1
|
Chandler JA. Inferring Mental States from Brain Data: Ethico-legal Questions about Social Uses of Brain Data. Hastings Cent Rep 2025; 55:22-32. [PMID: 40043184 PMCID: PMC11882133 DOI: 10.1002/hast.4958] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 03/09/2025]
Abstract
Neurotechnologies that collect and interpret data about brain activity are already in use for medical and nonmedical applications. Refinements of existing noninvasive techniques and the discovery of new ones will likely encourage broader uptake. The increased collection and use of brain data and, in particular, their use to infer the existence of mental states have led to questions about whether mental privacy may be threatened. It may be threatened if the brain data actually support inferences about the mind or if decisions are made about a person in the belief that the inferences are justified. This article considers the chain of inferences lying between data about neural activity and a particular mental state as well as the ethico-legal issues raised by making these inferences, focusing here on what the threshold of reliability should be for using brain data to infer mental states.
Collapse
|
2
|
Iacono WG. Psychology and the lie detector industry: A fifty-year perspective. Biol Psychol 2024; 190:108808. [PMID: 38718884 DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2024.108808] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/21/2024] [Revised: 04/27/2024] [Accepted: 05/01/2024] [Indexed: 06/02/2024]
Abstract
Fifty years ago, in a trenchant analysis that challenged applied lie detection theory and science, David Lykken (1974) brought polygraphic interrogation methods to the attention of academia with the hope that these techniques would come under the purview of psychology and psychophysiology. In this perspective, I examine how this application of psychophysiology has evolved over the last half century and how its status has changed for 1) the comparison (control) question test (CQT), used in forensic applications; 2) polygraph screening tests, used to evaluate examinee integrity; and 3) the concealed information technique (CIT), used to assess recognition memory of crime details. The criticisms of the CQT and screening tests advanced by Lykken have been amplified and focused by the academic community over the last half century. However, this has had little effect on how these methods are practiced and has not curtailed their use. Although most private sector employee screening tests are now prohibited, personnel screening of government employees has increased, and screening tests of sex offenders are now commonplace. Even though the CIT has captured the interest of psychophysiologists as a scientifically defensible technique, its field use is negligible. A primary purpose of polygraphic interrogations continues to be the extraction of admissions and confessions. The lack of change in the polygraph testing status quo stems in large part from unwavering government support for the use of these methods. As a result, polygraph theory and research support continues to rest on shaky ground while practice continues unfettered by valid criticism.
Collapse
|
3
|
Lin Y, Huang S, Mao J, Li M, Haihambo N, Wang F, Liang Y, Chen W, Han C. The neural oscillatory mechanism underlying human brain fingerprint recognition using a portable EEG acquisition device. Neuroimage 2024; 294:120637. [PMID: 38714216 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2024.120637] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/29/2024] [Revised: 03/31/2024] [Accepted: 05/04/2024] [Indexed: 05/09/2024] Open
Abstract
In recent years, brainprint recognition has emerged as a novel method of personal identity verification. Although studies have demonstrated the feasibility of this technology, some limitations hinder its further development into the society, such as insufficient efficiency (extended wear time for multi-channel EEG cap), complex experimental paradigms (more time in learning and completing experiments), and unclear neurobiological characteristics (lack of intuitive biomarkers and an inability to eliminate the impact of noise on individual differences). Overall, these limitations are due to the incomplete understanding of the underlying neural mechanisms. Therefore, this study aims to investigate the neural mechanisms behind brainwave recognition and simplify the operation process. We recorded prefrontal resting-state EEG data from 40 participants, which is followed up over nine months using a single-channel portable brainwave device. We found that portable devices can effectively and stably capture the characteristics of different subjects in the alpha band (8-13Hz) over long periods, as well as capturing their individual differences (no alpha peak, 1 alpha peak, or 2 alpha peaks). Through correlation analysis, alpha-band activity can reveal the uniqueness of the subjects compared to others within one minute. We further used a descriptive model to dissect the oscillatory and non-oscillatory components in the alpha band, demonstrating the different contributions of fine oscillatory features to individual differences (especially amplitude and bandwidth). Our study validated the feasibility of portable brainwave devices in brainwave recognition and the underlying neural oscillation mechanisms. The fine characteristics of various alpha oscillations will contribute to the accuracy of brainwave recognition, providing new insights for the development of future brainwave recognition technology.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yuchen Lin
- The Brain Cognition and Brain Disease Institute, Shenzhen Institute of Advanced Technology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Shenzhen, China
| | - Shaojia Huang
- Shenzhen Shuimu AI Technology Co., Ltd, Shenzhen, China
| | - Jidong Mao
- Shenzhen Shuimu AI Technology Co., Ltd, Shenzhen, China
| | - Meijia Li
- Faculty of Psychology and Center for Neuroscience, Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Brussels, Belgium
| | - Naem Haihambo
- Faculty of Psychology and Center for Neuroscience, Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Brussels, Belgium
| | - Fang Wang
- Shenzhen Shuimu AI Technology Co., Ltd, Shenzhen, China
| | - Yuping Liang
- Shenzhen Shuimu AI Technology Co., Ltd, Shenzhen, China
| | - Wufang Chen
- Shenzhen Shuimu AI Technology Co., Ltd, Shenzhen, China
| | - Chuanliang Han
- School of Biomedical Sciences and Gerald Choa Neuroscience Institute, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong SAR, China.
