1
|
Fugazza C, Jacques E, Nostri S, Kranzelic U, Sommese A, Miklósi Á. Shape and texture biases in dogs' generalization of trained objects. Sci Rep 2024; 14:28077. [PMID: 39543129 PMCID: PMC11564508 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-024-72244-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/06/2024] [Accepted: 09/05/2024] [Indexed: 11/17/2024] Open
Abstract
Shape bias, the tendency to link the meaning of words to the shape of objects, is a widely investigated phenomenon, but the extent to which it is linked to vocabulary acquisition and/or to tool using is controversial. Understanding how non-human animals generalize the properties of objects can provide insights into the evolutionary processes and cognitive mechanisms that influence this bias. We investigated object generalization in dogs, focusing on their tendency to attend to shape or texture in a two-way choice task. We analyzed data of thirty-five dogs that were successfully trained to discriminate a target object among distractors and retrieve it to their owner. In subsequent testing, dogs chose between objects similar to the target in either shape or texture. Results showed that dogs first approached objects of similar shape, but then predominantly chose objects of similar texture. This suggests a reliance on visual cues for initial assessment and tactile cues for final discrimination. Our findings highlight the influence of perceptual modalities on object generalization in dogs, showing that, before object manipulation, they seem to show a shape bias but, once they make physical contact with the objects, they rather rely on texture to generalize. This study contributes to our understanding of generalization in a non-verbal and non-tool using species and opens avenues for comparative investigations into the relationship between vocabulary acquisition, tool using and biases in object generalization.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Claudia Fugazza
- Department of Ethology, Eötvös Loránd University, Pázmány P. s 1c, 1117, Budapest, Hungary.
- ELTE NAP Comparative Ethology Research Group, Budapest, Hungary.
| | - Elodie Jacques
- Department of Ethology, Eötvös Loránd University, Pázmány P. s 1c, 1117, Budapest, Hungary
| | - Silvia Nostri
- Department of Ethology, Eötvös Loránd University, Pázmány P. s 1c, 1117, Budapest, Hungary
| | - Una Kranzelic
- Department of Ethology, Eötvös Loránd University, Pázmány P. s 1c, 1117, Budapest, Hungary
| | - Andrea Sommese
- Department of Ethology, Eötvös Loránd University, Pázmány P. s 1c, 1117, Budapest, Hungary
- ELTE NAP Comparative Ethology Research Group, Budapest, Hungary
| | - Ádám Miklósi
- Department of Ethology, Eötvös Loránd University, Pázmány P. s 1c, 1117, Budapest, Hungary
- ELTE NAP Comparative Ethology Research Group, Budapest, Hungary
- MTA-ELTE Comparative Ethology Research Group, Budapest, Hungary
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Krahn J, Azadian A, Cavalli C, Miller J, Protopopova A. Effect of pre-session discrimination training on performance in a judgement bias test in dogs. Anim Cogn 2024; 27:66. [PMID: 39395092 PMCID: PMC11470868 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01905-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/25/2024] [Revised: 09/16/2024] [Accepted: 09/17/2024] [Indexed: 10/14/2024]
Abstract
Spatial judgement bias tests (JBTs) can involve teaching animals that a bowl provides a reward in one location but does not in another. The animal is then presented with the bowl placed between the rewarded and the unrewarded locations (i.e., ambiguous locations) and their latency to approach reflects expectation of reward or 'optimism'. Some suggest that greater 'optimism' indicates better welfare. Performance in JBTs, however, may also indicate a learning history independently from welfare determinants. We hypothesized that dogs' 'optimism' in a follow-up JBT may be impacted by a learning treatment involving additional trials of a different discrimination task. Once enrolled, companion dogs (n = 16) were required to complete three study phases: (1) a pre-treatment JBT, (2) a learning treatment, and (3) a post-treatment JBT. During the JBTs, dogs were presented with five locations: one rewarded, one unrewarded, and three ambiguous (all unrewarded). Dogs were randomly assigned to a trial-based learning task-a nose-touch to the palm of the hand. In the Experimental discrimination treatment phase (n = 8), dogs were presented with two hands in each trial and only rewarded for touching one specific hand. In the Control treatment phase (n = 8), dogs were presented with one hand per trial in alternating sequence and were yoked to dogs in the Experimental group to receive the same number of rewarded and unrewarded trials (to control for possible frustration). Using a repeated measures mixed model with JBT repeated within dog, we found no difference in the change in approach latency to the ambiguous locations between the dogs across treatments. 'Optimism' as measured in this JBT was not altered by the additional discrimination trials used in our study.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Joseph Krahn
- Animal Welfare Program, Faculty of Land and Food Systems, The University of British Columbia, Vancouver, Canada
| | - Amin Azadian
- Animal Welfare Program, Faculty of Land and Food Systems, The University of British Columbia, Vancouver, Canada
| | - Camila Cavalli
- Animal Welfare Program, Faculty of Land and Food Systems, The University of British Columbia, Vancouver, Canada
| | - Julia Miller
- Animal Welfare Program, Faculty of Land and Food Systems, The University of British Columbia, Vancouver, Canada
- Department of Immunology, Pathophysiology and Veterinary Preventive Medicine, Wroclaw University of Environmental and Life Sciences, Wroclaw, Poland
| | - Alexandra Protopopova
- Animal Welfare Program, Faculty of Land and Food Systems, The University of British Columbia, Vancouver, Canada.
