1
|
Schmidgen J, Heinen T, Konrad K, Bender S. From preparation to post-processing: Insights into evoked and induced cortical activity during pre-cued motor reactions in children and adolescents. Neuroimage 2024; 297:120735. [PMID: 39002787 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2024.120735] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/07/2024] [Revised: 05/02/2024] [Accepted: 07/11/2024] [Indexed: 07/15/2024] Open
Abstract
INTRODUCTION The motor system undergoes significant development throughout childhood and adolescence. The contingent negative variation (CNV), a brain response reflecting preparation for upcoming actions, offers valuable insights into these changes. However, previous CNV studies of motor preparation have primarily focused on adults, leaving a gap in our understanding of how cortical activity related to motor planning and execution matures in children and adolescents. METHODS The study addresses this gap by investigating the maturation of motor preparation, pre-activation, and post-processing in 46 healthy, right-handed children and adolescents aged 5-16 years. To overcome the resolution limitations of previous studies, we combined 64-electrode high-density Electroencephalography (EEG) and advanced analysis techniques, such as event-related potentials (ERPs), mu-rhythm desynchronization as well as source localization approaches. The combined analyses provided an in-depth understanding of cortical activity during motor control. RESULTS Our data showed that children exhibited prolonged reaction times, increased errors, and a distinct pattern of cortical activation compared to adolescents. The findings suggest that the supplementary motor area (SMA) plays a progressively stronger role in motor planning and response evaluation as children age. Additionally, we observe a decrease in sensory processing and post-movement activity with development, potentially reflecting increased efficiency. Interestingly, adolescent subjects, unlike young adults in previous studies, did not yet show contralateral activation of motor areas during the motor preparation phase (late CNV). CONCLUSION The progressive increase in SMA activation and distinct cortical activation patterns in younger participants suggest immature motor areas. These immature regions might be a primary cause underlying the age-related increase in motor action control efficiency. Additionally, the study demonstrates a prolonged maturation of cortical motor areas, extending well into early adulthood, challenging the assumption that motor control is fully developed by late adolescence. This research, extending fundamental knowledge of motor control development, offers valuable insights that lay the foundation for understanding and treating motor control difficulties.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Julia Schmidgen
- Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, University of Cologne, University Hospital Cologne, Germany.
| | - Theresa Heinen
- Section Child Neuropsychology, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, RWTH, University Hospital, Aachen, Germany; JARA-BRAIN Institute II, Molecular Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, Forschungszentrum Jülich GmbH and RWTH Aachen University, Germany
| | - Kerstin Konrad
- Section Child Neuropsychology, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, RWTH, University Hospital, Aachen, Germany; JARA-BRAIN Institute II, Molecular Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, Forschungszentrum Jülich GmbH and RWTH Aachen University, Germany
| | - Stephan Bender
- Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, University of Cologne, University Hospital Cologne, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Sustained visuospatial attention enhances lateralized anticipatory ERP activity in sensory areas. Brain Struct Funct 2021; 226:457-470. [PMID: 33392666 DOI: 10.1007/s00429-020-02192-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/07/2020] [Accepted: 11/27/2020] [Indexed: 01/02/2023]
Abstract
The existence of neural correlates of spatial attention is not limited to the reactive stage of stimulus processing: neural activities subtending spatial attention are deployed well ahead of stimulus onset. ERP evidence supporting this proactive (top-down) attentional control is based on trial-by-trial S1-S2 paradigms, where the onset of a directional cue (S1) indicates on which side attention must be directed to respond to an upcoming target stimulus (S2). Crucially, S1 onset trigger both attention and motor preparation, therefore, these paradigms are not ideal to demonstrate the effect of attention at preparatory stage of processing. To isolate top-down anticipatory attention, the present study used a sustained attention paradigm based on a steady cue that indicates the attended side constantly throughout an entire block of trials, without any onset of an attentional cue. The main result consists in the description of the attention effect on the visual negativity (vN) component, a growing neural activity starting before stimulus presentation in extrastriate visual areas. The vN was consistently lateralized in the hemisphere contralateral to the attended side, regardless of the hand to be used. At the opposite, the lateralized motor activity emerged long after, confirming that the hand-selection process followed the spatial attention orientation process. The present study confirms the anticipatory nature of the vN component and corroborate its role in terms of preparatory visuospatial attention.
Collapse
|
3
|
Li J, Thakor N, Bezerianos A. Unilateral Exoskeleton Imposes Significantly Different Hemispherical Effect in Parietooccipital Region, but Not in Other Regions. Sci Rep 2018; 8:13470. [PMID: 30194397 PMCID: PMC6128944 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-018-31828-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/16/2018] [Accepted: 08/28/2018] [Indexed: 11/10/2022] Open
Abstract
In modern society, increasing people suffering from locomotor disabilities need an assistive exoskeleton to help them improve or restore ambulation. When walking is assisted by an exoskeleton, brain activities are altered as the closed-loop between brain and lower limbs is affected by the exoskeleton. Intuitively, a unilateral exoskeleton imposes differential effect on brain hemispheres (i.e., hemispherical effect) according to contralateral control mechanism. However, it is unclear whether hemispherical effect appears in whole hemisphere or particular region. To this end, we explored hemispherical effect on different brain regions using EEG data collected from 30 healthy participants during overground walking. The results showed that hemispherical effect was significantly different between regions when a unilateral exoskeleton was employed for walking assistance and no significance was observed for walking without the exoskeleton. Post-hoc t-test analysis revealed that hemispherical effect in the parietooccipital region significantly differed from other regions. In the parietooccipital region, a greater hemispherical effect was observed in beta band for exoskeleton-assisted walking compared to walking without exoskeleton, which was also found in the source analysis. These findings deepen the understanding of hemispherical effect of unilateral exoskeleton on brain and could aid the development of more efficient and suitable exoskeleton for walking assistance.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Junhua Li
- Singapore Institute for Neurotechnology (SINAPSE), Centre for Life Sciences, National University of Singapore, Singapore, 117456, Singapore.
- Laboratory for Brain-bionic Intelligence and Computational Neuroscience, Wuyi University, Jiangmen, 529020, China.