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Samantaray T, Gupta U, Saini J, Gupta CN. Unique Brain Network Identification Number for Parkinson's and Healthy Individuals Using Structural MRI. Brain Sci 2023; 13:1297. [PMID: 37759898 PMCID: PMC10526827 DOI: 10.3390/brainsci13091297] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/29/2023] [Revised: 08/25/2023] [Accepted: 09/05/2023] [Indexed: 09/29/2023] Open
Abstract
We propose a novel algorithm called Unique Brain Network Identification Number (UBNIN) for encoding the brain networks of individual subjects. To realize this objective, we employed structural MRI on 180 Parkinson's disease (PD) patients and 70 healthy controls (HC) from the National Institute of Mental Health and Neurosciences, India. We parcellated each subject's brain volume and constructed an individual adjacency matrix using the correlation between the gray matter volumes of every pair of regions. The unique code is derived from values representing connections for every node (i), weighted by a factor of 2-(i-1). The numerical representation (UBNIN) was observed to be distinct for each individual brain network, which may also be applied to other neuroimaging modalities. UBNIN ranges observed for PD were 15,360 to 17,768,936,615,460,608, and HC ranges were 12,288 to 17,733,751,438,064,640. This model may be implemented as a neural signature of a person's unique brain connectivity, thereby making it useful for brainprinting applications. Additionally, we segregated the above datasets into five age cohorts: A: ≤32 years (n1 = 4, n2 = 5), B: 33-42 years (n1 = 18, n2 = 14), C: 43-52 years (n1 = 42, n2 = 23), D: 53-62 years (n1 = 69, n2 = 22), and E: ≥63 years (n1 = 46, n2 = 6), where n1 and n2 are the number of individuals in PD and HC, respectively, to study the variation in network topology over age. Sparsity was adopted as the threshold estimate to binarize each age-based correlation matrix. Connectivity metrics were obtained using Brain Connectivity toolbox (Version 2019-03-03)-based MATLAB (R2020a) functions. For each age cohort, a decreasing trend was observed in the mean clustering coefficient with increasing sparsity. Significantly different clustering coefficients were noted in PD between age-cohort B and C (sparsity: 0.63, 0.66), C and E (sparsity: 0.66, 0.69), and in HC between E and B (sparsity: 0.75 and above 0.81), E and C (sparsity above 0.78), E and D (sparsity above 0.84), and C and D (sparsity: 0.9). Our findings suggest network connectivity patterns change with age, indicating network disruption may be due to the underlying neuropathology. Varying clustering coefficients for different cohorts indicate that information transfer between neighboring nodes changes with age. This provides evidence of age-related brain shrinkage and network degeneration. We also discuss limitations and provide an open-access link to software codes and a help file for the entire study.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tanmayee Samantaray
- Neural Engineering Lab, Department of Biosciences and Bioengineering, Indian Institute of Technology Guwahati, Guwahati 781039, India; (T.S.)
| | - Utsav Gupta
- Neural Engineering Lab, Department of Biosciences and Bioengineering, Indian Institute of Technology Guwahati, Guwahati 781039, India; (T.S.)
| | - Jitender Saini
- Department of Neuroimaging and Interventional Radiology, National Institute of Mental Health and Neurosciences, Bengaluru 560029, India;
| | - Cota Navin Gupta
- Neural Engineering Lab, Department of Biosciences and Bioengineering, Indian Institute of Technology Guwahati, Guwahati 781039, India; (T.S.)
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Farwell LA, Richardson GM. Brain fingerprinting field study on major, terrorist crimes supports the brain fingerprinting scientific standards hypothesis: classification concealed information test with P300 and P300-MERMER succeeds; comparison CIT fails. Cogn Neurodyn 2023; 17:63-104. [PMID: 36704633 PMCID: PMC9871152 DOI: 10.1007/s11571-022-09795-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/21/2020] [Revised: 01/27/2022] [Accepted: 03/01/2022] [Indexed: 01/29/2023] Open
Abstract
We conducted (I) 18 event-related potential (ERP) field tests to detect concealed information regarding major terrorist crimes and other real-world crimes and (II) 5 ERP tests regarding participation in a classified counterterrorism operation. This study is a test of the brain fingerprinting scientific standards hypothesis: that a specific set of methods for event-related potential (ERP) concealed information tests (CIT) known as the brain fingerprinting scientific standards provide the sufficient conditions to produce less than 1% error rate and greater than 95% median statistical confidence for individual determinations of whether the tested information is stored in each subject's brain. All previous published results in all laboratories are compatible with this hypothesis. We recorded P300 and P300-MERMER ERP responses to visual text stimuli of three types: targets contain known information, irrelevants contain unknown/irrelevant information, and probes contain the situation-relevant information to be tested, known only to the perpetrator and investigators. Classification CIT produced significantly better results than comparison CIT, independent of classification criteria. Classification CIT had 0% error rate; comparison CIT had 6% error rate. As in previous studies, classification-CIT median statistical confidences were approximately 99%, whereas comparison CIT statistical confidences were no better than chance for information-absent (IA) subjects (who did not know the tested information). Over half of the comparison-CIT IA determinations were invalid due to a less-than-chance computed probability of being correct. Experiment (I) results for median statistical confidence: Classification CIT, IA subjects: 98.6%; information-present (IP) subjects (who know the tested information): 99.9%; comparison CIT, IA subjects: 48.7%; IP subjects: 99.5%. Experiment (II) results (Classification CIT): error rate 0%, median statistical confidence 96.6%. Countermeasures had no effect on the classification CIT. These results, like all previous results in our laboratory and all others, support the brain fingerprinting scientific standards hypothesis and indicate that the classification CIT is a necessary condition for a reliable, accurate, and valid brainwave-based CIT. The comparison CIT, by contrast, produces high error rates and IA statistical confidences no better than chance. Supplementary Information The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1007/s11571-022-09795-1.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lawrence A. Farwell
