1
|
Hong SJ, Cho H. Extending and Testing Protection Motivation Theory in the Context of COVID-19 Contact-Tracing Technology: A Comparison of South Korea and the United States. HEALTH COMMUNICATION 2025:1-14. [PMID: 39749670 DOI: 10.1080/10410236.2024.2447107] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/04/2025]
Abstract
This study applies protection motivation theory (PMT) to the COVID-19 contact-tracing context by including privacy concerns, collective efficacy, and a mediator (fear of COVID-19) and tests it in the US and South Korea. The study uses a structural equation modeling (SEM) approach and a sample of 418 Americans and 444 South Koreans. According to the results, fear was positively associated with adoption intentions in the US sample but not in the Korean sample. Coping appraisals positively affected adoption intentions in both samples. However, while all types of coping appraisals were significant in the Korean sample, response efficacy was the only significant predictor among US participants. Privacy concerns were negatively associated with adoption intentions in the US sample, but not in the Korean sample. The results indicate that differences exist in the mediating role of fear connecting threat appraisals and adoption intentions in both countries. These findings hold important implications for future studies in AI-based health communication, especially in the areas of privacy management, protection motivation, and diverse cultural contexts.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Soo Jung Hong
- Department of Communications and New Media, National University of Singapore
| | - Hichang Cho
- Department of Communications and New Media, National University of Singapore
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Liu T, Zhang Y, Zhang M, Chen M, Yu S. Factors Influencing Consumer Willingness to Use AI-Driven Autonomous Taxis. Behav Sci (Basel) 2024; 14:1216. [PMID: 39767357 PMCID: PMC11673982 DOI: 10.3390/bs14121216] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/30/2024] [Revised: 12/15/2024] [Accepted: 12/16/2024] [Indexed: 01/11/2025] Open
Abstract
The advancement of autonomous driving technology, particularly Tesla's launch of its new Robotaxi, marks a transformation in transportation. Understanding the theoretical mechanisms that drive consumers' intention to use autonomous taxis is essential. This study develops a structural equation model (SEM), extending the applicability of the TAM and TPB model, and incorporates external factors like attitudes, subjective norms, traffic efficiency, and perceived cost-benefit into the model to analyze their impact on consumers' perceived characteristics (perceived usefulness and perceived ease of use). A survey of 427 valid responses revealed that attitudes, subjective norms, and perceived cost-benefit all have significant positive impacts on perceived usefulness and ease of use, which, in turn, are the primary drivers of consumers' intention to use. Additionally, perceived risk significantly weakens the positive effects of perceived usefulness and ease of use on the intention to use, underscoring its critical moderating role in the technology acceptance process. This paper suggests strategies to enhance consumer acceptance, including strengthening user perception through marketing and public experience activities, optimizing technology to improve user experience, reinforcing safety and privacy measures to reduce perceived risk, and highlighting the insurance mechanism, convenience, and economic benefits of autonomous taxis in marketing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tingyu Liu
- Department of Economics, Sejong University, Seoul 05006, Republic of Korea; (T.L.); (M.C.); (S.Y.)
- Economic and Trade Department, Yancheng Polytechnic College, Yancheng 224005, China
| | - Yizhou Zhang
- Department of Global Business, Anyang University, Anyang-si 14028, Republic of Korea;
| | - Mengze Zhang
- Department of Economics, Sejong University, Seoul 05006, Republic of Korea; (T.L.); (M.C.); (S.Y.)
| | - Min Chen
- Department of Economics, Sejong University, Seoul 05006, Republic of Korea; (T.L.); (M.C.); (S.Y.)
| | - Shangchun Yu
- Department of Economics, Sejong University, Seoul 05006, Republic of Korea; (T.L.); (M.C.); (S.Y.)
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Behnke M, Pietruch M, Chwiłkowska P, Wessel E, Kaczmarek LD, Assink M, Gross JJ. The Undoing Effect of Positive Emotions: A Meta-Analytic Review. EMOTION REVIEW 2022. [DOI: 10.1177/17540739221104457] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
The undoing hypothesis proposes that positive emotions serve to undo sympathetic arousal related to negative emotions and stress. However, a recent qualitative review challenged the undoing effect by presenting conflicting results. To address this issue quantitatively, we conducted a meta-analytic review of 16 studies ( N = 1,220; 72 effect sizes) measuring sympathetic recovery during elicited positive emotions and neutral conditions. Findings indicated that in most cases, positive emotions did not speed sympathetic recovery compared to neutral conditions. However, when a composite index of cardiovascular reactivity was used, undoing effects were evident. Our findings suggest the need for further work on the functions of positive emotions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Maciej Behnke
- Faculty of Psychology and Cognitive Science, Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznan, Wielkopolskie, Poland
| | - Magdalena Pietruch
- Faculty of Psychology and Cognitive Science, Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznan, Wielkopolskie, Poland
| | - Patrycja Chwiłkowska
- Faculty of Psychology and Cognitive Science, Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznan, Wielkopolskie, Poland
| | - Eliza Wessel
- Faculty of Psychology and Cognitive Science, Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznan, Wielkopolskie, Poland
| | - Lukasz D. Kaczmarek
- Faculty of Psychology and Cognitive Science, Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznan, Wielkopolskie, Poland
| | - Mark Assink
- Research Institute of Child Development and Education, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Noord-Holland, Netherlands
| | - James J. Gross
- Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, California, United States
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Moore JR, Guerra ZC, Heydarian NM, Londoño T, Castro Y. Associations of positive and negative affect on health risk behaviors among Latinos. Health Psychol 2022; 41:145-154. [PMID: 35157479 PMCID: PMC10868635 DOI: 10.1037/hea0001165] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 09/23/2023]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE Negative affect is widely emphasized in behavior modification interventions. However, positive affect is associated with smoking cessation, physical activity, and healthy dietary habits and may be an important treatment target. Few studies have examined the relationship between positive affect and health behaviors among Latinos, who disproportionately experience modifiable health risk behaviors. This study examined the independent associations of positive affect and negative affect on modifiable health risk behaviors among Latino adults. METHOD Data came from 432 persons who participated in one of two studies examining determinants of modifiable health risk behaviors among Latino adults. Primary outcomes were current smoking, at-risk drinking, insufficient physical activity, and insufficient fruit and vegetable consumption. Predictors were positive and negative affect as measured by the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule. Associations between positive and negative affect and health risk behaviors were examined using multiple logistic regression. RESULTS Positive affect was associated with lower odds of insufficient physical activity and insufficient fruit and vegetable consumption and lower odds of engaging in multiple health risk behaviors. Negative affect was associated with higher odds of at-risk drinking and current smoking. CONCLUSIONS Emphasis on increasing positive affect may be warranted for multiple health risk behavior interventions targeting Latinos. Intervention development efforts may also consider prioritizing positive affect for physical activity and dietary interventions, and negative affect for at-drinking and smoking cessation interventions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- John R. Moore