- Centre for Multidisciplinary Convergence Computing (CMCC), School of Computer Science and Engineering, Northwestern Polytechnical University, Xi'an, 710072, China.
| | - Nitish Thakor
- Singapore Institute for Neurotechnology (SINAPSE), Centre for Life Sciences, National University of Singapore, Singapore, 117456, Singapore
| | - Anastasios Bezerianos
- Singapore Institute for Neurotechnology (SINAPSE), Centre for Life Sciences, National University of Singapore, Singapore, 117456, Singapore
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Powell PS, Strunk J, James T, Polyn SM, Duarte A. Decoding selective attention to context memory: An aging study. Neuroimage 2018; 181:95-107. [PMID: 29991445 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.06.085] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/07/2018] [Revised: 06/18/2018] [Accepted: 06/30/2018] [Indexed: 11/26/2022] Open
Abstract
Emerging evidence has suggested that the tendency for older adults to bind too much contextual information during encoding (i.e., hyper-binding) may contribute to poorer memory for relevant contextual information during retrieval. While these findings are consistent with theories of age-related declines in selective attention and inhibitory control, the degree to which older adults are able to selectively attend to relevant contextual information during encoding is unknown. To better understand the neural dynamics associated with selective attention during encoding, the current study applied multivariate pattern analyses (MVPA) to oscillatory EEG in order to track moment-to-moment shifts of attention between relevant and irrelevant contextual information during encoding. Young and older adults studied pictures of objects in the presence of two contextual features: a color and a scene, and their attention was directed to the object's relationship with one of those contexts (i.e., target context). Results showed that patterns of oscillatory power successfully predicted whether selective attention was directed to a scene or color, across age groups. Individual differences in overall classification performance were associated with individual differences in target context memory accuracy during retrieval. However, changes in classification performance within a trial, suggestive of fluctuations in selective attention, predicted individual differences in hyper-binding. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to use MPVA techniques to decode attention during episodic encoding and the impact of attentional shifts toward distracting information on age-related context memory impairments and hyper-binding. These results are consistent with the as-of-yet unsubstantiated theory that age-related declines in context memory may be attributable to poorer selective attention and/or greater inhibitory deficits in older adults.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Patrick S Powell
- Georgia Institute of Technology, School of Psychology, 654 Cherry Street NW, Atlanta, GA, 30332-0170, United States.
| | - Jonathan Strunk
- Georgia Institute of Technology, School of Psychology, 654 Cherry Street NW, Atlanta, GA, 30332-0170, United States
| | - Taylor James
- Georgia Institute of Technology, School of Psychology, 654 Cherry Street NW, Atlanta, GA, 30332-0170, United States
| | - Sean M Polyn
- Vanderbilt University, Department of Psychology, PMB 407817, 2301 Vanderbilt Place, Nashville, TN, 37240-7817, United States
| | - Audrey Duarte
- Georgia Institute of Technology, School of Psychology, 654 Cherry Street NW, Atlanta, GA, 30332-0170, United States
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Sobierajewicz J, Przekoracka-Krawczyk A, Jaśkowski W, Verwey WB, van der Lubbe R. The influence of motor imagery on the learning of a fine hand motor skill. Exp Brain Res 2016; 235:305-320. [PMID: 27714404 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-016-4794-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/19/2016] [Accepted: 09/29/2016] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
Abstract
Motor imagery has been argued to affect the acquisition of motor skills. The present study examined the specificity of motor imagery on the learning of a fine hand motor skill by employing a modified discrete sequence production task: the Go/NoGo DSP task. After an informative cue, a response sequence had either to be executed, imagined, or withheld. To establish learning effects, the experiment was divided into a practice phase and a test phase. In the latter phase, we compared mean response times and accuracy during the execution of unfamiliar sequences, familiar imagined sequences, and familiar executed sequences. The electroencephalogram was measured in the practice phase to compare activity between motor imagery, motor execution, and a control condition in which responses should be withheld. Event-related potentials (ERPs) and event-related lateralizations (ERLs) showed strong similarities above cortical motor areas on trials requiring motor imagery and motor execution, while a major difference was found with trials on which the response sequence should be withheld. Behavioral results from the test phase showed that response times and accuracy improved after physical and mental practice relative to unfamiliar sequences (so-called sequence-specific learning effects), although the effect of motor learning by motor imagery was smaller than the effect of physical practice. These findings confirm that motor imagery also resembles motor execution in the case of a fine hand motor skill.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jagna Sobierajewicz
- Department of Cognitive Psychology, University of Finance and Management, Warsaw, Poland. .,Vision and Neuroscience Laboratory, NanoBioMedical Centre, Adam Mickiewicza University, Umultowska 85, 61-614, Poznan, Poland.
| | - Anna Przekoracka-Krawczyk
- Vision and Neuroscience Laboratory, NanoBioMedical Centre, Adam Mickiewicza University, Umultowska 85, 61-614, Poznan, Poland.,Laboratory of Vision Science and Optometry, Faculty of Physics, Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznan, Poland
| | - Wojciech Jaśkowski
- Institute of Computing Science, Poznan University of Technology, Poznan, Poland
| | - Willem B Verwey
- Cognitive Psychology and Ergonomics, University of Twente, Enschede, The Netherlands
| | - Rob van der Lubbe
- Department of Cognitive Psychology, University of Finance and Management, Warsaw, Poland.,Cognitive Psychology and Ergonomics, University of Twente, Enschede, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Chen J, Valsecchi M, Gegenfurtner KR. LRP predicts smooth pursuit eye movement onset during the ocular tracking of self-generated movements. J Neurophysiol 2016; 116:18-29. [PMID: 27009159 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00184.2016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/02/2016] [Accepted: 03/21/2016] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Several studies have indicated that human observers are very efficient at tracking self-generated hand movements with their gaze, yet it is not clear whether this is simply a by-product of the predictability of self-generated actions or if it results from a deeper coupling of the somatomotor and oculomotor systems. In a first behavioral experiment we compared pursuit performance as observers either followed their own finger or tracked a dot whose motion was externally generated but mimicked their finger motion. We found that even when the dot motion was completely predictable in terms of both onset time and kinematics, pursuit was not identical to that produced as the observers tracked their finger, as evidenced by increased rate of catch-up saccades and by the fact that in the initial phase of the movement gaze was lagging behind the dot, whereas it was ahead of the finger. In a second experiment we recorded EEG in the attempt to find a direct link between the finger motor preparation, indexed by the lateralized readiness potential (LRP) and the latency of smooth pursuit. After taking into account finger movement onset variability, we observed larger LRP amplitudes associated with earlier smooth pursuit onset across trials. The same held across subjects, where average LRP onset correlated with average eye latency. The evidence from both experiments concurs to indicate that a strong coupling exists between the motor systems leading to eye and finger movements and that simple top-down predictive signals are unlikely to support optimal coordination.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jing Chen
- Abteilung Allgemeine Psychologie, Justus-Liebig-Universität Giessen, Giessen, Germany
| | - Matteo Valsecchi
- Abteilung Allgemeine Psychologie, Justus-Liebig-Universität Giessen, Giessen, Germany
| | - Karl R Gegenfurtner
- Abteilung Allgemeine Psychologie, Justus-Liebig-Universität Giessen, Giessen, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Juravle G, Heed T, Spence C, Röder B. Neural correlates of tactile perception during pre-, peri-, and post-movement. Exp Brain Res 2016; 234:1293-305. [DOI: 10.1007/s00221-016-4589-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/31/2015] [Accepted: 01/30/2016] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