- Brain Fingerprinting Laboratories, 8825 34th Ave NE Suite L-155, Quil Ceda Village, WA 98271 USA
| | - Graham M. Richardson
- Brain Fingerprinting Laboratories, 8825 34th Ave NE Suite L-155, Quil Ceda Village, WA 98271 USA
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Ogino M, Hamada N, Mitsukura Y. Simultaneous multiple-stimulus auditory brain-computer interface with semi-supervised learning and prior probability distribution tuning. J Neural Eng 2022; 19. [PMID: 36317357 DOI: 10.1088/1741-2552/ac9edd] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/11/2022] [Accepted: 10/31/2022] [Indexed: 11/13/2022]
Abstract
Objective.Auditory brain-computer interfaces (BCIs) enable users to select commands based on the brain activity elicited by auditory stimuli. However, existing auditory BCI paradigms cannot increase the number of available commands without decreasing the selection speed, because each stimulus needs to be presented independently and sequentially under the standard oddball paradigm. To solve this problem, we propose a double-stimulus paradigm that simultaneously presents multiple auditory stimuli.Approach.For addition to an existing auditory BCI paradigm, the best discriminable sound was chosen following a subjective assessment. The new sound was located on the right-hand side and presented simultaneously with an existing sound from the left-hand side. A total of six sounds were used for implementing the auditory BCI with a 6 × 6 letter matrix. We employ semi-supervised learning (SSL) and prior probability distribution tuning to improve the accuracy of the paradigm. The SSL method involved updating of the classifier weights, and their prior probability distributions were adjusted using the following three types of distributions: uniform, empirical, and extended empirical (e-empirical). The performance was evaluated based on the BCI accuracy and information transfer rate (ITR).Main results.The double-stimulus paradigm resulted in a BCI accuracy of 67.89 ± 11.46% and an ITR of 2.67 ± 1.09 bits min-1, in the absence of SSL and with uniform distribution. The proposed combination of SSL with e-empirical distribution improved the BCI accuracy and ITR to 74.59 ± 12.12% and 3.37 ± 1.27 bits min-1, respectively. The event-related potential analysis revealed that contralateral and right-hemispheric dominances contributed to the BCI performance improvement.Significance.Our study demonstrated that a BCI based on multiple simultaneous auditory stimuli, incorporating SSL and e-empirical prior distribution, can increase the number of commands without sacrificing typing speed beyond the acceptable level of accuracy.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mikito Ogino
- Graduate School of Science and Technology, Keio University, Yokohama, Kanagawa, Japan
| | - Nozomu Hamada
- Faculty of Science and Technology, Keio University, Yokohama, Kanagawa, Japan
| | - Yasue Mitsukura
- Faculty of Science and Technology, Keio University, Yokohama, Kanagawa, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Treatment effects on event-related EEG potentials and oscillations in Alzheimer's disease. Int J Psychophysiol 2022; 177:179-201. [PMID: 35588964 DOI: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2022.05.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/30/2021] [Revised: 05/11/2022] [Accepted: 05/12/2022] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
Abstract
Alzheimer's disease dementia (ADD) is the most diffuse neurodegenerative disorder belonging to mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and dementia in old persons. This disease is provoked by an abnormal accumulation of amyloid-beta and tauopathy proteins in the brain. Very recently, the first disease-modifying drug has been licensed with reserve (i.e., Aducanumab). Therefore, there is a need to identify and use biomarkers probing the neurophysiological underpinnings of human cognitive functions to test the clinical efficacy of that drug. In this regard, event-related electroencephalographic potentials (ERPs) and oscillations (EROs) are promising candidates. Here, an Expert Panel from the Electrophysiology Professional Interest Area of the Alzheimer's Association and Global Brain Consortium reviewed the field literature on the effects of the most used symptomatic drug against ADD (i.e., Acetylcholinesterase inhibitors) on ERPs and EROs in ADD patients with MCI and dementia at the group level. The most convincing results were found in ADD patients. In those patients, Acetylcholinesterase inhibitors partially normalized ERP P300 peak latency and amplitude in oddball paradigms using visual stimuli. In these same paradigms, those drugs partially normalize ERO phase-locking at the theta band (4-7 Hz) and spectral coherence between electrode pairs at the gamma (around 40 Hz) band. These results are of great interest and may motivate multicentric, double-blind, randomized, and placebo-controlled clinical trials in MCI and ADD patients for final cross-validation.
