means to probe etiology and to characterize phenotypes. Over the past decade, however, tasks originally developed to investigate learning processes in typical development have been brought to bear on developmental processes in children with IDD. This brief review will focus on one learning process in particular—statistical learning—and will provide an overview of what has been learned thus far from studies using statistical learning tasks with different groups of children with IDD conditions. While a full picture is not yet available, results to date suggest that studies of learning are both feasible and informative about learning processes that may differ across diagnostic groups, particularly as they relate to language acquisition. More generally, studies focused on learning processes may be highly informative about different developmental trajectories both across groups and within groups of children.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jenny R Saffran
- Waisman Center, University of Wisconsin-Madison, 1500 Highland Ave, Madison, WI, 53705, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
188
|
Abstract
Participants in social interactions often imitate one another, thereby enhancing their affiliation. Here we probe the nature and early development of imitation-based affiliation through studies of infants’ preferences for animated characters who imitate, or are imitated by, other characters. Four experiments provide evidence that preverbal infants preferentially attend to and approach individuals who imitate others. This preferential engagement is elicited by the elements of mimicry in simple acts of helping. It does not, however, extend to the targets of imitation in these interactions. This set of findings suggests infants’ imitation-based preferences are not well explained by homophily, prestige, or familiarity. We propose instead that infants perceive imitation as an indicator of valuable attributes in a potential social partner, including the capacity and motivation for social attention and coordinated action.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lindsey J. Powell
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology
| | | |
Collapse
|
189
|
Sim ZL, Xu F. Another Look at Looking Time: Surprise as Rational Statistical Inference. Top Cogn Sci 2018; 11:154-163. [DOI: 10.1111/tops.12393] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/31/2016] [Revised: 10/04/2018] [Accepted: 10/04/2018] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Zi L. Sim
- Department of Psychology; University of California, Berkeley
| | - Fei Xu
- Department of Psychology; University of California, Berkeley
| |
Collapse
|
190
|
The Little Engine That Can: Infants’ Persistence Matters. Trends Cogn Sci 2018; 22:965-968. [DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.07.012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/04/2018] [Revised: 07/13/2018] [Accepted: 07/17/2018] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
|
191
|
Gliga T. Telling Apart Motor Noise and Exploratory Behavior, in Early Development. Front Psychol 2018; 9:1939. [PMID: 30369897 PMCID: PMC6194153 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01939] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/21/2017] [Accepted: 09/20/2018] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Infants' minutes long babbling bouts or repetitive reaching for or mouthing of whatever they can get their hands on gives very much the impression of active exploration, a building block for early learning. But how can we tell apart active exploration from the activity of an immature motor system, attempting but failing to achieve goal directed behavior? I will focus here on evidence that infants increase motor activity and variability when faced with opportunities to gather new information (about their own bodies or the world) and propose this as a guiding principle for separating variability generated for exploration from noise. I will discuss mechanisms generating movement variability, and suggests that, in the various forms it takes, from deliberate hypothesis testing to increasing environmental variability, it could be exploited for learning. However, understanding how variability in motor acts contributes to early learning will require more in-depth investigations of both the nature of and the contextual modulation of this variability.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Teodora Gliga
- School of Psychology, University of East Anglia, Norwich, United Kingdom
- Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, Birkbeck, University of London, London, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
192
|
Lucca K, Wilbourn MP. The what and the how: Information-seeking pointing gestures facilitate learning labels and functions. J Exp Child Psychol 2018; 178:417-436. [PMID: 30318380 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2018.08.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/01/2017] [Revised: 08/07/2018] [Accepted: 08/08/2018] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