|
8
|
Liu Q, Balsters JH, Baechinger M, van der Groen O, Wenderoth N, Mantini D. Estimating a neutral reference for electroencephalographic recordings: the importance of using a high-density montage and a realistic head model. J Neural Eng 2015; 12:056012. [PMID: 26305167 PMCID: PMC4719184 DOI: 10.1088/1741-2560/12/5/056012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 80] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/03/2022]
Abstract
Objective. In electroencephalography (EEG) measurements, the signal of each recording electrode is contrasted with a reference electrode or a combination of electrodes. The estimation of a neutral reference is a long-standing issue in EEG data analysis, which has motivated the proposal of different re-referencing methods, among which linked-mastoid re-referencing (LMR), average re-referencing (AR) and reference electrode standardization technique (REST). In this study we quantitatively assessed the extent to which the use of a high-density montage and a realistic head model can impact on the optimal estimation of a neutral reference for EEG recordings. Approach. Using simulated recordings generated by projecting specific source activity over the sensors, we assessed to what extent AR, REST and LMR may distort the scalp topography. We examined the impact electrode coverage has on AR and REST, and how accurate the REST reconstruction is for realistic and less realistic (three-layer and single-layer spherical) head models, and with possible uncertainty in the electrode positions. We assessed LMR, AR and REST also in the presence of typical EEG artifacts that are mixed in the recordings. Finally, we applied them to real EEG data collected in a target detection experiment to corroborate our findings on simulated data. Main results. Both AR and REST have relatively low reconstruction errors compared to LMR, and that REST is less sensitive than AR and LMR to artifacts mixed in the EEG data. For both AR and REST, high electrode density yields low re-referencing reconstruction errors. A realistic head model is critical for REST, leading to a more accurate estimate of a neutral reference compared to spherical head models. With a low-density montage, REST shows a more reliable reconstruction than AR either with a realistic or a three-layer spherical head model. Conversely, with a high-density montage AR yields better results unless precise information on electrode positions is available. Significance. Our study is the first to quantitatively assess the performance of EEG re-referencing techniques in relation to the use of a high-density montage and a realistic head model. We hope our study will help researchers in the choice of the most effective re-referencing approach for their EEG studies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Quanying Liu
- Neural Control of Movement Laboratory, ETH Zurich, 8057 Zurich, Switzerland. Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford OX1 3UD, UK
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
9
|
Lateralized power spectra of the EEG as an index of visuospatial attention. Adv Cogn Psychol 2013; 9:184-201. [PMID: 24605177 PMCID: PMC3902831 DOI: 10.2478/v10053-008-0144-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/28/2012] [Accepted: 05/27/2013] [Indexed: 11/29/2022] Open
Abstract
The electroencephalogram (EEG) was measured in an endogenous orienting paradigm
where symbolic cues indicated the likely side of to-be-discriminated targets.
Combined results of event-related lateralizations (ERLs) and a newly derived
measure from wavelet analyses that we applied on the raw EEG and individual
event-related potentials (ERPs), the lateralized power spectra (LPS) and the
LPS-ERP, respectively, confirmed the common view that endogenous orienting
operates by anterior processes, probably originating from the frontal eye
fields, modulating processing in parietal and occipital areas. The LPS data
indicated that modulation takes place by increased inhibition of the irrelevant
visual field and/or disinhibition of the relevant to-be-attended visual field.
Combined use of ERLs, the LPS, and the LPS-ERP indicated that most of the
involved processes can be characterized as externally evoked, either or not with
clear individual differences as some evoked effects were only visible in the
LPS-ERERP, whereas few processes seemed to have an internally induced nature.
Use of the LPS and the LPS-ERP may be advantageous as it enables to determine
the involvement of internally generated lateralized processes that are not
strictly bound to an event like stimulus onset.
Collapse
|
10
|
Levels of visuo-spatial selection: An ERP study of negative priming. Brain Cogn 2013; 83:203-17. [DOI: 10.1016/j.bandc.2013.08.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/12/2012] [Revised: 06/12/2013] [Accepted: 08/23/2013] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
|
11
|
Gherri E, Forster B. The orienting of attention during eye and hand movements: ERP evidence for similar frame of reference but different spatially specific modulations of tactile processing. Biol Psychol 2012; 91:172-84. [DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.06.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/29/2011] [Revised: 06/23/2012] [Accepted: 06/24/2012] [Indexed: 10/28/2022]
|
12
|
Juravle G, Deubel H, Spence C. Attention and suppression affect tactile perception in reach-to-grasp movements. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2011; 138:302-10. [PMID: 21872190 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2011.08.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/21/2011] [Revised: 07/20/2011] [Accepted: 08/02/2011] [Indexed: 10/17/2022] Open
Abstract
Reaching with the hand is characterized by a decrease in sensitivity to tactile stimuli presented to the moving hand. Here, we investigated whether tactile suppression can be canceled by attentional orienting. In a first experiment, participants performed a dual-task involving a goal-directed movement paired with the speeded detection of a tactile pulse. The pulse was either delivered to the moving or stationary hand, during movement preparation, execution, or the post-movement phase. Furthermore, stimulation was delivered with equal probability to either hand, or with a higher probability to either the moving or resting hand. The results highlighted faster RTs under conditions of higher probability of stimulation delivery to both moving and resting hands, thus indicating an attentional effect. For the motor preparation period, RTs were faster only at the resting hand under conditions where tactile stimulation was more likely to be delivered there. In a second experiment, a non-speeded perceptual task was used as a secondary task and tactile discrimination thresholds were recorded. Tactile stimulation was delivered concomitantly at both index fingers either in the movement preparation period (both before and after the selection of the movement effector had taken place), in the motor execution period, or, in a control condition, in the time-window of motor execution, but the movement of the hand was restrained. In the preparation period, tactile thresholds were comparable for the two timings of stimulation delivery; i.e., before and after the selection of the movement effector had taken place. These results therefore suggest that shortly prior to, and during, the execution of goal-directed movements, a combined facilitatory and inhibitory influence acts on tactile perception.
Collapse
|
13
|
De Kleine E, Van der Lubbe RH. Decreased load on general motor preparation and visual-working memory while preparing familiar as compared to unfamiliar movement sequences. Brain Cogn 2011; 75:126-34. [DOI: 10.1016/j.bandc.2010.10.013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/28/2010] [Revised: 10/25/2010] [Accepted: 10/29/2010] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
|
14
|
Van der Lubbe RH, Abrahamse EL. The premotor theory of attention and the Simon effect. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2011; 136:259-64. [PMID: 20940067 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.09.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/31/2010] [Revised: 09/14/2010] [Accepted: 09/18/2010] [Indexed: 11/26/2022] Open
Abstract
In the paper by Hommel (2011-this issue), the roles of the theory of event coding (TEC) and the premotor theory of attention (PMTA) for the Simon effect were considered. PMTA was treated by Hommel in terms of the proposal that attentional orienting can be viewed as the preparation of a saccade towards a certain location, and was dismissed as providing no useful contribution for an attentional explanation of the Simon effect. Here we considered a more recent and broader conception of the PMTA, compared this approach with TEC, and confronted both approaches with a few studies focusing on the role of spatial attention for the Simon effect. It was argued that PMTA may account more easily for various studies examining the influence of spatial attention on the Simon effect. We concluded our paper by listing some elements that an overall encompassing theory on the Simon effect should contain.