Collapse
|
8
|
Xu L, Xu M, Jung TP, Ming D. Review of brain encoding and decoding mechanisms for EEG-based brain-computer interface. Cogn Neurodyn 2021; 15:569-584. [PMID: 34367361 PMCID: PMC8286913 DOI: 10.1007/s11571-021-09676-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 37] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/08/2020] [Revised: 03/10/2021] [Accepted: 03/26/2021] [Indexed: 01/04/2023] Open
Abstract
A brain-computer interface (BCI) can connect humans and machines directly and has achieved successful applications in the past few decades. Many new BCI paradigms and algorithms have been developed in recent years. Therefore, it is necessary to review new progress in BCIs. This paper summarizes progress for EEG-based BCIs from the perspective of encoding paradigms and decoding algorithms, which are two key elements of BCI systems. Encoding paradigms are grouped by their underlying neural meachanisms, namely sensory- and motor-related, vision-related, cognition-related and hybrid paradigms. Decoding algorithms are reviewed in four categories, namely decomposition algorithms, Riemannian geometry, deep learning and transfer learning. This review will provide a comprehensive overview of both modern primary paradigms and algorithms, making it helpful for those who are developing BCI systems.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lichao Xu
- Academy of Medical Engineering and Translational Medicine, Tianjin University, Tianjin, China
| | - Minpeng Xu
- Academy of Medical Engineering and Translational Medicine, Tianjin University, Tianjin, China
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, College of Precision Instruments and Optoelectronics Engineering, Tianjin University, Tianjin, China
| | - Tzyy-Ping Jung
- Academy of Medical Engineering and Translational Medicine, Tianjin University, Tianjin, China
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, College of Precision Instruments and Optoelectronics Engineering, Tianjin University, Tianjin, China
- Swartz Center for Computational Neuroscience, University of California, San Diego, USA
| | - Dong Ming
- Academy of Medical Engineering and Translational Medicine, Tianjin University, Tianjin, China
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, College of Precision Instruments and Optoelectronics Engineering, Tianjin University, Tianjin, China
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Hellerstedt R, Moccia A, Brunskill CM, Bowman H, Bergström ZM. Aging reduces EEG markers of recognition despite intact performance: Implications for forensic memory detection. Cortex 2021; 140:80-97. [PMID: 33951486 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2021.03.015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/19/2020] [Revised: 02/15/2021] [Accepted: 03/17/2021] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
ERP-based forensic memory detection is based on the logic that guilty suspects will hold incriminating knowledge about crimes they have committed, and therefore should show parietal ERP positivities related to recognition when presented with reminders of their crimes. We predicted that such forensic memory detection might however be inaccurate in older adults, because of changes to recognition-related brain activity that occurs with aging. We measured both ERPs and EEG oscillations associated with episodic old/new recognition and forensic memory detection in 30 younger (age < 30) and 30 older (age > 65) adults. EEG oscillations were included as a complementary measure which is less sensitive to temporal variability and component overlap than ERPs. In line with predictions, recognition-related parietal ERP positivities were significantly reduced in the older compared to younger group in both tasks, despite highly similar behavioural performance. We also observed aging-related reductions in oscillatory markers of recognition in the forensic memory detection test, while the oscillatory effects associated with episodic recognition were similar across age groups. This pattern of results suggests that while both forensic memory detection and episodic recognition are accompanied by aging-induced reductions in parietal ERP positivities, these reductions may be caused by non-overlapping mechanisms across the two tasks. Our findings suggest that EEG-based forensic memory detection tests are less valid in older than younger populations, limiting their practical applications.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Robin Hellerstedt
- School of Psychology, University of Kent, UK; Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, UK.
| | - Arianna Moccia
- School of Psychology, University of Kent, UK; School of Psychology, University of Sussex, UK
| | | | - Howard Bowman
- School of Computing, University of Kent, UK; School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, UK
| | | |
Collapse
|
10
|
Brydges CR, Barceló F, Nguyen AT, Fox AM. Fast fronto-parietal cortical dynamics of conflict detection and context updating in a flanker task. Cogn Neurodyn 2020; 14:795-814. [PMID: 33101532 DOI: 10.1007/s11571-020-09628-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/22/2019] [Revised: 08/04/2020] [Accepted: 08/16/2020] [Indexed: 11/25/2022] Open
Abstract
Recent research has found that the traditional target P3 consists of a family of P3-like positivities that can be functionally and topographically dissociated from one another. The current study examined target N2 and P3-like subcomponents indexing conflict detection and context updating at low- and high-order levels in the neural hierarchy during cognitive control. Electroencephalographic signals were recorded from 45 young adults while they completed a hybrid go/nogo flanker task, and Residue Iteration Decomposition (RIDE) was applied to functionally dissociate these peaks. Analyses showed a stimulus-locked frontal N2 revealing early detection and fast perceptual categorization of nogo, congruent and incongruent trials, resulting in frontal P3-like activity elicited by nogo trials in the latency-variable RIDE cluster, and by incongruent trials in the response-locked cluster. The congruent trials did not elicit frontal P3-like activity. These findings suggest that behavioral incongruency effects are related to intermediate and later stages of motor response re-programming.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Christopher R Brydges
- School of Psychological Science (M304), University of Western Australia, 35 Stirling Highway, Perth, WA 6009 Australia.,Department of Human Development and Family Studies, Colorado State University, Fort Collins, USA
| | - Francisco Barceló
- Laboratory of Neuropsychology, University of the Balearic Islands, Majorca, Spain
| | - An T Nguyen
- School of Psychological Science (M304), University of Western Australia, 35 Stirling Highway, Perth, WA 6009 Australia.,Neurocognitive Development Unit, School of Psychological Science, University of Western Australia, Perth, Australia
| | - Allison M Fox
- School of Psychological Science (M304), University of Western Australia, 35 Stirling Highway, Perth, WA 6009 Australia.,Neurocognitive Development Unit, School of Psychological Science, University of Western Australia, Perth, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Olson J, Rosenfeld JP, Perrault E. Deleterious effects of probe-related versus irrelevant targets on the "CIT effect" in the P300- and RT-based three-stimulus protocol for detection of concealed information. Psychophysiology 2019; 56:e13459. [PMID: 31424105 DOI: 10.1111/psyp.13459] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/07/2019] [Revised: 06/08/2019] [Accepted: 07/13/2019] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