- Steve Hicks School of Social Work, The University of Texas at Austin, Austin, Texas
| | - Zully C. Guerra
- Steve Hicks School of Social Work, The University of Texas at Austin, Austin, Texas
| | - Nazanin M. Heydarian
- Steve Hicks School of Social Work, The University of Texas at Austin, Austin, Texas
| | - Tatiana Londoño
- Steve Hicks School of Social Work, The University of Texas at Austin, Austin, Texas
| | - Yessenia Castro
- Steve Hicks School of Social Work, The University of Texas at Austin, Austin, Texas
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Shirish A. Cognitive-affective appraisal of technostressors by ICT-based mobile workers and their impacts on technostrain. HUMAN SYSTEMS MANAGEMENT 2021. [DOI: 10.3233/hsm-200979] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND: ICT based mobile working gives organizational flexibility, productivity and performance but at the same time it can lead to techno-stress and technostrain perceptions. A high level of technostrain amongst ICT-based mobile workers would impact their well-being, leading to lesser than expected gains from such organizational ICT investments. Given this paradox, we examine the actual transactional and relational stressor-strain coping response processes in this novel context. OBJECTIVE: The broad research aim of this paper is to explain the relative importance of the cognitive and affective processes used amongst ICT-based mobile workers when coping with technostressors. METHODS: Specifically, based on technology frames literature, we develop dual-path serial mediation models, showing the relationships between technostress-technostrain via two processes: (a) the primary cognitive appraisal process mobilization (threat/opportunity technology frame) and (b) the secondary affective resource process mobilization (affect towards ICT use) to account for technostrain perceptions. We use survey data from 165 ICT-based mobile managers from diverse work settings to empirically confirm the theorized models. RESULTS/CONCLUSIONS: A predominant cognitive ‘threat frames’ leads to increase in technostrain, which decreases if ‘affective resource’ is available for coping. This relationship is inverse in the case of ‘opportunity frames’ path, as technostrain perceptions decreases with and without affective resource mobilization. Implications to theory, practice and methods are also discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anuragini Shirish
- LITEM, Université Evry, IMT-BS, Université Paris-Saclay, Evry, France
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Clemens KS, Tull MT, Murray AB, Boardley D, Tipton J, Geers AL. Examination of the relationship between affect, values, and physical activity among cancer survivors. JOURNAL OF CONTEXTUAL BEHAVIORAL SCIENCE 2020. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jcbs.2020.08.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
|
7
|
Klasko-Foster LB, Przybyla S, Orom H, Gage-Bouchard E, Kiviniemi MT. The influence of affect on HPV vaccine decision making in an HPV vaccine naïve college student population. Prev Med Rep 2020; 20:101195. [PMID: 32983851 PMCID: PMC7498828 DOI: 10.1016/j.pmedr.2020.101195] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/05/2020] [Revised: 07/30/2020] [Accepted: 08/23/2020] [Indexed: 01/05/2023] Open
Abstract
The HPV vaccine is recommended for all adolescents starting at age 11, but coverage is low, especially in the young adult population. The CDC is prioritizing catch-up vaccination and has expanded recommendations for all young adults to age 26. College students may be ideal targets for HPV vaccine interventions as they typically have on-site clinics that offer prevention services and students are in the position to make decisions about their own healthcare. We examined the risk perceptions of 101 HPV vaccine-naïve college students, both in terms of risk cognition (beliefs about susceptibility to HPV-related cancers and genital warts) and affect (worry and fear regarding HPV-related health outcomes) as they relate to HPV vaccine intentions. Participants completed an online survey, reporting absolute and comparative risk perceptions for HPV-related cancers/genital warts, fear and worry related to getting HPV-related cancer and/or genital warts, desire for positive emotions, affective associations with the HPV vaccine, and intentions to get the HPV vaccine. More fear/worry about vaccination was directly associated with increased vaccine intentions. The perceived risk to intentions relation included an indirect effect via fear/worry. Desire for positive affect strengthened this relation. Positive affective associations with the HPV vaccine were also related to increased vaccine intentions. Given the public health impact of increasing HPV vaccine coverage for young adults, educational strategies framing the HPV vaccine positively while decreasing fear/worry related to negative health outcomes might increase interest in on-campus catch-up vaccination.
Collapse
|
8
|
Behaviour change interventions: getting in touch with individual differences, values and emotions. J Dev Orig Health Dis 2020; 11:589-598. [PMID: 32718366 DOI: 10.1017/s2040174420000604] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
Abstract
Systematic reviews and meta-analyses suggest that behaviour change interventions have modest effect sizes, struggle to demonstrate effect in the long term and that there is high heterogeneity between studies. Such interventions take huge effort to design and run for relatively small returns in terms of changes to behaviour.So why do behaviour change interventions not work and how can we make them more effective? This article offers some ideas about what may underpin the failure of behaviour change interventions. We propose three main reasons that may explain why our current methods of conducting behaviour change interventions struggle to achieve the changes we expect: 1) our current model for testing the efficacy or effectiveness of interventions tends to a mean effect size. This ignores individual differences in response to interventions; 2) our interventions tend to assume that everyone values health in the way we do as health professionals; and 3) the great majority of our interventions focus on addressing cognitions as mechanisms of change. We appeal to people's logic and rationality rather than recognising that much of what we do and how we behave, including our health behaviours, is governed as much by how we feel and how engaged we are emotionally as it is with what we plan and intend to do.Drawing on our team's experience of developing multiple interventions to promote and support health behaviour change with a variety of populations in different global contexts, this article explores strategies with potential to address these issues.