Collapse
|
15
|
Gherri E, Eimer M. Manual response preparation disrupts spatial attention: an electrophysiological investigation of links between action and attention. Neuropsychologia 2010; 48:961-9. [PMID: 19944707 PMCID: PMC2854796 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.11.017] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/07/2009] [Revised: 10/05/2009] [Accepted: 11/20/2009] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Previous behavioural and neuroscience studies have shown that the systems involved in the control of attention and action are functionally and anatomically linked. We used behavioural and event-related brain potential measures to investigate whether such links are mandatory or merely optional. Cues presented at the start of each trial instructed participants to shift attention to the left or right side and to simultaneously prepare to a finger movement with their left or right hand. In different trials, cues were followed by a central Go signal, requiring execution of the prepared manual response (motor task), or by a peripheral visual stimulus, which required a target-non-target discrimination only when presented on the cued side (attention task). Lateralised ERP components indicative of covert attention shifts were found when attention and action were directed to the same side (same side condition), but not when attention and action were directed to opposite sides (opposite sides condition). Likewise, effects of spatial attention on the processing of peripheral visual stimuli were present only when attention and action were directed to the same side, but not in the opposite sides condition. These results demonstrate that preparing a manual response on one side severely disrupts the attentional selection of visual stimuli on the other side, and suggest that it is not possible to simultaneously direct attention and action to different locations in space. They support the hypothesis that the control of spatial attention and action are implemented by shared brain circuits, and are therefore linked in a mandatory fashion.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elena Gherri
- School of Psychology, Birkbeck College, University of London, Malet Street, London, WC1E 7HX, United Kingdom.
| | | |
Collapse
|
16
|
Abstract
Neuroimaging studies have provided evidence that a bilateral frontal-parietal network is involved in voluntary attentional control. However, because those studies used instructive cue stimuli, some of the activity may have been due to interactions between cue processing and voluntary orienting. Here, we show that self-initiated voluntary orienting, in the absence of any cue stimulus, evokes activity in this frontal-parietal network. In contrast to the typical symmetric activity observed with cued attentional shifts, self-initiated shifts showed a hemispheric asymmetry consistent with studies of unilateral neglect patients. Specifically, the right hemisphere was equally involved in orienting to either visual field, whereas the left hemisphere was biased toward the contralateral field. Our data show that the asymmetry of attentional control can be revealed in neuroimaging of healthy subjects, when voluntary orienting is effectively isolated.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Joseph B Hopfinger
- Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, North Carolina 27599-3270, USA.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
17
|
Tzelepi A, Laskaris N, Amditis A, Kapoula Z. Cortical activity preceding vertical saccades: a MEG study. Brain Res 2010; 1321:105-16. [PMID: 20079341 DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.01.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/31/2009] [Revised: 10/06/2009] [Accepted: 01/01/2010] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
Abstract
Previous studies have shown that upward saccade latencies are faster than downward saccade latencies in certain tasks. This asymmetry does not appear to represent a general main effect of the visual, or the vertical oculomotor system. In this study we examined the cortical activity underlying this latency asymmetry. We used MEG to assess cortical activity related to horizontal and vertical saccade preparation, and eye movement recordings to assess saccade latencies in a modified delay task. The reconstructed cortical activity was examined with respect to the onset of the target stimulus and the onset of the saccade. Upward saccades were faster than downward saccades, in agreement with previous studies. Although to a large extent, horizontal and vertical targets activated similar areas, there were also some differences. The earlier difference was found 100-150 ms after target onset over the right supramarginal gyrus when subjects attended to location-cues. Down cues activated this area faster than up cues. Moreover, cue-related activity was stronger over the left frontal cortex for up than down cues. In contrast, saccade-related activity over the same area was stronger when preceding downward than upward saccades. The results suggest that stimuli in the upper and lower visual field may have different impacts on accessing networks related to visual attention and motor preparation resulting in different behavioral asymmetries.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Areti Tzelepi
- Iris Group, LPPA CNRS-Collège de France, Paris, France.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
18
|
Kranczioch C, Mathews S, Dean PJA, Sterr A. On the equivalence of executed and imagined movements: evidence from lateralized motor and nonmotor potentials. Hum Brain Mapp 2009; 30:3275-86. [PMID: 19253343 DOI: 10.1002/hbm.20748] [Citation(s) in RCA: 44] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
The neural simulation theory assumes that motor imagery and motor execution draw on a shared set of mechanisms underlying motor cognition. Evidence is accumulating that motor imagery and motor execution have many common features. The extent of the similarity and whether it spreads into the preparation phase is however unclear. This study used electroencephalographic recordings to compare the effects of providing advance information about upcoming movements on preparatory processing in a motor imagery and execution paradigm. Event-related potential data were recorded in a priming task where participants were cued to perform simple or complex finger movements. We hypothesized that a high degree of functional similarity of motor imagery and motor execution should be reflected in similar alterations of lateralized preparatory activity. Lateralized preparatory activity was indeed very similar, showing both motor-related (lateralized readiness potential, LRP) and cognitive components (anterior directing-attention negativity or ADAN, late directing-attention positivity or LDAP). Dipole analysis revealed that LRP, ADAN, and LDAP sources were very comparable for motor imagination and execution. Results generally support the idea of common underlying functional networks subserving both the preparation for execution and imagery of movements. They also provide a broader context for this notion by revealing similarities in cognitive components associated with the movement tasks.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Cornelia Kranczioch
- Biomagnetic Center, Department of Neurology, University Hospital Jena, Jena, Germany
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
19
|
Gherri E, Van Velzen J, Eimer M. The instructed context of a motor task modulates covert response preparation and shifts of spatial attention. Psychophysiology 2009; 46:655-67. [DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00800.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
|
20
|
Baldauf D, Deubel H. Attentional selection of multiple goal positions before rapid hand movement sequences: an event-related potential study. J Cogn Neurosci 2009; 21:18-29. [PMID: 18510446 DOI: 10.1162/jocn.2008.21021] [Citation(s) in RCA: 47] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
Abstract
A dot-probe paradigm was used to provide physiological evidence for the parallel selection of multiple movement goals before rapid hand movement sequences. Participants executed a sequence of manual pointing movements to two out of three possible goal positions. During movement preparation, a task-irrelevant visual transient (a dot probe) was flashed either at one of both movement goals, or at the third, movement-irrelevant location. The results revealed that the N1 component induced by the presentation of the dot was enhanced if the dot was flashed at one of the movement goals, indicating that both target positions were attended before the initialization of the movement sequence. A second experiment showed that movement-irrelevant locations between the movement goals were not attended, suggesting that attention splits into spatially distinct foci.