Two groups of participants committed the same mock crime in which one of two items, a watch or a ring, was removed from a drawer and concealed. One group, the crime-familiar group next experienced a three-stimulus protocol (3SP), a Concealed Information Test (CIT), in which they were tested on the stolen (probe) item presented in a random series of five irrelevant (unseen) stimuli from the same jewelry category. A left-hand button press, meaning "I don't recognize" was to follow each of these six items. A right-hand press ("I do recognize") was to follow the one other presented item, the target item, which in the case of the crime-familiar group was the other, not-stolen item in the drawer at the mock crime scene. For the other crime-unfamiliar group, the target was a sixth unseen irrelevant item as in the original P300 CIT. In terms of P300 latency and reaction time (RT), crime-familiar participants processed all stimuli faster than crime-unfamiliar participants. The CIT effects (probe-minus-irrelevant differences) for crime-familiar group members were inferior to those of crime-unfamiliar group members for RT and P300 amplitude measures. Thus, familiar targets negatively impact the 3SP.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Joseph Olson
- Department of Psychology, Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois
| | - J Peter Rosenfeld
- Department of Psychology, Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois
| | - Ella Perrault
- Department of Psychology, Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Bigenwald A, Chambon V. Criminal Responsibility and Neuroscience: No Revolution Yet. Front Psychol 2019; 10:1406. [PMID: 31316418 PMCID: PMC6610327 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01406] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/15/2019] [Accepted: 05/31/2019] [Indexed: 11/25/2022] Open
Abstract
Since the 1990's, neurolaw is on the rise. At the heart of heated debates lies the recurrent theme of a neuro-revolution of criminal responsibility. However, caution should be observed: the alleged foundations of criminal responsibility (amongst which free will) are often inaccurate and the relative imperviousness of its real foundations to scientific facts often underestimated. Neuroscientific findings may impact on social institutions, but only insofar as they also engage in a political justification of the changes being called for, convince populations, and take into consideration the ensuing consequences. Moreover, the many limits of neuroscientific tools call for increased vigilance when, if ever, using neuroscientific evidence in a courtroom. In this article, we aim at setting the basis for future sound debates on the contribution of neuroscience to criminal law, and in particular to the assessment of criminal responsibility. As such, we provide analytical tools to grasp the political and normative nature of criminal responsibility and review the current or projected use of neuroscience in the law, all the while bearing in mind the highly publicized question: can neuroscience revolutionize criminal responsibility? Answering this question implicitly requires answering a second question: should neuroscience revolutionize the institution of criminal responsibility? Answering both, in turn, requires drawing the line between science and normativity, revolution and dialogue, fantasies and legitimate hopes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ariane Bigenwald
- Département de Philosophie, Université Paris I Panthéon Sorbonne, Paris, France
- Institut Jean Nicod (ENS – EHESS – CNRS), Département d’Etudes Cognitives, Ecole Normale Supérieure, PSL University, Paris, France
| | - Valerian Chambon
- Institut Jean Nicod (ENS – EHESS – CNRS), Département d’Etudes Cognitives, Ecole Normale Supérieure, PSL University, Paris, France
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Kraft CJ, Giordano J. Integrating Brain Science and Law: Neuroscientific Evidence and Legal Perspectives on Protecting Individual Liberties. Front Neurosci 2017; 11:621. [PMID: 29167633 PMCID: PMC5682320 DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2017.00621] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/29/2017] [Accepted: 10/24/2017] [Indexed: 12/02/2022] Open
Abstract
Advances in neuroscientific techniques have found increasingly broader applications, including in legal neuroscience (or “neurolaw”), where experts in the brain sciences are called to testify in the courtroom. But does the incursion of neuroscience into the legal sphere constitute a threat to individual liberties? And what legal protections are there against such threats? In this paper, we outline individual rights as they interact with neuroscientific methods. We then proceed to examine the current uses of neuroscientific evidence, and ultimately determine whether the rights of the individual are endangered by such approaches. Based on our analysis, we conclude that while federal evidence rules constitute a substantial hurdle for the use of neuroscientific evidence, more ethical safeguards are needed to protect against future violations of fundamental rights. Finally, we assert that it will be increasingly imperative for the legal and neuroscientific communities to work together to better define the limits, capabilities, and intended direction of neuroscientific methods applicable for use in law.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Calvin J Kraft
- Program of Liberal Studies, Neuroscience and Behavior, University of Notre Dame, Notre Dame, IN, United States.,Departments of Neurology and Biochemistry, Pellegrino Center for Clinical Bioethics, Georgetown University Medical Center, Washington, DC, United States
| | - James Giordano
- Departments of Neurology and Biochemistry, Pellegrino Center for Clinical Bioethics, Georgetown University Medical Center, Washington, DC, United States
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Huang M, Jin J, Zhang Y, Hu D, Wang X. Usage of drip drops as stimuli in an auditory P300 BCI paradigm. Cogn Neurodyn 2017; 12:85-94. [PMID: 29435089 DOI: 10.1007/s11571-017-9456-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/17/2017] [Revised: 07/17/2017] [Accepted: 10/10/2017] [Indexed: 11/28/2022] Open
Abstract
Recently, many auditory BCIs are using beeps as auditory stimuli, while beeps sound unnatural and unpleasant for some people. It is proved that natural sounds make people feel comfortable, decrease fatigue, and improve the performance of auditory BCI systems. Drip drop is a kind of natural sounds that makes humans feel relaxed and comfortable. In this work, three kinds of drip drops were used as stimuli in an auditory-based BCI system to improve the user-friendness of the system. This study explored whether drip drops could be used as stimuli in the auditory BCI system. The auditory BCI paradigm with drip-drop stimuli, which was called the drip-drop paradigm (DP), was compared with the auditory paradigm with beep stimuli, also known as the beep paradigm (BP), in items of event-related potential amplitudes, online accuracies and scores on the likability and difficulty to demonstrate the advantages of DP. DP obtained significantly higher online accuracy and information transfer rate than the BP (p < 0.05, Wilcoxon signed test; p < 0.05, Wilcoxon signed test). Besides, DP obtained higher scores on the likability with no significant difference on the difficulty (p < 0.05, Wilcoxon signed test). The results showed that the drip drops were reliable acoustic materials as stimuli in an auditory BCI system.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Minqiang Huang