Collapse
|
9
|
Klasko-Foster LB, Keller MM, Kiviniemi MT. Is it disgusting or am I just easily disgusted? The relation between situational disgust, dispositional disgust, and colonoscopy intentions. Eur J Cancer Care (Engl) 2020; 29:e13244. [PMID: 32596994 DOI: 10.1111/ecc.13244] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/31/2018] [Revised: 03/03/2020] [Accepted: 04/16/2020] [Indexed: 01/08/2023]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE While early detection is an effective way to reduce mortality from colorectal cancer, screening rates are low. An underlying factor in screening completion failure may be experiences of disgust when learning about screening and/or dispositional disgust. METHOD Participants recruited via Amazon MTurk (N = 296) read information about colonoscopy and completed an online survey assessing both dispositional forms of disgust (i.e. trait disgust and disgust sensitivity) and situational forms, including state disgust and disgust associated with colonoscopy. Participants reported intentions to discuss colonoscopy with a provider and to prepare for and complete screening. RESULTS Greater state disgust and the degree to which one associated disgust with colonoscopy predicted lower screening, preparation and provider discussion intentions. By contrast, neither trait disgust nor disgust sensitivity was associated with intentions. Both disgust sensitivity and trait disgust moderated the state disgust to intentions relation. CONCLUSIONS This is one of few investigations of disgust examining the relation between specific types and colonoscopy intentions. Screening uptake may be improved by identifying specific components of disgust that have an effect on colonoscopy intentions. Future work focusing on the interplay between different disgust mechanisms as they relate to colonoscopy behaviour is important for intervention development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lynne B Klasko-Foster
- Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, Alpert Medical School, Brown University, Providence, RI, USA
| | - Maria M Keller
- Department of Community Health and Health Behavior, School of Public Health and Health Professions, University at Buffalo, SUNY, Buffalo, NY, USA
| | - Marc T Kiviniemi
- Department of Health, Behavior, and Society, College of Public Health, University of Kentucky, Lexington, KY, USA
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Klasko-Foster LB, Kiviniemi MT, Jandorf LH, Erwin DO. Affective components of perceived risk mediate the relation between cognitively-based perceived risk and colonoscopy screening. J Behav Med 2020; 43:121-130. [PMID: 31065890 PMCID: PMC6834895 DOI: 10.1007/s10865-019-00049-w] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/18/2018] [Accepted: 04/24/2019] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
Perceived risk is a common component of health decision making theory. When affective components of risk are assessed as predictors of a behavior, they are usually examined separately from cognitive components. Less frequently examined are more complex interplays between affect and cognition. We hypothesized that cognitive and affective risk components would both have direct effects on colonoscopy behavior/intentions and that affective components would mediate the relationship of cognitively-based perceived risk to colonoscopy screening. In two secondary analyses, participants reported their cognitive and affective perceived risk for colorectal cancer, past colonoscopy behavior, and future screening intentions. In both studies, cognitive and affective risk components were associated with increased screening behavior/intentions and cognitive risk components were mediated through affective risk. Given the impact of early detection on colorectal cancer prevention, educational strategies highlighting both components of risk may be important to increase screening rates.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lynne B Klasko-Foster
- Department of Community Health and Health Behavior, School of Public Health and Health Professions, University at Buffalo, SUNY, 3435 Main Street, 312 Kimball Tower, Buffalo, NY, 14214, USA.
| | - Marc T Kiviniemi
- Department of Health, Behavior, and Society, College of Public Health, University of Kentucky, Bowman Hall Room 346, 151 Washington Avenue, Lexington, KY, 40536, USA
| | - Lina H Jandorf
- Department of Population Health Science and Policy, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, One Gustave L. Levy Place, Box 1077, New York, NY, 10029, USA
| | - Deborah O Erwin
- Department of Cancer Prevention and Control, Roswell Park Comprehensive Cancer Center, Elm and Carlton Streets, Buffalo, NY, 14263, USA
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Iosifyan M, Schmidt M, Hurbault A, Mayaux J, Delafosse C, Mishenko M, Nion N, Demoule A, Similowski T. "I had the feeling that I was trapped": a bedside qualitative study of cognitive and affective attitudes toward noninvasive ventilation in patients with acute respiratory failure. Ann Intensive Care 2019; 9:134. [PMID: 31792644 PMCID: PMC6888797 DOI: 10.1186/s13613-019-0608-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/07/2019] [Accepted: 11/22/2019] [Indexed: 12/15/2022] Open
Abstract
Background Noninvasive ventilation (NIV) is the application of mechanical ventilation through a mask. It is used to treat certain forms of acute respiratory failure in intensive care units (ICU). NIV has clinical benefits but can be anxiogenic for the patients. This study aimed at describing cognitive and affective attitudes toward NIV among patients experiencing NIV for the first time in the context of an ICU stay. Methods Semi-structured interviews were conducted in 10 patients during their ICU stay and soon after their first NIV experience. None of the patients had ever received NIV previously. Evaluative assertion analysis and thematic analysis were used to investigate cognitive and affective attitudes toward NIV before, during, and after the first NIV experience, as well as patient attitudes toward caregivers and relatives. Results Before their first NIV session, the cognitive attitudes of the patients were generally positive. They became less so and more ambiguous during and after NIV, as the patients discovered the actual barriers associated with NIV. Affective attitudes during NIV were more negative than affective attitudes before and after NIV, with reports of dyspnea, anxiety, fear, claustrophobic feelings, and reactivation of past traumatic experiences. The patients had more positive attitudes toward the presence of a caregiver during NIV, compared to the presence of a family member. Conclusion This study corroborates the possibly negative—or even traumatic—nature of the NIV experience, with emphasis on the role of affective attitudes. This is a rationale for evaluating the impact of NIV-targeted psychological interventions in ICU patients with acute respiratory failure.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marina Iosifyan
- UMRS1158 Neurophysiologie Respiratoire Expérimentale et Clinique, INSERM, Sorbonne Université, 75005, Paris, France.,Service de Pneumologie, Médecine Intensive et Réanimation, Département R3S, AP-HP, Groupe Hospitalier Pitié-Salpêtrière Charles Foix, 47-83 boulevard de l'Hôpital, 75013, Paris, France.,Department of Psychology, National Research University Higher School of Economics, Moscow, Russia
| | - Matthieu Schmidt
- Service de Réanimation Médicale de l'Institut de Cardiologie, AP-HP, Groupe Hospitalier Pitié-Salpêtrière Charles Foix, 75013, Paris, France
| | - Amélie Hurbault
- Service de Pneumologie, Médecine Intensive et Réanimation, Département R3S, AP-HP, Groupe Hospitalier Pitié-Salpêtrière Charles Foix, 47-83 boulevard de l'Hôpital, 75013, Paris, France
| | - Julien Mayaux
- Service de Pneumologie, Médecine Intensive et Réanimation, Département R3S, AP-HP, Groupe Hospitalier Pitié-Salpêtrière Charles Foix, 47-83 boulevard de l'Hôpital, 75013, Paris, France
| | - Christian Delafosse
- Service de Pneumologie-Explorations du Sommeil, Hôpital Simone Veil, 95600, Eaubonne, France
| | - Marina Mishenko
- Laboratoire Psychopathologie et Processus de Santé, EA 4057, Université Paris Descartes, 75005, Paris, France.,Laboratoire de psychologie du développement et de l'éducation de l'enfant, UMR 8240, CNRS, Université Paris Descartes & Université Caen Basse-Normandie, 75005, Paris, France
| | - Nathalie Nion
- Service de Pneumologie, Médecine Intensive et Réanimation, Département R3S, AP-HP, Groupe Hospitalier Pitié-Salpêtrière Charles Foix, 47-83 boulevard de l'Hôpital, 75013, Paris, France
| | - Alexandre Demoule
- UMRS1158 Neurophysiologie Respiratoire Expérimentale et Clinique, INSERM, Sorbonne Université, 75005, Paris, France.,Service de Pneumologie, Médecine Intensive et Réanimation, Département R3S, AP-HP, Groupe Hospitalier Pitié-Salpêtrière Charles Foix, 47-83 boulevard de l'Hôpital, 75013, Paris, France
| | - Thomas Similowski
- UMRS1158 Neurophysiologie Respiratoire Expérimentale et Clinique, INSERM, Sorbonne Université, 75005, Paris, France. .,Service de Pneumologie, Médecine Intensive et Réanimation, Département R3S, AP-HP, Groupe Hospitalier Pitié-Salpêtrière Charles Foix, 47-83 boulevard de l'Hôpital, 75013, Paris, France.