Collapse
|
21
|
Gherri E, Eimer M. Links between eye movement preparation and the attentional processing of tactile events: an event-related brain potential study. Clin Neurophysiol 2008; 119:2587-97. [PMID: 18786857 PMCID: PMC2781105 DOI: 10.1016/j.clinph.2008.07.214] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/22/2008] [Revised: 07/23/2008] [Accepted: 07/28/2008] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE We investigated whether the covert preparation of saccadic eye movements results in spatially specific modulations of somatosensory processing. METHODS ERPs were recorded in a spatial cueing experiment where auditory cues preceded tactile stimuli delivered to the left or right hand. In the Saccade task, cues signalled that an eye movement towards the left or right hand had to be prepared. In the Covert Attention task, cues signalled the direction of a covert shift of tactile attention. RESULTS A lateralized component previously observed during cued shifts of spatial attention (ADAN) was elicited in the cue-target interval in both tasks. The somatosensory N140 component was enhanced for tactile stimuli presented to the hand on the cued side. This modulation was present not just in the Covert Attention task, but also in the Saccade task. Longer-latency effects of spatial cueing were only present in the Covert Attention task. CONCLUSIONS Covert shifts of attention and saccade preparation have similar effects on early stages of tactile processing, suggesting that both are mediated by overlapping control processes. SIGNIFICANCE These findings support the premotor theory of attention by demonstrating that the programming of eye movements has spatially selective effects on somatosensory processing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elena Gherri
- School of Psychology, Birkbeck College, University of London, Malet Street, WC1E 7HX London, UK.
| | | |
Collapse
|
22
|
Gherri E, Driver J, Eimer M. Eye movement preparation causes spatially-specific modulation of auditory processing: new evidence from event-related brain potentials. Brain Res 2008; 1224:88-101. [PMID: 18614157 PMCID: PMC2782354 DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.05.044] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/28/2007] [Revised: 05/16/2008] [Accepted: 05/18/2008] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
Abstract
To investigate whether saccade preparation can modulate processing of auditory stimuli in a spatially-specific fashion, ERPs were recorded for a Saccade task, in which the direction of a prepared saccade was cued, prior to an imperative auditory stimulus indicating whether to execute or withhold that saccade. For comparison, we also ran a conventional Covert Attention task, where the same cue now indicated the direction for a covert endogenous attentional shift prior to an auditory target-nontarget discrimination. Lateralised components previously observed during cued shifts of attention (ADAN, LDAP) did not differ significantly across tasks, indicating commonalities between auditory spatial attention and oculomotor control. Moreover, in both tasks, spatially-specific modulation of auditory processing was subsequently found, with enhanced negativity for lateral auditory nontarget stimuli at cued versus uncued locations. This modulation started earlier and was more pronounced for the Covert Attention task, but was also reliably present in the Saccade task, demonstrating that the effects of covert saccade preparation on auditory processing can be similar to effects of endogenous covert attentional orienting, albeit smaller. These findings provide new evidence for similarities but also some differences between oculomotor preparation and shifts of endogenous spatial attention. They also show that saccade preparation can affect not just vision, but also sensory processing of auditory events.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elena Gherri
- School of Psychology, Birkbeck College, London, UK.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
23
|
Abrahamse EL, Van der Lubbe RHJ. Endogenous orienting modulates the Simon effect: critical factors in experimental design. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2008; 72:261-72. [PMID: 17351789 PMCID: PMC2668658 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-007-0110-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/25/2006] [Accepted: 02/06/2007] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
Abstract
Responses are faster when the side of stimulus and response correspond than when they do not correspond, even if stimulus location is irrelevant to the task at hand: the correspondence, spatial compatibility effect, or Simon effect. Generally, it is assumed that an automatically generated spatial code is responsible for this effect, but the precise mechanism underlying the formation of this code is still under dispute. Two major alternatives have been proposed: the referential-coding account, which can be subdivided into a static version and an attention-centered version, and the attention-shift account. These accounts hold clear-cut predictions for attentional cuing experiments. The former would assume a Simon effect irrespective of attentional cuing in its static version, whereas the attention-centered version of the referential-coding account and the attention-shift account would predict a decreased Simon effect on validly as opposed to invalid cued trials. However, results from previous studies are equivocal to the effects of attentional cuing on the Simon effect. We argue here that attentional cueing reliably modulates the Simon effect if some crucial experimental conditions, mostly relevant for optimizing attentional allocation, are met. Furthermore, we propose that the Simon effect may be better understood within the perspective of supra-modal spatial attention, thereby providing an explanation for observed discrepancies in the literature.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elger L. Abrahamse
- Cognitive Psychology and Ergonomics, Faculty of Behavioral Sciences, Universiteit Twente, Postbus 217, 7500 AE Enschede, The Netherlands
| | - Rob H. J. Van der Lubbe
- Cognitive Psychology and Ergonomics, Faculty of Behavioral Sciences, Universiteit Twente, Postbus 217, 7500 AE Enschede, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Jongen EMM, Smulders FTY, Van der Heiden JSH. Lateralized ERP components related to spatial orienting: Discriminating the direction of attention from processing sensory aspects of the cue. Psychophysiology 2007; 44:968-86. [PMID: 17617171 DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00557.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 43] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Abstract
Two spatial cueing experiments were conducted to examine the functional significance of lateralized ERP components after cue-onset and to discriminate components related to sensory cue aspects and components related to the direction of attention. In Experiment 1, a simple detection task was presented. In Experiment 2, attentional selection was augmented. Two unimodal visual cueing tasks were presented using nonlateralized line cues and lateralized arrow cues. Lateralized cue effects and modulation after stimulus onset were stronger in Experiment 2. An early posterior component was related to the physical shape of arrows. A posterior negativity (EDAN) may be related to the encoding of direction from arrow cues. An anterior negativity (ADAN) and a posterior positivity (LDAP) were related to the direction of attention. The ADAN was delayed when it was more difficult to derive cue meaning. Finally, the data suggested an overlap of the LDAP and the EDAN.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ellen M M Jongen
- Department of Neurocognition, Faculty of Psychology, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
25
|
Gherri E, Van Velzen J, Eimer M. Dissociating effector and movement direction selection during the preparation of manual reaching movements: evidence from lateralized ERP components. Clin Neurophysiol 2007; 118:2031-49. [PMID: 17646131 PMCID: PMC2386665 DOI: 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.06.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/29/2007] [Revised: 05/04/2007] [Accepted: 06/10/2007] [Indexed: 12/02/2022]
Abstract
Objective The present study investigated whether lateralized ERP components triggered during covert manual response preparation (ADAN, LDAP) reflect effector selection, the selection of movement direction, or both. Methods Event-related brain potentials were recorded during a response precueing paradigm where visual cues provided either partial (Experiment 1) or full (Experiment 2) information about the response hand and the direction for a subsequent reaching movement. Results ADAN and LDAP components were elicited even when only partial response information was available, demonstrating that they do not require the presence of a fully specified motor program. The ADAN was elicited in a similar fashion regardless of whether effector or movement direction information was provided, suggesting that the underlying mechanisms are equally sensitive to both types of response-related information. In contrast, the LDAP was larger in response to cues providing effector information, but was also reliably present when movement direction was available. Conclusions ADAN and LDAP components reflect preparatory activity within anterior and posterior parts of the parieto-premotor sensorimotor network where different parameters for manual reaching movements are programmed independently. Significance These results support the claim of the premotor theory of attention that shared sensorimotor control mechanisms are involved in attention and motor programming.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elena Gherri