- 1Key Laboratory of Advanced Control and Optimization for Chemical Processes, Ministry of Education, East China University of Science and Technology, Shanghai, People's Republic of China
| | - Jing Jin
- 1Key Laboratory of Advanced Control and Optimization for Chemical Processes, Ministry of Education, East China University of Science and Technology, Shanghai, People's Republic of China
| | - Yu Zhang
- 1Key Laboratory of Advanced Control and Optimization for Chemical Processes, Ministry of Education, East China University of Science and Technology, Shanghai, People's Republic of China
| | - Dewen Hu
- 2College of Mechatronics and Automation, National University of Defense Technology, Changsha, Hunan 410073 People's Republic of China
| | - Xingyu Wang
- 1Key Laboratory of Advanced Control and Optimization for Chemical Processes, Ministry of Education, East China University of Science and Technology, Shanghai, People's Republic of China
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
|
16
|
Di H, Zhang X. Deception Detection by Hybrid-Pair Wireless fNIRS System. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF DIGITAL CRIME AND FORENSICS 2017. [DOI: 10.4018/ijdcf.2017040102] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
Abstract
Normal fNIRS setting up was limited by superficial physiological noises when applied into the deception detection. We designed a hybrid-pair wireless fNIRS system to improve the detection. The system takes advantages of short-pair channel to suppress the effect of physiological noises, and wireless module to improve the comfortableness of wearing it. We applied the system into a modified Guilty Knowledge Test. The experiment demonstrated that normal metrics might hint different energy consume during lying, while the regional oxygen saturation rSO2, specific in the system, is sensitive to indicate a lying.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hong Di
- University of International Relations, Department of Information Science and Technology, Beijing, China
| | - Xin Zhang
- Chinese Academy of Sciences, Institute of Automation, Beijing, China
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Huang M, Daly I, Jin J, Zhang Y, Wang X, Cichocki A. An exploration of spatial auditory BCI paradigms with different sounds: music notes versus beeps. Cogn Neurodyn 2016; 10:201-9. [PMID: 27275376 PMCID: PMC4870409 DOI: 10.1007/s11571-016-9377-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/31/2015] [Revised: 01/13/2016] [Accepted: 01/19/2016] [Indexed: 10/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Visual brain-computer interfaces (BCIs) are not suitable for people who cannot reliably maintain their eye gaze. Considering that this group usually maintains audition, an auditory based BCI may be a good choice for them. In this paper, we explore two auditory patterns: (1) a pattern utilizing symmetrical spatial cues with multiple frequency beeps [called the high low medium (HLM) pattern], and (2) a pattern utilizing non-symmetrical spatial cues with six tones derived from the diatonic scale [called the diatonic scale (DS) pattern]. These two patterns are compared to each other in terms of accuracy to determine which auditory pattern is better. The HLM pattern uses three different frequency beeps and has a symmetrical spatial distribution. The DS pattern uses six spoken stimuli, which are six notes solmizated as "do", "re", "mi", "fa", "sol" and "la", and derived from the diatonic scale. These six sounds are distributed to six, spatially distributed, speakers. Thus, we compare a BCI paradigm using beeps with another BCI paradigm using tones on the diatonic scale, when the stimuli are spatially distributed. Although no significant differences are found between the ERPs, the HLM pattern performs better than the DS pattern: the online accuracy achieved with the HLM pattern is significantly higher than that achieved with the DS pattern (p = 0.0028).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Minqiang Huang
- />Key Laboratory of Advanced Control and Optimization for Chemical Processes, Ministry of Education, East China University of Science and Technology, Shanghai, People’s Republic of China
| | - Ian Daly
- />Brain Embodiment Lab, School of Systems Engineering, University of Reading, Reading, UK
| | - Jing Jin
- />Key Laboratory of Advanced Control and Optimization for Chemical Processes, Ministry of Education, East China University of Science and Technology, Shanghai, People’s Republic of China
| | - Yu Zhang
- />Key Laboratory of Advanced Control and Optimization for Chemical Processes, Ministry of Education, East China University of Science and Technology, Shanghai, People’s Republic of China
| | - Xingyu Wang
- />Key Laboratory of Advanced Control and Optimization for Chemical Processes, Ministry of Education, East China University of Science and Technology, Shanghai, People’s Republic of China
| | - Andrzej Cichocki
- />Lab for Advanced Brain Signal Processing and BTCC, Riken BSI, Wako-shi, Japan
- />Skolkowo Institute of Science and Technology, Moscow, Russia
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Meijer EH, Verschuere B, Gamer M, Merckelbach H, Ben-Shakhar G. Deception detection with behavioral, autonomic, and neural measures: Conceptual and methodological considerations that warrant modesty. Psychophysiology 2016; 53:593-604. [DOI: 10.1111/psyp.12609] [Citation(s) in RCA: 41] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/08/2015] [Accepted: 12/18/2015] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Ewout H. Meijer
- Department of Clinical Psychological Science; Maastricht University; Maastricht The Netherlands
| | - Bruno Verschuere
- Department of Clinical Psychological Science; Maastricht University; Maastricht The Netherlands
- Department of Clinical Psychology; University of Amsterdam; Amsterdam The Netherlands
| | - Matthias Gamer
- Department of Psychology; University of Würzburg; Würzburg Germany
| | - Harald Merckelbach
- Department of Clinical Psychological Science; Maastricht University; Maastricht The Netherlands
| | | |
Collapse
|
19
|
Vicianova M. Historical Techniques of Lie Detection. EUROPES JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY 2015; 11:522-34. [PMID: 27247675 PMCID: PMC4873061 DOI: 10.5964/ejop.v11i3.919] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/13/2015] [Accepted: 07/17/2015] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
Since time immemorial, lying has been a part of everyday life. For this reason, it has become a subject of interest in several disciplines, including psychology. The purpose of this article is to provide a general overview of the literature and thinking to date about the evolution of lie detection techniques. The first part explores ancient methods recorded circa 1000 B.C. (e.g., God’s judgment in Europe). The second part describes technical methods based on sciences such as phrenology, polygraph and graphology. This is followed by an outline of more modern-day approaches such as FACS (Facial Action Coding System), functional MRI, and Brain Fingerprinting. Finally, after the familiarization with the historical development of techniques for lie detection, we discuss the scope for new initiatives not only in the area of designing new methods, but also for the research into lie detection itself, such as its motives and regulatory issues related to deception.