Infants' pointing gestures are clear and salient markers of their interest. As a result, they afford infants with a targeted and precise way of eliciting information from others. The current study investigated whether, similar to older children's question asking, infants' pointing gestures are produced to obtain information. Specifically, in a single experimental study, we examined whether 18-month-olds (N = 36) point to request specific types of information and how this translates into learning across domains. We elicited pointing from infants in a context that would naturally lend itself to information seeking (i.e., out-of-reach novel objects). In response to infants' points, an experimenter provided a label, a function, or no information for each pointed-to object. We assessed infants' persistence after receiving different types of information and their subsequent ability to form label-object or function-object associations. When infants pointed and received no information or functions, they persisted significantly more often than when they pointed and received labels, suggesting that they were most satisfied with receiving labels for objects compared with functions or no information. Infants successfully mapped both labels and functions onto objects. When infants expressed their interest in a novel object in a manner other than pointing, such as reaching, they (a) were equally satisfied with receiving object labels, functions, or no information and (b) did not successfully learn either labels or functions. Together, these findings demonstrate that infants' pointing gestures are specific requests for labels that facilitate the acquisition of various types of information. In doing so, this work connects the research on information seeking during infancy to the established literature on question asking during childhood.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kelsey Lucca
- Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, NC 27708, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
193
|
Sutherland SL, Cimpian A. Developmental evidence for a link between the inherence bias in explanation and psychological essentialism. J Exp Child Psychol 2018; 177:265-281. [PMID: 30286389 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2018.06.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/09/2018] [Revised: 06/07/2018] [Accepted: 06/22/2018] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
The assumption that natural and social categories have deeper "essences" is a fundamental feature of the conceptual system, with wide-ranging consequences for behavior. What are the developmental origins of this assumption? We propose that essentialism emerges in part from a bias in the process of generating explanations that leads reasoners to overuse inherent or intrinsic features. Consistent with this proposal, the inherence bias in 4-year-olds' explanations predicted the strength of their essentialist beliefs (Study 1; N = 64), and manipulations of the inherence bias in 4- to 7-year-olds (Studies 2 and 3; N = 112 each) led to subsequent changes in the essentialist beliefs of children who attended to the manipulation. These findings contribute to our understanding of the origins of essentialism.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Shelbie L Sutherland
- Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario M5S 3G3, Canada.
| | - Andrei Cimpian
- Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, NY 10003, USA
| |
Collapse
|
194
|
Benitez VL, Saffran JR. Predictable Events Enhance Word Learning in Toddlers. Curr Biol 2018; 28:2787-2793.e4. [PMID: 30122525 PMCID: PMC6148368 DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.06.017] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/13/2018] [Revised: 05/16/2018] [Accepted: 06/11/2018] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
Sensitivity to the predictability of the environment supports young children's learning in many domains [1, 2], including language [3-6]; perception [7, 8]; and the processing of objects, space, and time [1, 9]. Predictable regularities allow observers to generate expectations about upcoming events and to learn from violations of those expectations [10, 11]. Given the benefits of detecting both predictable and unpredictable events, a key question concerns which types of input facilitate learning in young children. In the current research, we assessed the effects of predictability on toddlers' word learning by embedding word-learning moments within events that were either predicted or violated predictions. 2-year-olds observed a continuous visual sequence in which novel objects were revealed from one of four locations in a predictable spatiotemporal pattern (1, 2, 3, 4). Objects were then labeled either during events that were predicted by the sequence (1, 2, 3, 4) or events that violated the sequence (1, 2, 3, 2). Results from two studies revealed better word learning for objects labeled during predictable events than objects labeled during unpredictable events. These findings suggest that predictable events create advantageous learning moments for toddlers, with implications for the role played by predictable input in early development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Viridiana L Benitez
- Department of Psychology, Arizona State University, 950 S. McAllister Ave., Tempe, AZ 85287, USA.
| | - Jenny R Saffran
- Waisman Center, University of Wisconsin, Madison, 1500 Highland Ave., Madison, WI 53705, USA
| |
Collapse
|
195
|
Abstract
Belief revision can occur at multiple levels of abstraction, including lower-level and higher-order beliefs. It remains unclear, however, how conflicting evidence interacts with prior beliefs to encourage higher-order belief revision. This study explores how 4- and 5-year-olds (N = 96) respond to evidence that directly conflicts with their causal higher-order beliefs. When shown a single event that directly violated a strongly supported prior belief, preschoolers largely maintained their initial higher-order belief. However, when the prior belief was more weakly supported and the counterevidence was stronger, children changed their minds. These findings indicate that young children can revise their higher-order beliefs and, furthermore, do so depending on the strength of both the evidence and their prior beliefs.