- School of Psychology, Birkbeck, University of London, Malet Street, London WC1E 7HX, UK.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
26
|
Time-varying Brain Potentials and Interhemispheric Coherences of Anterior and Posterior Regions during Repetitive Unimanual Finger Movements. SENSORS 2007. [DOI: 10.3390/s7060960] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
|
27
|
Kennett S, van Velzen J, Eimer M, Driver J. Disentangling gaze shifts from preparatory ERP effects during spatial attention. Psychophysiology 2007; 44:69-78. [PMID: 17241141 PMCID: PMC1805658 DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2006.00470.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
Abstract
After a cue directing attention to one side, anterior event-related potentials (ERPs) show contralateral negativity (anterior directing attention negativity, ADAN). It is unclear whether ADAN effects are contaminated by contralateral negativity arising from residual gaze shifts. Conversely, it is possible that ADAN-related potentials contaminate the horizontal electrooculogram (HEOG), via volume conduction. To evaluate these possibilities, we used high-resolution infrared eye tracking while recording EEG and HEOG in a cued spatial-attention task. We found that, after conventional ERP and HEOG preprocessing exclusions, small but systematic residual gaze shifts in the cued direction can remain, as revealed by the infrared measure. Nevertheless, by using this measure for more stringent exclusion of small gaze shifts, we confirmed that reliable ADAN components remain for preparatory spatial attention in the absence of any systematic gaze shifts toward the cued side.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Steffan Kennett
- UCL Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience & Department of Psychology, University College London, UK
| | - José van Velzen
- Department of Psychology, Goldsmiths College, University of London, London, UK
| | - Martin Eimer
- School of Psychology, Birkbeck College, University of London, UK
| | - Jon Driver
- UCL Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience & Department of Psychology, University College London, UK
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
van Schie HT, Bekkering H. Neural mechanisms underlying immediate and final action goals in object use reflected by slow wave brain potentials. Brain Res 2007; 1148:183-97. [PMID: 17412310 DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.02.085] [Citation(s) in RCA: 31] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/18/2006] [Revised: 02/12/2007] [Accepted: 02/15/2007] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
Abstract
Event-related brain potentials were used to study the neural mechanisms underlying goal-directed object use distinguishing between processes supporting immediate and final action goals during action planning and execution. Subjects performed a grasping and transportation task in which actions were cued either with the immediate action goal (the part of the object to grasp) or with the final action goal of the movement (the end position for transportation). Slow wave potentials dissociated between processes supporting immediate and final goals: reaching for the object was accompanied by the development of a parietal-occipital slow wave that peaked in congruency with the grasping event, whereas transport of the object towards the final goal location was found accompanied by slow wave components developing over left frontal regions with a peak towards the movement end. Source localization of cueing differences indicated activation centered around the parieto-occipital sulcus during reaching of the immediate action goal, followed by enhanced activation in the anterior prefrontal cortex during transport to the final action goal. These results suggest the existence of separate neural controllers for immediate and final action goals during the execution of goal-directed actions with objects.
Collapse
|
29
|
Eimer M, Van Velzen J, Gherri E, Press C. ERP correlates of shared control mechanisms involved in saccade preparation and in covert attention. Brain Res 2007; 1135:154-66. [PMID: 17198687 PMCID: PMC2248222 DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.12.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 41] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/27/2006] [Revised: 11/30/2006] [Accepted: 12/06/2006] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
We investigated whether attention shifts and eye movement preparation are mediated by shared control mechanisms, as claimed by the premotor theory of attention. ERPs were recorded in three tasks where directional cues presented at the beginning of each trial instructed participants to direct their attention to the cued side without eye movements (Covert task), to prepare an eye movement in the cued direction without attention shifts (Saccade task) or both (Combined task). A peripheral visual Go/Nogo stimulus that was presented 800 ms after cue onset signalled whether responses had to be executed or withheld. Lateralised ERP components triggered during the cue-target interval, which are assumed to reflect preparatory control mechanisms that mediate attentional orienting, were very similar across tasks. They were also present in the Saccade task, which was designed to discourage any concomitant covert attention shifts. These results support the hypothesis that saccade preparation and attentional orienting are implemented by common control structures. There were however systematic differences in the impact of eye movement programming and covert attention on ERPs triggered in response to visual stimuli at cued versus uncued locations. It is concluded that, although the preparatory processes underlying saccade programming and covert attentional orienting may be based on common mechanisms, they nevertheless differ in their spatially specific effects on visual information processing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Martin Eimer
- Department of Psychology, Birkbeck College, University of London, Malet Street, London WC1E 7HX, England, UK.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
30
|
van der Lubbe RHJ, Havik MM, Bekker EM, Postma A. Task-dependent exogenous cuing effects depend on cue modality. Psychophysiology 2006; 43:145-60. [PMID: 16712585 DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2006.00393.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
Task-dependent exogenous cuing effects on reaction time in detection and discrimination tasks have been ascribed to delayed withdrawal of attention in discrimination tasks. Alternatively, these differences may be due to cue-induced response inhibition in detection tasks. Unimodal and crossmodal versions of the Posner paradigm were examined with short cue-target intervals. Targets above or below fixation required either detection or discrimination responses. Cuing effects were determined for the target-elicited P1 component and for the lateralized readiness potential (LRP). Task-dependent cuing effects on reaction time were found in the unimodal but not in the crossmodal version, but not for the P1 component. The LRP data indicated that inhibition of return in the unimodal detection task had a premotoric locus. These findings suggest that inhibition in the unimodal detection task resulted from speeded motor inhibition triggered by the visual cue.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rob H J van der Lubbe
- Cognitive Psychology and Ergonomics, Faculty of Behavioral Sciences, Universiteit Twente, Enschede, The Netherlands.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
31
|
van der Lubbe RHJ, Neggers SFW, Verleger R, Kenemans JL. Spatiotemporal overlap between brain activation related to saccade preparation and attentional orienting. Brain Res 2006; 1072:133-52. [PMID: 16427618 DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.11.087] [Citation(s) in RCA: 72] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/07/2004] [Revised: 11/29/2005] [Accepted: 11/30/2005] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
Abstract
Recent brain imaging studies provided evidence that the brain areas involved with attentional orienting and the preparation of saccades largely overlap, which may indicate that focusing attention at a specific location can be considered as an unexecuted saccade towards that location (i.e. the premotor theory of attention). Alternatively, it may be proposed that attentional orienting is simply relevant for preparing saccades, but the two processes may also be completely unrelated. In two experiments, we examined temporal activation of brain areas by measuring the electroencephalogram. Central cues indicated the likely side (left or right) at which a to-be-attended target would occur, or to which a saccade had to be prepared. Cue direction-related activity was determined, time-locked to cue onset. In addition, in our second experiment, delayed saccades had to be carried out, which allows to focus on processes strongly related to saccade execution. In nearly all tasks, an early directing attention negativity (EDAN), an anterior directing attention negativity (ADAN), and a late directing attention positivity (LDAP) were observed, time-locked to cue onset. Source analyses supported the view that this activity probably originates from areas within the ventral intraparietal sulcus (vIPS) and the frontal eye fields (FEF). The saccade-locked analysis also indicated that the FEF plays an important role in triggering saccades, but the role of vIPS appears to be minimal. The latter finding disfavors the premotor theory of attention, as it suggests that the relation between attention and action is less direct.