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Ferrer RA, Ellis EM. Moving beyond categorization to understand affective influences on real world health decisions. SOCIAL AND PERSONALITY PSYCHOLOGY COMPASS 2019; 13. [PMID: 33912229 DOI: 10.1111/spc3.12502] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/11/2022]
Abstract
This paper provides an overview of affect and health decision-making research, with a focus on identifying gaps, opportunities, and challenges to guide future research. We begin by defining common categorical distinctions of affective processes that influence health decisions: integral (i.e., related to the decision) and incidental (i.e., normatively unrelated to the decision) influences, and current (experienced in the moment) and anticipated ("cognitive representations" of future affect) affect. We then summarize key discoveries within the most common categories of affective influences on health decision making: current integral affect, current incidental affect, and anticipated integral affect. Finally, we highlight research gaps, challenges, and opportunities for future directions for research aimed at translating affective and decision science theory to improve our understanding of, and ability to intervene upon, health decision making.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rebecca A Ferrer
- Basic Biobehavioral and Psychological Sciences Branch, National Cancer Institute
| | - Erin M Ellis
- Office of Disease Prevention, National Institutes of Health
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Murray AB, Boardley D, Wasshenova EV, Mahas R, Kiviniemi MT, Tipton J, Geers AL. Affective determinants of physical activity in cancer survivors. Psychol Health 2019; 35:593-612. [PMID: 31657226 DOI: 10.1080/08870446.2019.1658761] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/13/2022]
Abstract
Objective: Research with non-cancer survivor populations finds affective variables to be important determinants of physical activity. This study assessed the ability of explicit and implicit affective variables to predict the physical activity of cancer survivors, above that accounted for by cognitive variables. The study also tested whether the affective variables were connected to physical activity directly or indirectly through their association with behavioural intentions. Design: In a cross-sectional design, cancer survivors (n = 122) completed questionnaires and an implicit affect task. Main outcome measures: Self-report measures assessed three affective variables (e.g. positive affective associations), five cognitive variables (e.g. cognitive beliefs), physical activity estimates, behavioural intentions and participants also completed an implicit affect task regarding physical activity. Results: Two of the three explicit affect variables and the implicit affect variable accounted for significant variability in physical activity estimates beyond that accounted for by the cognitive variables. Positive affective associations were the strongest predictor in multivariate analyses. Behavioural intentions did not mediate the link between the affect variables and physical activity estimates. Conclusions: Explicit and implicit affective variables are direct and unique predictors of physical activity in cancer survivors. Physical activity interventions for cancer survivors should target both explicit affect and implicit affect.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ashley B Murray
- Department of Psychology, University of Toledo, Toledo, OH, USA
| | - Debra Boardley
- School of Population Health, College of Health and Human Services, University of Toledo, Toledo, OH, USA
| | - Emily Van Wasshenova
- School of Population Health, College of Health and Human Services, University of Toledo, Toledo, OH, USA
| | - Rachel Mahas
- College of Nursing and health, Madonna University, Livonia, MI, USA
| | - Marc T Kiviniemi
- Department of Health, Behavior and Society, University of Kentucky, Lexington, KY, USA
| | - Janelle Tipton
- Eleanor N. Dana Cancer Center, University of Toledo Medical Center, Toledo, OH, USA
| | - Andrew L Geers
- Department of Psychology, University of Toledo, Toledo, OH, USA
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Stevens CJ, Gillman AS, Gardiner CK, Montanaro EA, Bryan AD, Conner M. Feel good now or regret it later? The respective roles of affective attitudes and anticipated affective reactions for explaining health-promoting and health risk behavioral intentions. JOURNAL OF APPLIED SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 2019; 49:331-348. [PMID: 31511748 DOI: 10.1111/jasp.12584] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/21/2022]
Abstract
Evidence supporting the incorporation of affective constructs, such as affective attitudes and anticipated regret, into theoretical models of health behavior has been mounting in recent years; however, the role of positive anticipated affective reactions (e.g., pride) has been largely unexplored. The purpose of the present investigation was to assess how affective attitudes and anticipated affective reactions (both pride and regret for performing a behavior or not) may provide distinct utility for understanding intentions to perform health-promoting and health risk behaviors over and above cognitive attitudes and other established theoretical constructs from the theory of planned behavior (TPB). Participants (N = 210) were recruited via Amazon's Mechanical Turk to complete a one-time online battery assessing TPB and affective constructs. Self-reported intentions served as the main outcome measure, and hierarchical linear modeling was used to examine the effects of TPB and affective constructs across behaviors. Controlling for TPB constructs, more positive affective attitudes and greater anticipated regret, but not anticipated pride, predicted intentions to engage in future health behaviors. Anticipated affective reactions contributed explanatory variance for intentions to perform health risk behaviors, but anticipated pride and regret were not associated with intentions to perform health risk behaviors. Contributions made via the inclusion of both positively and negatively valence anticipated affective reactions for both action and inaction (performing a behavior or not) across a range of health promoting and health risk behaviors are discussed, as well as implications for future intervention work.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Courtney J Stevens
- Department of Psychology & Neuroscience, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, Colorado
| | - Arielle S Gillman
- Department of Psychology & Neuroscience, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, Colorado
| | - Casey K Gardiner
- Department of Psychology & Neuroscience, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, Colorado
| | - Erika A Montanaro
- Department of Psychology & Neuroscience, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, Colorado