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Bastos APM, Evenson A, Wood PM, Houghton ZN, Naranjo L, Smith GE, Cairo-Evans A, Korpos L, Terwilliger J, Raghunath S, Paul C, Hou H, Rossano F. How do soundboard-trained dogs respond to human button presses? An investigation into word comprehension. PLoS One 2024; 19:e0307189. [PMID: 39196871 PMCID: PMC11355551 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0307189] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/18/2024] [Accepted: 07/01/2024] [Indexed: 08/30/2024] Open
Abstract
Past research on interspecies communication has shown that animals can be trained to use Augmentative Interspecies Communication (AIC) devices, such as soundboards, to make simple requests of their caretakers. The recent uptake in AIC devices by hundreds of pet owners around the world offers a novel opportunity to investigate whether AIC is possible with owner-trained family dogs. To answer this question, we carried out two studies to test pet dogs' ability to recognise and respond appropriately to food-related, play-related, and outside-related words on their soundboards. One study was conducted by researchers, and the other by citizen scientists who followed the same procedure. Further, we investigated whether these behaviours depended on the identity of the person presenting the word (unfamiliar person or dog's owner) and the mode of its presentation (spoken or produced by a pressed button). We find that dogs produced contextually appropriate behaviours for both play-related and outside-related words regardless of the identity of the person producing them and the mode in which they were produced. Therefore, pet dogs can be successfully taught by their owners to associate words recorded onto soundboard buttons to their outcomes in the real world, and they respond appropriately to these words even when they are presented in the absence of any other cues, such as the owner's body language.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Amalia P. M. Bastos
- Department of Cognitive Science, University of California, San Diego, San Diego, California, United States of America
- Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, United States of America
| | - Ashley Evenson
- College of Arts and Sciences, Canisius College, Buffalo, New York, United States of America
- FluentPet, Inc, San Diego, California, United States of America
| | - Patrick M. Wood
- Department of Cognitive Science, University of California, San Diego, San Diego, California, United States of America
- Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, United States of America
- FluentPet, Inc, San Diego, California, United States of America
| | - Zachary N. Houghton
- Department of Linguistics, University of California, Davis, Davis, California, United States of America
| | - Lucas Naranjo
- FluentPet, Inc, San Diego, California, United States of America
- Statistics and Operational Research Department, Universitat de València, Valencia, Spain
| | - Gabriella E. Smith
- Comparative Cognition, Messerli Research Institute, University of Veterinary Medicine Vienna, Medical University of Vienna, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
- School of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of St. Andrews, St. Andrews, United Kingdom
| | - Alexandria Cairo-Evans
- Department of Cognitive Science, University of California, San Diego, San Diego, California, United States of America
| | - Lisa Korpos
- Department of Cognitive Science, University of California, San Diego, San Diego, California, United States of America
- FluentPet, Inc, San Diego, California, United States of America
| | - Jack Terwilliger
- Department of Cognitive Science, University of California, San Diego, San Diego, California, United States of America
| | - Sarita Raghunath
- Department of Cognitive Science, University of California, San Diego, San Diego, California, United States of America
| | - Cassandra Paul
- Department of Cognitive Science, University of California, San Diego, San Diego, California, United States of America
| | - Hairou Hou
- Department of Cognitive Science, University of California, San Diego, San Diego, California, United States of America
| | - Federico Rossano
- Department of Cognitive Science, University of California, San Diego, San Diego, California, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Dror S, Miklósi Á, Sommese A, Fugazza C. A citizen science model turns anecdotes into evidence by revealing similar characteristics among Gifted Word Learner dogs. Sci Rep 2023; 13:21747. [PMID: 38097634 PMCID: PMC10721640 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-023-47864-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/20/2023] [Accepted: 11/19/2023] [Indexed: 12/17/2023] Open
Abstract
Dogs that have a vocabulary of object labels (Gifted Word Learner dogs-GWL dogs) have great potential as a comparative model for studying a variety of cognitive mechanisms. However, only a handful of studies, with a small sample size of 1 or 2 dogs, have examined this phenomenon. GWL dogs appear to share many of the same distinctive characteristics, but due to their rarity, it is not clear if these similarities are only anecdotal or indeed reflect characteristics that are similar in these rare individuals. Here we present the first study conducted on a relatively large sample of 41 GWL dogs that were recruited and tested using a citizen science model. After testing the dogs' receptive vocabulary of toy names, we asked the owners to complete a questionnaire about their and their dog's life experiences. Our findings highlight several characteristics that are shared among most GWL dogs, such as their learning speed, their large vocabulary, and that they learned the names of the toys spontaneously, without the explicit intent of their owners. Our findings validate previous anecdotal evidence on common characteristics of GWL dogs and supply additional support to the hypothesis that these dogs represent a unique group of dogs.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Shany Dror
- Department of Ethology, Eötvös Loránd University, Pázmány P. s 1c, 6th Floor, Budapest, 1117, Hungary.