Collapse
|
32
|
Manual response preparation and saccade programming are linked to attention shifts: ERP evidence for covert attentional orienting and spatially specific modulations of visual processing. Brain Res 2006; 1105:7-19. [PMID: 16448629 DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.10.060] [Citation(s) in RCA: 47] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/17/2005] [Revised: 10/17/2005] [Accepted: 10/20/2005] [Indexed: 11/21/2022]
Abstract
The premotor theory of attention claims that attentional shifts are triggered during response programming, regardless of which response modality is involved. To investigate this claim, event-related brain potentials (ERPs) were recorded while participants covertly prepared a left or right response, as indicated by a precue presented at the beginning of each trial. Cues signalled a left or right eye movement in the saccade task, and a left or right manual response in the manual task. The cued response had to be executed or withheld following the presentation of a Go/Nogo stimulus. Although there were systematic differences between ERPs triggered during covert manual and saccade preparation, lateralised ERP components sensitive to the direction of a cued response were very similar for both tasks, and also similar to the components previously found during cued shifts of endogenous spatial attention. This is consistent with the claim that the control of attention and of covert response preparation are closely linked. N1 components triggered by task-irrelevant visual probes presented during the covert response preparation interval were enhanced when these probes were presented close to cued response hand in the manual task, and at the saccade target location in the saccade task. This demonstrates that both manual and saccade preparation result in spatially specific modulations of visual processing, in line with the predictions of the premotor theory.
Collapse
|
33
|
Hanisch C, Radach R, Holtkamp K, Herpertz-Dahlmann B, Konrad K. Oculomotor inhibition in children with and without attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). J Neural Transm (Vienna) 2005; 113:671-84. [PMID: 16082513 DOI: 10.1007/s00702-005-0344-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 49] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/21/2004] [Accepted: 05/21/2005] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
The aim of the present study was to distinguish between a general deficit in oculomotor control and a deficit restricted to inhibitory functions in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). In addition, we were interested in differentiating between a general inhibition deficit and deficient subfunctions of inhibition. We used a prosaccade task to measure general oculomotor abilities in 22 children with ADHD and in age- and gender-matched healthy controls. A fixation, an antisaccade and a countermanding saccade task were used to measure specific aspects of oculomotor inhibition. Two major results were obtained: First, our prosaccade task suggests similar saccadic response preparation and saccadic accuracy in the ADHD compared to the control children. Secondly, the fixation and the countermanding saccade task indicate deficits on measures of oculomotor inhibition in the ADHD group. While patients were specifically impaired in stopping an already initiated response or in suppressing exploratory saccades in a novel situation, inhibition of a prepotent response was not deficient. Our data thus indicate an underlying impairment in cognitive inhibition in ADHD that has been associated with prefrontal lobe functions. More specifically, as the anterior cingulate gyrus has been associated with the countermanding saccade task and group differences were most pronounced in this paradigm our data are in line with imaging data stressing the importance of this cortical structure in the pathophysiology of ADHD.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- C Hanisch
- Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, University Hospital Aachen, Germany
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
34
|
Eimer M, Forster B, Van Velzen J, Prabhu G. Covert manual response preparation triggers attentional shifts: ERP evidence for the premotor theory of attention. Neuropsychologia 2005; 43:957-66. [PMID: 15716166 PMCID: PMC2254498 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.08.011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 83] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/27/2004] [Accepted: 08/27/2004] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
The premotor theory of attention claims that the preparation of goal-directed action and shifts of attention are closely linked, because they are controlled by shared sensorymotor mechanisms. Until now, support for this theory has come primarily from studies demonstrating links between saccade programming and attention shifts. The present event-related brain potential (ERP) study demonstrated that attentional orienting processes are also elicited during the covert preparation of unimanual responses. ERPs were recorded in the interval between a visual response-hand selection cue and a subsequent visual Go/Nogo signal when participants prepared to lift their left or right index finger. Lateralised ERP components elicited during response preparation were very similar to components previously observed during instructed endogenous attention shifts, indicating that analogous attentional orienting processes are activated in both cases. Somatosensory ERP components (P90, N140) were enhanced when task-irrelevant tactile probes were delivered during response preparation to the hand involved in an anticipated response, even when probes were presented well in advance of response execution. These results suggest that attentional shifts are triggered during unimanual response preparation, as predicted by the premotor theory. This link between manual response programming and attention is consistent with the hypothesis that common mechanisms are involved in the control of attention and action.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Martin Eimer
- School of Psychology, Birkbeck College, University of London, London WC1E 7HX, UK.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
35
|
Van der Lubbe RHJ, Jaśkowski P, Verleger R. Mechanisms underlying spatial coding in a multiple-item Simon task. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2004; 69:179-90. [PMID: 15597183 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-004-0176-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/07/2003] [Accepted: 02/21/2004] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
Abstract
Choice responses are faster when target position and response side correspond than when they do not, even if target position is response-irrelevant. This "Simon effect" has also been observed in case of multi-item arrays. Generally, it is assumed that an automatically generated spatial response code is responsible for the effect. The referential-coding account assumes that this code is directly related to the target, although the moment of production of the code is not fully clear. The attention-shift account assumes that the code is directly related to the direction of the most recent attentional shift. An experiment was performed in which left or right target locations were indicated by arrows occurring before (precue), simultaneous with (simcue), or after (postcue) six-element arrays. Overt responses and EEG potentials were recorded. The Simon effect was present in all conditions, and decreased when responses were slower. No relation was found between amplitude of posterior lateralized components and the magnitude of the Simon effect. A posterior contralateral negativity was also found after presenting the arrays in the precue condition, which might reflect the reorienting of attention toward the target position. The results are more favorable to the referential-coding account although this account becomes very similar to the attention-shift hypothesis as the moment of formation of the spatial response code is related to effective target onset rather than to stimulus onset.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rob H J Van der Lubbe
- Psychological Laboratory, Helmholtz Instituut, Utrecht University, Heidelberglaan 2, 3584 CS Utrecht, The Netherlands.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
36
|