| | - Angela D Bryan
- Department of Psychology & Neuroscience, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, Colorado
| | - Mark Conner
- School of Psychology, University of Leeds, Leeds, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Ellis EM, Elwyn G, Nelson WL, Scalia P, Kobrin SC, Ferrer RA. Interventions to Engage Affective Forecasting in Health-Related Decision Making: A Meta-Analysis. Ann Behav Med 2018; 52:157-174. [PMID: 29538630 PMCID: PMC7189982 DOI: 10.1093/abm/kax024] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/14/2022] Open
Abstract
Background People often use affective forecasts, or predictions about how a decision will make them feel, to guide medical and health decision making. However, these forecasts are susceptible to biases and inaccuracies that can have consequential effects on decision making and health. Purpose A meta-analysis was performed to determine the effectiveness of intervening to address affective forecasting as a means of helping patients make better health-related choices. Methods We included between-subjects experimental and intervention studies that targeted variables related to affective forecasting (e.g., anticipated regret, anticipated affect) as a means of changing health behaviors or decisions. We determined the overall effect of these interventions on targeted affective constructs and behavioral outcomes, and whether conceptual and methodological factors moderated these effects. Results A total of 133 independent effect sizes were identified from 37 publications (N = 72,020). Overall, affective forecasting interventions changed anticipated regret, d = 0.24, 95% confidence interval (CI) (0.15, 0.32), p < .001, behavior, d = 0.29, 95% CI (0.13, 0.45), p < .001, and behavioral intentions, d = 0.19, 95% CI (0.11, 0.28), p < .001, all measured immediately postintervention. Interventions did not change anticipated positive and negative affect, and effects on intentions and regret did not extend to follow-up time points, ps > .05. Generally, effects were not moderated by conceptual model, intervention intensity, or behavioral context. Conclusions Affective forecasting interventions had a small consistent effect on behavioral outcomes regardless of intervention intensity and conceptual framework, suggesting such constructs are promising intervention targets across several health domains.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Erin M Ellis
- Basic Biobehavioral and Psychological Sciences Branch, Behavioral Research Program, National Cancer Institute, Rockville, MD, USA
| | - Glyn Elwyn
- The Dartmouth Institute for Health Policy and Clinical Practice, Dartmouth College, Lebanon, NH, USA
| | - Wendy L Nelson
- Basic Biobehavioral and Psychological Sciences Branch, Behavioral Research Program, National Cancer Institute, Rockville, MD, USA
| | - Peter Scalia
- The Dartmouth Institute for Health Policy and Clinical Practice, Dartmouth College, Lebanon, NH, USA
| | - Sarah C Kobrin
- Basic Biobehavioral and Psychological Sciences Branch, Behavioral Research Program, National Cancer Institute, Rockville, MD, USA
| | - Rebecca A Ferrer
- Basic Biobehavioral and Psychological Sciences Branch, Behavioral Research Program, National Cancer Institute, Rockville, MD, USA
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Ellis EM, Rajagopal R, Kiviniemi MT. The interplay between feelings and beliefs about condoms as predictors of their use. Psychol Health 2018; 33:176-192. [PMID: 28448163 PMCID: PMC6148362 DOI: 10.1080/08870446.2017.1320797] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE Negative feelings about condoms are a key barrier to their use. Using the behavioural affective associations model, we examined the joint effects of affective associations and cognitive beliefs about condoms on condom use. DESIGN In Study 1 (N = 97), students completed measures of their affective associations and cognitive beliefs about sex and condoms, sexual activity and condom use. In Study 2 (N = 171), a measure of behavioural intentions and condom selection task were added. MAIN OUTCOME MEASURES Condom use measured in Study 1 as (1) current condom use, and (2) willingness to use condoms; in Study 2 as: (1) behavioural intentions, (2) number of condoms selected. RESULTS Affective associations with sex and condoms were behaviour-specific, were directly associated with the respective behaviour, and mediated the relations of cognitive beliefs to behaviour, ps < .05. In Study 2, affective associations were associated with behavioural intentions and the number of condoms selected, ps < .05; cognitive beliefs were indirectly associated with these outcomes through affective associations, indirect effects: ps < .05. CONCLUSIONS Affective associations are a behaviour-specific and proximal predictor of condom use, mediating the effect of cognitive beliefs, suggesting they may be a particularly viable intervention target.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Erin M. Ellis
- Department of Community Health and Health Behavior, University at Buffalo, SUNY
| | | | - Marc T. Kiviniemi
- Department of Community Health and Health Behavior, University at Buffalo, SUNY
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Van Cappellen P, Rice EL, Catalino LI, Fredrickson BL. Positive affective processes underlie positive health behaviour change. Psychol Health 2018; 33:77-97. [PMID: 28498722 PMCID: PMC5682236 DOI: 10.1080/08870446.2017.1320798] [Citation(s) in RCA: 134] [Impact Index Per Article: 19.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/26/2022]
Abstract
Positive health behaviours such as physical activity can prevent or reverse many chronic conditions, yet a majority of people fall short of leading a healthy lifestyle. Recent discoveries in affective science point to promising approaches to circumvent barriers to lifestyle change. Here, we present a new theoretical framework that integrates scientific knowledge about positive affect with that on implicit processes. The upward spiral theory of lifestyle change explains how positive affect can facilitate long-term adherence to positive health behaviours. The inner loop of this spiral model identifies nonconscious motives as a central mechanism of behavioural maintenance. Positive affect experienced during health behaviours increases incentive salience for cues associated with those behaviours, which in turn, implicitly guides attention and the everyday decisions to repeat those behaviours. The outer loop represents the evidence-backed claim, based on Fredrickson's broaden-and-build theory, that positive affect builds a suite of endogenous resources, which may in turn amplify the positive affect experienced during positive health behaviours and strengthen the nonconscious motives. We offer published and preliminary evidence in favour of the theory, contrast it to other dominant theories of health behaviour change, and highlight attendant implications for interventions that merit testing.