- Doctoral School of Biology, Institute of Biology, ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Hungary.
| | - Ádám Miklósi
- Department of Ethology, Eötvös Loránd University, Pázmány P. s 1c, 6th Floor, Budapest, 1117, Hungary
- MTA-ELTE Comparative Ethology Research Group, Budapest, Hungary
- ELTE-ELKH NAP Comparative Ethology Research, Budapest, Hungary
| | - Andrea Sommese
- Department of Ethology, Eötvös Loránd University, Pázmány P. s 1c, 6th Floor, Budapest, 1117, Hungary
| | - Claudia Fugazza
- Department of Ethology, Eötvös Loránd University, Pázmány P. s 1c, 6th Floor, Budapest, 1117, Hungary
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Rogers CM, Salzman MM, Li Z, Merten N, Russell LJ, Lillesand HK, Mowat FM. Subjective vision assessment in companion dogs using dogVLQ demonstrates age-associated visual dysfunction. Front Vet Sci 2023; 10:1244518. [PMID: 37662991 PMCID: PMC10469761 DOI: 10.3389/fvets.2023.1244518] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/22/2023] [Accepted: 08/07/2023] [Indexed: 09/05/2023] Open
Abstract
Introduction Dim light vision as assessed by proxy and clinical tools is commonly impaired in older humans and impacts quality of life. Although proxy visual assessment tools have been developed for dogs, it is unclear if they are sensitive enough to detect subtle visual dysfunction in older dogs. We sought to determine if a newly designed proxy visual function questionnaire could detect age-associated differences in visual behaviors in varying lighting conditions in dogs. Methods A 27-item questionnaire (the dog variable lighting questionnaire, dogVLQ) was designed to assess visual behavior in dogs in different lighting settings. We conducted the dogVLQ, a previously validated visual function questionnaire the dog vision impairment score and performed light- and dark-adapted electroretinography (ERG) on a subset of dogs. Questionnaire scores were analyzed for dog age associations using correlation analysis. Results Questionnaire responses from 235 dog owners were obtained (122 female, 112 male dogs), 79 of which underwent ERG (43 female, 36 male dogs). Bright light visual behavior was significantly associated with light-adapted bright flash ERG amplitudes, visual behavior in near darkness was associated with dark-adapted ERG amplitudes. The dogVLQ identified worse vision in older dogs in bright light, dim light, and darkness; predicted onset was younger for vision in near darkness. Older dogs had more difficulty navigating transitions between lighting conditions. Discussion Subjective dog owner assessment of visual function associates with objective measurement of retinal function in dogs and supports reduced vision-mediated behaviors in older dogs.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Callie M. Rogers
- Department of Surgical Sciences, School of Veterinary Medicine, University of Wisconsin–Madison, Madison, WI, United States
| | - Michele M. Salzman
- Department of Surgical Sciences, School of Veterinary Medicine, University of Wisconsin–Madison, Madison, WI, United States
| | - Zhanhai Li
- Department of Biostatistics and Medical Informatics, University of Wisconsin–Madison, Madison, WI, United States
| | - Natascha Merten
- Department of Population Health Sciences, School of Medicine and Public Health, University of Wisconsin–Madison, Madison, WI, United States
- Department of Medicine (Geriatrics and Gerontology), School of Medicine and Public Health, University of Wisconsin–Madison, Madison, WI, United States
| | - Leah J. Russell
- Department of Surgical Sciences, School of Veterinary Medicine, University of Wisconsin–Madison, Madison, WI, United States
| | - Hannah K. Lillesand
- Department of Surgical Sciences, School of Veterinary Medicine, University of Wisconsin–Madison, Madison, WI, United States
| | - Freya M. Mowat
- Department of Surgical Sciences, School of Veterinary Medicine, University of Wisconsin–Madison, Madison, WI, United States
- Department of Ophthalmology and Visual Sciences, School of Medicine and Public Health, University of Wisconsin–Madison, Madison, WI, United States
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Favela LH, Machery E. Investigating the concept of representation in the neural and psychological sciences. Front Psychol 2023; 14:1165622. [PMID: 37359883 PMCID: PMC10284684 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1165622] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/23/2023] [Accepted: 04/19/2023] [Indexed: 06/28/2023] Open
Abstract