Collapse
|
196
|
Ronfard S, Zambrana IM, Hermansen TK, Kelemen D. Question-asking in childhood: A review of the literature and a framework for understanding its development. DEVELOPMENTAL REVIEW 2018. [DOI: 10.1016/j.dr.2018.05.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 60] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
|
197
|
Bramley NR, Gerstenberg T, Tenenbaum JB, Gureckis TM. Intuitive experimentation in the physical world. Cogn Psychol 2018; 105:9-38. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2018.05.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/12/2017] [Revised: 03/27/2018] [Accepted: 05/11/2018] [Indexed: 10/14/2022]
|
198
|
Zosh JM, Hirsh-Pasek K, Hopkins EJ, Jensen H, Liu C, Neale D, Solis SL, Whitebread D. Accessing the Inaccessible: Redefining Play as a Spectrum. Front Psychol 2018; 9:1124. [PMID: 30116208 PMCID: PMC6084083 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01124] [Citation(s) in RCA: 45] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/05/2018] [Accepted: 06/12/2018] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Defining play has plagued researchers and philosophers for years. From describing play as an inaccessible concept due to its complexity, to providing checklists of features, the field has struggled with how to conceptualize and operationalize “play.” This theoretical piece reviews the literature about both play and learning and suggests that by viewing play as a spectrum – that ranges from free play (no guidance or support) to guided play and games (including purposeful adult support while maintaining playful elements), we better capture the true essence of play and explain its relationship to learning. Insights from the Science of Learning allow us to better understand why play supports learning across social and academic domains. By changing the lens through which we conceptualize play, we account for previous findings in a cohesive way while also proposing new avenues of exploration for the field to study the role of learning through play across age and context.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jennifer M Zosh
- The Pennsylvania State University Brandywine, Media, PA, United States
| | - Kathy Hirsh-Pasek
- Department of Psychology, Temple University, Philadelphia, PA, United States.,The Brookings Institution, Washington, DC, United States
| | - Emily J Hopkins
- Department of Psychology, Temple University, Philadelphia, PA, United States
| | | | - Claire Liu
- Graduate School of Education, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, United States
| | - Dave Neale
- School of Education, University of Delaware, Newark, DE, United States
| | - S Lynneth Solis
- Graduate School of Education, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, United States
| | - David Whitebread
- Homerton College, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
199
|
Asking the right questions about the psychology of human inquiry: Nine open challenges. Psychon Bull Rev 2018; 26:1548-1587. [DOI: 10.3758/s13423-018-1470-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
200
|
Muentener P, Herrig E, Schulz L. The Efficiency of Infants' Exploratory Play Is Related to Longer-Term Cognitive Development. Front Psychol 2018; 9:635. [PMID: 29904360 PMCID: PMC5991261 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00635] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/30/2017] [Accepted: 04/16/2018] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
In this longitudinal study we examined the stability of exploratory play in infancy and its relation to cognitive development in early childhood. We assessed infants' (N = 130, mean age at enrollment = 12.02 months, SD = 3.5 months; range: 5-19 months) exploratory play four times over 9 months. Exploratory play was indexed by infants' attention to novelty, inductive generalizations, efficiency of exploration, face preferences, and imitative learning. We assessed cognitive development at the fourth visit for the full sample, and again at age three for a subset of the sample (n = 38). The only measure that was stable over infancy was the efficiency of exploration. Additionally, infants' efficiency score predicted vocabulary size and distinguished at-risk infants recruited from early intervention sites from those not at risk. Follow-up analyses at age three provided additional evidence for the importance of the efficiency measure: more efficient exploration was correlated with higher IQ scores. These results suggest that the efficiency of infants' exploratory play can be informative about longer-term cognitive development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Paul Muentener
- Department of Psychology, Tufts University, Medford, MA, United States
| | - Elise Herrig
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Medford, MA, United States
| | - Laura Schulz
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Medford, MA, United States
| |
Collapse
|