Collapse
|
18
|
Magnan RE, Shorey Fennell BR, Brady JM. Health decision making and behavior: The role of affect-laden constructs. SOCIAL AND PERSONALITY PSYCHOLOGY COMPASS 2017. [DOI: 10.1111/spc3.12333] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
|
19
|
Kiviniemi MT, Ellis EM, Hall MG, Moss JL, Lillie SE, Brewer NT, Klein WM. Mediation, moderation, and context: Understanding complex relations among cognition, affect, and health behaviour. Psychol Health 2017; 33:98-116. [DOI: 10.1080/08870446.2017.1324973] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/31/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Marc T. Kiviniemi
- Department of Community Health and Health Behavior, University at Buffalo, SUNY, Buffalo, NY, USA
| | - Erin M. Ellis
- Behavioral Research Program, National Cancer Institute, Rockville, MD, USA
| | - Marissa G. Hall
- Department of Health Behavior, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, NC, USA
| | - Jennifer L. Moss
- Behavioral Research Program, National Cancer Institute, Rockville, MD, USA
| | - Sarah E. Lillie
- Minneapolis Veteran Affairs Health Care System, Minneapolis, MN, USA
| | - Noel T. Brewer
- Department of Health Behavior, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, NC, USA
| | - William M.P. Klein
- Behavioral Research Program, National Cancer Institute, Rockville, MD, USA
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Hu J, Zhen S, Yu C, Zhang Q, Zhang W. Sensation Seeking and Online Gaming Addiction in Adolescents: A Moderated Mediation Model of Positive Affective Associations and Impulsivity. Front Psychol 2017; 8:699. [PMID: 28529494 PMCID: PMC5418345 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00699] [Citation(s) in RCA: 46] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/01/2016] [Accepted: 04/21/2017] [Indexed: 12/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Based on the Dual Systems Model (Somerville et al., 2010; Steinberg, 2010a) and the biosocial-affect model (Romer and Hennessy, 2007) of adolescent sensation seeking and problem behaviors, the present study examined how (affective associations with online games as a mediator) and when (impulsivity as a moderator) did sensation seeking influence online gaming addiction in adolescence. A total of 375 Chinese male adolescents (mean age = 16.02 years, SD = 0.85) from southern China completed anonymous questionnaires regarding sensation seeking, positive affective associations with online games, impulsivity, and online gaming addiction. Our findings revealed that sensation seeking, positive affective associations with online games and impulsivity were each significantly and positively associated with online gaming addiction in adolescents. Positive affective associations mediated the relationship between sensation seeking and online gaming addiction. Further, impulsivity moderated the relationship between positive affective associations and online gaming addiction, such that the association between positive affective association and online gaming addiction was stronger for high than for low impulsivity adolescents. These findings underscore the importance of integrating the biosocial-affect model and the Dual Systems Model to understand how and when sensation seeking impacts adolescent online gaming addiction.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jianping Hu
- Laboratory for Behavioral and Regional Finance, Guangdong University of FinanceGuangzhou, China
| | - Shuangju Zhen
- School of Psychology and Center for Studies of Psychological Application, South China Normal UniversityGuangzhou, China
| | - Chengfu Yu
- School of Education and Center for Mind and Brain Science, Guangzhou UniversityGuangzhou, China
| | | | - Wei Zhang
- School of Psychology and Center for Studies of Psychological Application, South China Normal UniversityGuangzhou, China
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Kiviniemi MT. Structure and content of affective associations with health behaviours: Is the behaviour 'good OR bad' or 'good AND bad'. Psychol Health 2017; 33:159-175. [PMID: 28391707 DOI: 10.1080/08870446.2017.1314476] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/22/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE The feelings and emotions individuals associate with health-related behaviours influence engagement in those behaviours. However, the structure and the content of these affective associations have not been examined. The studies reported here examined competing hypotheses about the structure (unidimensional or bidimensional) and content (generalised affect or specific emotions) of affective associations with two health-related behaviours: physical activity and fruit/vegetable consumption. DESIGN For each behaviour, participants (fruit and vegetable consumption n = 149; physical activity n = 199) completed an assessment of the association of 40 positive and 51 negative affect concepts with the behaviour. MAIN OUTCOME MEASURES Ratings of affective associations with each behaviour. RESULTS Confirmatory factor analyses comparing unidimensional and bidimensional affect structure models showed that the structure of individuals' affective associations was bidimensional for both behaviours - positive and negative affective associations were shown to be separate and distinct constructs. Exploratory factor analyses supported a model of affective associations as generalised affect for both behaviours. CONCLUSION Affective associations with both physical activity and with fruit/vegetable consumption consist of separate positive and negative dimensions of generalised affect. These findings lead to recommendations for research and intervention development based on the implications for how affective associations might operate to influence behavioural decision-making.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marc T Kiviniemi
- a Department of Community Health and Health Behavior , University at Buffalo , Buffalo , NY , USA
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Ruhl H, Holub SC, Dolan EA. The reasoned/reactive model: A new approach to examining eating decisions among female college dieters and nondieters. Eat Behav 2016; 23:33-40. [PMID: 27448513 DOI: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2016.07.011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/30/2016] [Revised: 06/27/2016] [Accepted: 07/13/2016] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
Female college students are prone to unhealthy eating patterns that can impact long-term health. This study examined female students' healthy and unhealthy eating behaviors with three decision-making models. Specifically, the theory of reasoned action, prototype/willingness model, and new reasoned/reactive model were compared to determine how reasoned (logical) and reactive (impulsive) factors relate to dietary decisions. Females (N=583, Mage=20.89years) completed measures on reasoned cognitions about foods (attitudes, subjective norms, nutrition knowledge, intentions to eat foods), reactive cognitions about foods (prototypes, affect, willingness to eat foods), dieting, and food consumption. Structural equation modeling (SEM) revealed the new reasoned/reactive model to be the preeminent model for examining eating behaviors. This model showed that attitudes were related to intentions and willingness to eat healthy and unhealthy foods. Affect was related to willingness to eat healthy and unhealthy foods, whereas nutrition knowledge was related to intentions and willingness to eat healthy foods only. Intentions and willingness were related to healthy and unhealthy food consumption. Dieting status played a moderating role in the model and revealed mean-level differences between dieters and nondieters. This study highlights the importance of specific factors in relation to female students' eating decisions and unveils a comprehensive model for examining health behaviors.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Holly Ruhl
- The University of Texas at Dallas, 800 W. Campbell Rd., GR 41, Richardson, TX 75080, USA.