Collapse
|
20
|
Neuman Y, Assaf D, Israeli N. Identifying the location of a concealed object through unintentional eye movements. Front Psychol 2015; 6:381. [PMID: 25904879 PMCID: PMC4389367 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00381] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/20/2014] [Accepted: 03/17/2015] [Indexed: 11/15/2022] Open
Abstract
In some investigative and interrogative contexts, the investigator is seeking to identify the location of an object (e.g., implanted bomb) which is known to a given subject (e.g., a terrorist). In this paper, we present a non-intrusive methodology for uncovering the loci of a concealed object by analyzing the subject's eye movements. Using a combination of eye tracking, psychological manipulation and a search algorithm, we have performed two experiments. In the first experiment, we have gained 58% hit rate in identifying the location of the concealed object and in the second experiment 56% hit rate. The pros and cons of the methodology for forensic investigation are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yair Neuman
- Homeland Security Institute and Department of Education, Ben-Gurion University of the NegevBeer-Sheva, Israel
- *Correspondence: Yair Neuman, Homeland Security Institute and Department of Education, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beer-Sheva 84105, Israel
| | - Dan Assaf
- Independent ResearcherPetakh-Tikva, Israel
| | | |
Collapse
|
21
|
Credibility assessment: Preliminary Process Theory, the polygraph process, and construct validity. Int J Psychophysiol 2015; 95:3-13. [DOI: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.06.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/28/2013] [Revised: 06/02/2014] [Accepted: 06/04/2014] [Indexed: 11/21/2022]
|
22
|
Farwell LA, Richardson DC, Richardson GM, Furedy JJ. Brain fingerprinting classification concealed information test detects US Navy military medical information with P300. Front Neurosci 2014; 8:410. [PMID: 25565941 PMCID: PMC4274905 DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00410] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/31/2014] [Accepted: 11/23/2014] [Indexed: 12/05/2022] Open
Abstract
A classification concealed information test (CIT) used the “brain fingerprinting” method of applying P300 event-related potential (ERP) in detecting information that is (1) acquired in real life and (2) unique to US Navy experts in military medicine. Military medicine experts and non-experts were asked to push buttons in response to three types of text stimuli. Targets contain known information relevant to military medicine, are identified to subjects as relevant, and require pushing one button. Subjects are told to push another button to all other stimuli. Probes contain concealed information relevant to military medicine, and are not identified to subjects. Irrelevants contain equally plausible, but incorrect/irrelevant information. Error rate was 0%. Median and mean statistical confidences for individual determinations were 99.9% with no indeterminates (results lacking sufficiently high statistical confidence to be classified). We compared error rate and statistical confidence for determinations of both information present and information absent produced by classification CIT (Is a probe ERP more similar to a target or to an irrelevant ERP?) vs. comparison CIT (Does a probe produce a larger ERP than an irrelevant?) using P300 plus the late negative component (LNP; together, P300-MERMER). Comparison CIT produced a significantly higher error rate (20%) and lower statistical confidences: mean 67%; information-absent mean was 28.9%, less than chance (50%). We compared analysis using P300 alone with the P300 + LNP. P300 alone produced the same 0% error rate but significantly lower statistical confidences. These findings add to the evidence that the brain fingerprinting methods as described here provide sufficient conditions to produce less than 1% error rate and greater than 95% median statistical confidence in a CIT on information obtained in the course of real life that is characteristic of individuals with specific training, expertise, or organizational affiliation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lawrence A Farwell
- Brain Fingerprinting Laboratories, Inc./Brain Fingerprinting, LLC Seattle, WA, USA
| | - Drew C Richardson
- Federal Bureau of Investigation, FBI Laboratory Quantico, VA, USA (at the time of the research)
| | - Graham M Richardson
- Department of Cell and Developmental Biology, Vanderbilt University Nashville, TN, USA
| | - John J Furedy
- Department of Psychology, University of Toronto Toronto, ON, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
The Effects of Sweep Numbers Per Average and Protocol Type on the Accuracy of the P300-Based Concealed Information Test. Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback 2014; 39:67-73. [DOI: 10.1007/s10484-014-9244-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
|
24
|
Detección de información oculta mediante potenciales relacionados con eventos. ANUARIO DE PSICOLOGÍA JURÍDICA 2014. [DOI: 10.1016/j.apj.2014.06.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/20/2022] Open
|
25
|
Farwell LA, Richardson DC, Richardson GM. Brain fingerprinting field studies comparing P300-MERMER and P300 brainwave responses in the detection of concealed information. Cogn Neurodyn 2013; 7:263-99. [PMID: 23869200 PMCID: PMC3713201 DOI: 10.1007/s11571-012-9230-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/28/2012] [Revised: 08/29/2012] [Accepted: 11/20/2012] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
Brain fingerprinting detects concealed information stored in the brain by measuring brainwave responses. We compared P300 and P300-MERMER event-related brain potentials for error rate/accuracy and statistical confidence in four field/real-life studies. 76 tests detected presence or absence of information regarding (1) real-life events including felony crimes; (2) real crimes with substantial consequences (either a judicial outcome, i.e., evidence admitted in court, or a $100,000 reward for beating the test); (3) knowledge unique to FBI agents; and (4) knowledge unique to explosives (EOD/IED) experts. With both P300 and P300-MERMER, error rate was 0 %: determinations were 100 % accurate, no false negatives or false positives; also no indeterminates. Countermeasures had no effect. Median statistical confidence for determinations was 99.9 % with P300-MERMER and 99.6 % with P300. Brain fingerprinting methods and scientific standards for laboratory and field applications are discussed. Major differences in methods that produce different results are identified. Markedly different methods in other studies have produced over 10 times higher error rates and markedly lower statistical confidences than those of these, our previous studies, and independent replications. Data support the hypothesis that accuracy, reliability, and validity depend on following the brain fingerprinting scientific standards outlined herein.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lawrence A. Farwell