Kaiser J, Ulrich R, Lutzenberger W. Dynamics of sensorimotor cortex activation to spatial sounds precueing ipsi- versus contralateral manual responses. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2003; 17:573-83. [PMID: 14561446 DOI: 10.1016/s0926-6410(03)00171-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
Abstract
Spatially informative visual precues give rise to event-related potential asymmetries with higher negativities over the contralateral hemisphere. However the attribution of these potentials to sensorimotor areas is still unclear. The present magnetoencephalography study assessed movement preparation processes to auditory spatial precues. Event-related desynchronization (ERD) was measured to test the hypothesis that lateralized sounds would give rise to a fast, stimulus-driven activation of motor networks independent of the precued response side. The lateralized vowels /a/ and /e/ served as precues for either ipsi- or contralateral responses, respectively, which had to be executed when an imperative stimulus was presented 1 s after precue onset. Two separate experiments were conducted with either blocked or mixed presentation of ipsi- and contralateral precues. Beta ERD over sensorimotor regions representing the stimulus side was elicited by both types of precues approximately 200 ms after their onset. For contralateral precues, a switch of beta ERD to the response hemisphere took place approximately 400 ms after trial-onset, peaking prior to the imperative stimulus (approximately 800 ms post trial-onset). Signal subspace projection demonstrated a high topographical correspondence between the early precue-related ERD and the pattern immediately preceding the response, suggesting that both were generated in similar motor networks. Apparently lateralized sounds give rise to an early activation of contralateral motor networks independent of the precued response. This suggests strong associations between space processing and action preparation networks, with fast activations preceding a detailed cortical analysis of stimulus meaning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jochen Kaiser
- MEG-Center, Institute of Medical Psychology and Behavioral Neurobiology, Eberhard-Karls-University of Tübingen, Gartenstrasse 29, 72074 Tübingen, Germany.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
37
|
Jentzsch I, Leuthold H. Advance movement preparation of eye, foot, and hand: a comparative study using movement-related brain potentials. BRAIN RESEARCH. COGNITIVE BRAIN RESEARCH 2002; 14:201-17. [PMID: 12067693 DOI: 10.1016/s0926-6410(02)00107-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 32] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
The present study was designed to test the inter-relationship between generalized motor programs (GMPs) and movement preparation by asking participants to perform movements with eye, foot, or hand. In two independent experiments a response precuing task was employed that combined the recording of movement-related brain potentials (MRPs) with dipole source analysis. Behavioral results indicated the utilization of advance information about movement direction and effector. When eye and hand movements were involved (experiment1) partial advance information about response side but not effector induced parallel motor programming of eye and hand at an abstract but not effector-specific level. In contrast, when partial precues specified side of a forthcoming hand or foot movement (experiment 2) foot and hand were prepared in parallel both at abstract and at effector-specific levels of motor programming. Consistent with the GMP view, these results indicate that effector-specific preparation is possible even when the effector is not yet known as long as a common motor program controls the demanded movements. However, because parallel specification of divergent movement pattern (eye, hand) at an abstract level was not predicted by the GMP, we propose a model of advance movement preparation that takes into account neurofunctional considerations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ines Jentzsch
- Department of Psychology, University of Glasgow, 58 Hillhead Street, G12 8QB, Glasgow, UK.
| | | |
Collapse
|
38
|
van der Lubbe RH, Jaśkowski P, Wauschkuhn B, Verleger R. Influence of Time Pressure in a Simple Response Task, a Choice-by-Location Task, and the Simon Task. J PSYCHOPHYSIOL 2001. [DOI: 10.1027//0269-8803.15.4.241] [Citation(s) in RCA: 59] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
Abstract
Abstract The influence of strategy was examined for a simple response task, a choice-by-location task, and the Simon task by varying time pressure. Besides reaction time (RT) and accuracy, we measured response force and derived two measures from the event-related EEG potential to form an index for attentional orienting (posterior contralateral negativity: PCN) and the start of motor activation (the lateralized readiness potential: LRP). For the choice-by-location task and the Simon task, effects of time pressure were found on the response-locked LRP, but not on the onset of the PCN and the stimulus-locked LRP. Thus, strategy influences processing after the start of motor activation in choice tasks. A small effect of time pressure was found on the peak latency of the PCN in the Simon task, which suggests that time pressure may affect attentional orienting. In the simple response task, time pressure reduced the amplitude of the PCN. This finding suggests that strategy affects attentional orienting to stimuli when these stimuli are not highly relevant. Finally, the effect of time pressure on RT was much larger in the simple response task than in the other tasks, which may be ascribed to the possibility of preparing the required response in the simple response task.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rob H.J. van der Lubbe
- Psychological Laboratory, Utrecht University, The Netherlands
- Department of Neurology, Medical University of Lübeck, Germany
| | - Piotr Jaśkowski
- Department of Neurology, Medical University of Lübeck, Germany
- Department of Psychophysiology, Casimirus The Great University of Bydgoszcz, Poland
| | | | - Rolf Verleger
- Department of Neurology, Medical University of Lübeck, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
39
|
Verleger R, Vollmer C, Wauschkuhn B, van der Lubbe RH, Wascher E. Dimensional overlap between arrows as cueing stimuli and responses? Evidence from contra-ipsilateral differences in EEG potentials. BRAIN RESEARCH. COGNITIVE BRAIN RESEARCH 2000; 10:99-109. [PMID: 10978697 DOI: 10.1016/s0926-6410(00)00032-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 42] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
Abstract
In the S1-S2 interval, 400 ms after an arrow as S1, an EEG-potential difference occurs between scalp sites contralateral and ipsilateral to arrow direction. Eimer [J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 21 (1995) 837-854] interpreted this difference as a sign of automatic activation of the manual response, due to dimensional overlap of arrows and responses. However, according to Kornblum et al.'s [Psychol. Rev. 97 (1990) 253-270] notion of dimensional overlap, responses can only be automatically primed if they are included in the response set. Therefore, participants of the present study had to respond to S2 in separate blocks either by key-press, as in Eimer [J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 21 (1995) 837-854], or by making saccades. In addition, contra-ipsilateral differences were recorded not only from central positions, overlying the hand-motor area, but across the whole scalp. Contralateral negativity at 400 ms after S1 was indeed found over the hand-motor area in the 'hand blocks'. However, this 'L-400' (=lateralization at 400 ms) was generally as large in the 'eye' blocks as in the 'hand' blocks. Therefore, L-400 does not reflect automatic activation of manual responses in the sense of Kornblum et al. [Psychol. Rev. 97 (1990) 253-270]. Further, its topographical maximum was more anterior than the hand-motor-related negativity that preceded the manual response ('LRP') with its maximum at central sites. Therefore, L-400 probably does not originate in the hand-motor cortex. Rather, it may be related to activity of the lateral premotor cortex found in fMRI studies of spatial orienting. The present EEG study extends these studies by delimiting the time period of this activity, suggesting that it reflects encoding of the spatial properties of the arrow for action.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- R Verleger
- Department of Neurology, Medical University of Lübeck, D 23538, Lübeck, Germany.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|