| | - Shayla C Holub
- The University of Texas at Dallas, 800 W. Campbell Rd., GR 41, Richardson, TX 75080, USA
| | - Elaine A Dolan
- The University of Texas at Dallas, 800 W. Campbell Rd., GR 41, Richardson, TX 75080, USA
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Kiviniemi MT, Brown-Kramer CR. Planning versus action: Different decision-making processes predict plans to change one's diet versus actual dietary behavior. J Health Psychol 2015; 20:556-68. [PMID: 25903243 DOI: 10.1177/1359105315576605] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022] Open
Abstract
Most health decision-making models posit that deciding to engage in a health behavior involves forming a behavioral intention which then leads to actual behavior. However, behavioral intentions and actual behavior may not be functionally equivalent. Two studies examined whether decision-making factors predicting dietary behaviors were the same as or distinct from those predicting intentions. Actual dietary behavior was proximally predicted by affective associations with the behavior. By contrast, behavioral intentions were predicted by cognitive beliefs about behaviors, with no contribution of affective associations. This dissociation has implications for understanding individual regulation of health behaviors and for behavior change interventions.
Collapse
|
24
|
Brown-Kramer CR, Kiviniemi MT. Affective associations and cognitive beliefs relate to individuals’ decisions to perform testicular or breast self-exams. J Behav Med 2015; 38:664-72. [DOI: 10.1007/s10865-015-9641-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/07/2014] [Accepted: 03/30/2015] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
|
25
|
Disgusted, embarrassed, annoyed: affective associations relate to uptake of colonoscopy screening. Ann Behav Med 2015; 48:112-9. [PMID: 24500079 DOI: 10.1007/s12160-013-9580-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 36] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/24/2022] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND Uptake of colorectal cancer screening is lower than desired. Screening decision making research has traditionally focused on benefits and barriers to screening. This study examines the relation of affective associations with screening (feelings and emotions associated with screening) to colonoscopy screening uptake. METHODS Participants were 103 African American community adults. Participants completed a structured interview assessing perceived benefits of and barriers to colonoscopy screening, affective associations with colonoscopy, colonoscopy screening behavior, and intentions for future screening. RESULTS Higher positive and lower negative affective associations with screening were both significant predictors of colonoscopy uptake. Affective associations fully mediated the relation of perceived benefits and barriers to screening uptake. Affective associations were associated with intentions for future screening. CONCLUSIONS Incorporation of affective associations into models of screening decision making and intervention approaches to address screening compliance has utility for advancing our understanding of screening adherence as well as increasing screening rates.
Collapse
|
26
|
O'Neill SC, Tercyak KP, Baytop C, Alford SH, McBride CM. A new approach to assessing affect and the emotional implications of personal genomic testing for common disease risk. Public Health Genomics 2015; 18:104-12. [PMID: 25612474 PMCID: PMC4348333 DOI: 10.1159/000370101] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/24/2014] [Accepted: 11/25/2014] [Indexed: 01/20/2023] Open
Abstract
AIMS Personal genomic testing (PGT) for common disease risk is becoming increasingly frequent, but little is known about people's array of emotional reactions to learning their genomic risk profiles and the psychological harms/benefits of PGT. We conducted a study of post-PGT affect, including positive, neutral, and negative states that may arise after testing. METHODS A total of 228 healthy adults received PGT for common disease variants and completed a semistructured research interview within 2 weeks of disclosure. The study participants reported how the PGT results made them feel in their own words. Using an iterative coding process, the responses were organized into three broad affective categories: negative, neutral, and positive affect. RESULTS Neutral affect was the most prevalent response (53.9%), followed by positive affect (26.9%) and negative affect (19.2%). We found no differences by gender, race, or education. CONCLUSIONS While <20% of participants reported negative affect in response to learning their genomic risk profile for common diseases, a majority experienced either neutral or positive emotions. These findings contribute to the growing evidence that PGT does not impose significant psychological harms. Moreover, they point to a need to better link theories and assessments in both emotional and cognitive processing to capitalize on PGT information for healthy behavior change.
Collapse
|
27
|
Abstract
In the past two to three decades health behavior scientists have increasingly emphasized affect-related concepts (including, but not limited to emotion) in their attempts to understand and facilitate change in important health behaviors, such as smoking, eating, physical activity, substance abuse, and sex. This article provides a narrative review of this burgeoning literature, including relevant theory and research on affective response (e.g., hedonic response to eating and drug use), incidental affect (e.g., work-related stress as a determinant of alcohol use), affect processing (e.g., anticipated regret for illicit sex or skipping an exercise session), and affectively charged motivation (e.g., cigarette craving). An integrative dual-processing framework is presented that suggests pathways through which affect-related concepts may interrelate to influence health behavior.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- David M. Williams
- Department of Behavioral and Social Sciences, Brown University School of Public Health, USA
| | - Daniel R. Evans
- Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, Alpert Medical School of Brown University, USA
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
Implicit affective associations predict snack choice for those with low, but not high levels of eating disorder symptomatology. Appetite 2014; 77:122-30. [PMID: 24630936 DOI: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.03.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/07/2013] [Revised: 01/30/2014] [Accepted: 03/03/2014] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
The central purpose of the current study was to examine whether eating disorder symptomatology moderates the extent to which implicitly and explicitly measured affective associations with fruits, or the feelings individuals associate with them, predict food choice. Participants (N = 107) completed both implicit and explicit measures of affective associations with fruits, a self-report of eating disorder symptomatology. In a subsequent snack selection task, they selected either a granola bar or fruit as a measure of their food choice behavior. Logistic regression analyses revealed that eating disorder symptomatology moderated the relation of implicit affective associations on behavior, OR = 0.27, p = .024, 95% CI (0.085, 0.84). A test of the simple effects indicated that implicit affective associations predicted snack choice at or below mean symptomatology levels [OR = 2.073, p = .02, 95% CI (1.12, 3.84)], but not at high levels [OR = 0.56, p = .28, 95% CI (0.18, 1.74)]. The effects of explicit measures on eating behavior were not moderated by eating disorder symptomatology (ps > .05). These findings advance our understanding of both the relation of implicit affective associations on behavior, as well as the ways in which eating disorder symptomatology may impact the decision-making process, thereby perpetuating disorder-related eating behavior. As such, they have implications for both health decision-making theory, as well as for the study and treatment of eating disorders.