- Government Works, Inc., Brainwave Science, 257 Turnpike Road, Southborough, MA 01772 USA
| | - Drew C. Richardson
- Present Address: Federal Bureau of Investigation (FBI) Laboratory, 314 High Meadow Lane, Greenville, VA 24440 USA
| | - Graham M. Richardson
- Department of Cell and Developmental Biology, Vanderbilt University, MRB III Laboratory U 3200, 465 21st Ave. South, Nashville, TN 37232 USA
| |
Collapse
|
26
|
Pan J, Li Y, Gu Z, Yu Z. A comparison study of two P300 speller paradigms for brain-computer interface. Cogn Neurodyn 2013; 7:523-9. [PMID: 24427224 DOI: 10.1007/s11571-013-9253-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/09/2012] [Revised: 03/15/2013] [Accepted: 04/05/2013] [Indexed: 11/30/2022] Open
Abstract
In this paper, a comparison of two existing P300 spellers is conducted. In the first speller, the visual stimuli of characters are presented in a single character (SC) paradigm and each button corresponding to a character flashes individually in a random order. The second speller is based on a region-based (RB) paradigm. In the first level, all characters are grouped and each button corresponding to a group flashes individually in a random order. Once a group is selected, the characters in it will appear on the flashing buttons of the second level for the selection of desired character. In a spelling experiment involving 12 subjects, higher online accuracy was obtained on the RB paradigm-based P300 speller than the SC paradigm-based P300 speller. Furthermore, we analyzed P300 detection performance, the P300 waveforms and Fisher ratios using the data collected by the two spellers. It was found that the stimuli display paradigm of the RB speller enhances P300 potential and is more suitable for P300 detection.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jiahui Pan
- School of Nanhai College, South China Normal University, Guangzhou, 510640 China
| | - Yuanqing Li
- School of Automation Science and Engineering, South China University of Technology, Guangzhou, 510640 China
| | - Zhenghui Gu
- School of Automation Science and Engineering, South China University of Technology, Guangzhou, 510640 China
| | - Zhuliang Yu
- School of Automation Science and Engineering, South China University of Technology, Guangzhou, 510640 China
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Meijer EH, Ben-Shakhar G, Verschuere B, Donchin E. A comment on Farwell (2012): brain fingerprinting: a comprehensive tutorial review of detection of concealed information with event-related brain potentials. Cogn Neurodyn 2013; 7:155-8. [PMID: 23493984 PMCID: PMC3595430 DOI: 10.1007/s11571-012-9217-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/31/2012] [Revised: 07/30/2012] [Accepted: 08/07/2012] [Indexed: 12/03/2022] Open
Abstract
In a recent issue of Cognitive Neurodynamics Farwell (Cogn Neurodyn 6:115-154, 2012) published a comprehensive tutorial review of the use of Event Related Brain Potentials (ERP) in the detection of concealed information. Farwell's review covered much of his own work employing his "brain fingerprinting" technology. All his work showed a 100 % accuracy rate in detecting concealed information. We argue in this comment that Farwell (Cogn Neurodyn 6:115-154, 2012) is misleading and misrepresents the scientific status of brain fingerprinting technology.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ewout H. Meijer
- Department of Clinical Psychological Science, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, PO BOX 616, 6200 MD Maastricht, The Netherlands
- Department of Psychology, The Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Jerusalem, Israel
| | - Gershon Ben-Shakhar
- Department of Psychology, The Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Jerusalem, Israel
| | - Bruno Verschuere
- Department of Clinical Psychological Science, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, PO BOX 616, 6200 MD Maastricht, The Netherlands
- Faculty of Social and Behavioural Sciences, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
- Department of Psychology, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium
| | - Emanuel Donchin
- Department of Psychology, University of South Florida, Tampa, FL USA
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
Farwell LA, Richardson DC. Brain fingerprinting: let's focus on the science-a reply to Meijer, Ben-Shakhar, Verschuere, and Donchin. Cogn Neurodyn 2013; 7:159-66. [PMID: 23494087 PMCID: PMC3595431 DOI: 10.1007/s11571-012-9238-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/12/2012] [Revised: 12/09/2012] [Accepted: 12/24/2012] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Farwell in Cogn Neurodyn 6:115-154, (2012) reviewed all research on brainwave-based detection of concealed information published in English, including the author's laboratory and field research. He hypothesized that specific methods are sufficient to obtain less than 1 % error rate and high statistical confidence, and some of them are necessary. Farwell proposed 20 brain fingerprinting scientific standards embodying these methods. He documented the fact that all previous research and data are compatible with these hypotheses and standards. Farwell explained why failure to meet these standards resulted in decrements in performance of other, alternative methods. Meijer et al. criticized Farwell in Cogn Neurodyn 6:115-154, (2012) and Farwell personally. The authors stated their disagreement with Farwell's hypotheses, but did not cite any data that contradict the three hypotheses, nor did they propose alternative hypotheses or standards. Meijer et al. made demonstrable misstatements of fact, including false ad hominem statements about Farwell, and impugned Farwell's motives and character. We provide supporting evidence for Farwell's three hypotheses, clarify several issues, correct Meijer et al.'s misstatements of fact, and propose that the progress of science is best served by practicing science: designing and conducting research to test and as necessary modify the proposed hypotheses and standards that explain the existing data.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lawrence A. Farwell
- Brainwave Science/Brain Fingerprinting Laboratories, Government Works, Inc., 257 Turnpike Road, Suite 220, Southborough, MA 01772 USA
| | - Drew C. Richardson
- FBI Laboratory (at the time of the research), 314 High Meadow Lane, Greenville, VA 24440 USA
| |
Collapse
|