The concept of representation is commonly treated as indispensable to research on brains, behavior, and cognition. Nevertheless, systematic evidence about the ways the concept is applied remains scarce. We present the results of an experiment aimed at elucidating what researchers mean by "representation." Participants were an international group of psychologists, neuroscientists, and philosophers (N = 736). Applying elicitation methodology, participants responded to a survey with experimental scenarios aimed at invoking applications of "representation" and five other ways of describing how the brain responds to stimuli. While we find little disciplinary variation in the application of "representation" and other expressions (e.g., "about" and "carry information"), the results suggest that researchers exhibit uncertainty about what sorts of brain activity involve representations or not; they also prefer non-representational, causal characterizations of the brain's response to stimuli. Potential consequences of these findings are explored, such as reforming or eliminating the concept of representation from use.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Luis H. Favela
- Department of Philosophy, University of Central Florida, Orlando, FL, United States
- Cognitive Sciences Program, University of Central Florida, Orlando, FL, United States
| | - Edouard Machery
- Department of History and Philosophy of Science, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
- Center for Philosophy of Science, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
- African Centre for Epistemology and Philosophy of Science, University of Johannesburg, Johannesburg, South Africa
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Souza P, Guo K, Mills DS, Resende B, Albuquerque N. How Do Dogs Behave When Presented with Situations of Different Emotional Valences? Animals (Basel) 2023; 13:1027. [PMID: 36978568 PMCID: PMC10044040 DOI: 10.3390/ani13061027] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/01/2023] [Revised: 02/28/2023] [Accepted: 03/09/2023] [Indexed: 03/18/2023] Open
Abstract
Dogs are good models for studying behaviour and cognition as they have complex social capabilities. In the current study, we observed how human emotional valences (positive, neutral and negative) affected aspects of dogs' behaviour. We expected that dogs would exhibit more approaching behaviours in the positive condition and more signs of avoidance in the negative one. We analysed videos of 70 adult pet dogs of various breeds taken from an experiment in which one of two actors expressed an emotion and dogs could freely explore the environment for 30 s. Our results show that dogs exhibit differential behaviour when presented with different emotional valences. Two behaviours arose that might be linked to a reciprocal positive emotional state in dogs: tail raised between 90° and 180° and physical contact during sniffing. These behaviours are associated with an active search for information. In the positive conditions, dogs were more willing to explore the social environment and gather information from the actors.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Paulo Souza
- Institute of Biosciences, University of São Paulo, São Paulo 05508-090, Brazil
| | - Kun Guo
- School of Psychology, University of Lincoln, Lincoln LN6 7TS, UK;
| | - Daniel S. Mills
- Department of Life Sciences, University of Lincoln, Lincoln LN6 7TS, UK;
| | - Briseida Resende
- Institute of Psychology, University of São Paulo, São Paulo 05508-030, Brazil;
| | - Natalia Albuquerque
- Institute of Psychology, University of São Paulo, São Paulo 05508-030, Brazil;
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
A novel task of canine olfaction for use in adult and senior pet dogs. Sci Rep 2023; 13:2224. [PMID: 36754988 PMCID: PMC9908929 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-023-29361-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/20/2022] [Accepted: 02/02/2023] [Indexed: 02/10/2023] Open
Abstract
While much work has been done in the field of canine olfaction, there has been little exploration of hyposmia or anosmia. This is partly due to difficulties in reducing confounds like training history and environmental distraction. The current study describes a novel olfaction test using spontaneous search behavior in dogs to find a hidden food treat in a three-choice task with both light-phase and dark-phase conditions. The study was performed in 18 adult control dogs, 18 senior/geriatric dogs enrolled in a longitudinal aging study, and a single dog with severe nasal pathology. In the senior/geriatric and control groups, dogs performed with higher accuracy (p < 0.0001) and were less likely to show biased selection strategy (p < 0.01) in the dark-phase than light-phase. While senior/geriatric dogs performed above chance, they had lower accuracy in the dark-phase compared to controls (p = 0.036). Dogs who scored higher on an owner questionnaire of cognitive decline showed a positive correlation with performance in the dark-phase; performance on additional cognitive tests did not correlate with performance in the dark-phase. This task can be used to quantify canine olfaction using clearly defined endpoints and spontaneous behaviors thus making it feasible to compare between and within groups of pet dogs.
Collapse
|