Collapse
|
29
|
Kiviniemi MT, Ellis EM. Worry about skin cancer mediates the relation of perceived cancer risk and sunscreen use. J Behav Med 2013; 37:1069-74. [PMID: 24072431 DOI: 10.1007/s10865-013-9538-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/12/2013] [Accepted: 09/04/2013] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
Abstract
Preventive health behaviors are believed to be motivated in part by a person's perception of risk for a particular health problem. Risk contains a cognitive component, beliefs about the chances of a health problem occurring, and an affective component, fear or worry about the health problem. Although both have been shown to influence behavior, the nature of their interrelation as an influence on behavior has not been examined. Data from the 2005 Health Information National Trends Survey, a US nationally-representative telephone survey was analyzed. Participants reported perceived absolute and comparative risk for skin cancer, feelings of worry about skin cancer, and sunscreen use behavior. Analyses examined main effects models for the relation between perceived risk, worry, and sunscreen use, as well as both moderated and mediated models. For both absolute and comparative risk, the relation between cognitively-based perceived risk for skin cancer and sunscreen use was fully mediated by feelings of worry, as evidenced by significant direct effects of worry (bs > 0.046, ps < 0.01) and indirect effects of risk through worry (bs > 0.19, ps < 0.01). When worry was included in the models, direct effects of risk perceptions were non-significant (bs < 0.11, ps < 0.10). No evidence was found for moderated effects of worry on the relation between risk and behavior. While cognitive risk appraisals do influence decision making and may be addressed by interventions, these findings demonstrate that affectively-based risk components play a key role in behavior regulation. Affectively-based risk might be an effective target for interventions and should be incorporated more fully in decision-making models.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marc T Kiviniemi
- Department of Community Health and Health Behavior, University at Buffalo, SUNY, 314 Kimball Tower, 3435 Main Street, Buffalo, NY, 14214, USA,
| | | |
Collapse
|
30
|
Walsh EM, Kiviniemi MT. Changing how I feel about the food: experimentally manipulated affective associations with fruits change fruit choice behaviors. J Behav Med 2013; 37:322-31. [PMID: 23299831 DOI: 10.1007/s10865-012-9490-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 54] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/10/2012] [Accepted: 12/31/2012] [Indexed: 01/12/2023]
Abstract
Fewer than half of Americans meet current recommendations for fruit and vegetable intake. The behavioral affective associations model posits that feelings and emotions associated with a behavior are a proximal influence on decision making. Cross-sectional evidence supports the model and suggests that affective associations predict fruit and vegetable consumption. The purpose of this study was to test whether a causal relation exists between affective associations about fruits and future fruit consumption behavior, as measured by a snack selection task. Following a baseline assessment of cognitive and affective variables, participants' (N = 161) affective associations about fruits were experimentally manipulated with an implicit priming paradigm. Images of fruits were repeatedly paired with positive, negative, or neutral affective stimuli. The key outcome measure was a behavioral choice task in which participants chose between fruit and a granola bar. Participants in the positive prime condition were three times more likely than those in the negative condition to select a piece of fruit over the granola bar alternative in the snack selection task. They were also twice as likely as those in the neutral condition to select fruit. There were no changes in self-reported affective associations or cognitive beliefs. These findings provide further evidence of the implicit and direct influence of affective associations on behavior, suggesting the need to both incorporate the role of affect in health decision making models, as well as the potential utility of intervention strategies targeting affective associations with health-related behaviors.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Erin M Walsh
- Department of Community Health and Health Behavior, University at Buffalo, SUNY, 329 Kimball Tower, 3435 Main Street, Buffalo, NY, 14214, USA,
| | | |
Collapse
|
31
|
The impact of emotional health on fruit and vegetable consumption in young men: A qualitative study. Proc Nutr Soc 2013. [DOI: 10.1017/s0029665113002310] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
|
32
|
Ferrer RA, Bergman HE, Klein WMP. Worry as a Predictor of Nutrition Behaviors. HEALTH EDUCATION & BEHAVIOR 2012; 40:88-96. [DOI: 10.1177/1090198112439410] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
Worry has been shown to predict a variety of health behaviors, such as cancer screening, yet there are few studies linking worry and nutrition. This study used nationally representative data from National Cancer Institute’s Food Attitudes and Behavior Survey ( n = 3,397) to examine the association between health-related worry and a variety of nutrition behaviors. Greater worry was associated with higher fruit and vegetable consumption ( B = 0.19, p < .01), but also more meals eaten when watching television ( B = 0.34, p < .01) and fewer with family ( B = −0.13, p = .02). Importantly, and counterintuitively, greater worry appeared to reverse the conventional relationship between self-efficacy and dietary restriction; those who were self-efficacious and worried were less likely to restrict unhealthy foods. Similarly, worry attenuated the relationship between perceived benefits and special effort to buy produce. A complex relationship between worry and nutrition emerged, with potentially important clinical implications.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rebecca A. Ferrer
- Behavioral Research Program, Division of Cancer Control and Population Sciences, National Cancer Institute, Bethesda, MD, USA
| | - Hannah E. Bergman
- Behavioral Research Program, Division of Cancer Control and Population Sciences, National Cancer Institute, Bethesda, MD, USA
| | - William M. P. Klein
- Behavioral Research Program, Division of Cancer Control and Population Sciences, National Cancer Institute, Bethesda, MD, USA
| |
Collapse
|
33
|
Kranz S, Marshall YW, Wight A, Bordi PL, Kris-Etherton PM. Liking and consumption of high-fiber snacks in preschool-age children. Food Qual Prefer 2011. [DOI: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2011.02.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
|
34
|
Kiviniemi MT, Orom H, Giovino GA. Race/ethnicity, psychological distress, and fruit/vegetable consumption. The nature of the distress-behavior relation differs by race/ethnicity. Appetite 2011; 56:737-40. [PMID: 21349306 PMCID: PMC3100390 DOI: 10.1016/j.appet.2011.02.012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/10/2010] [Revised: 02/11/2011] [Accepted: 02/15/2011] [Indexed: 12/22/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE We explored how the relation between psychological distress and fruit/vegetable consumption differed as a function of race/ethnicity. METHOD Data from the 2007 Health Information National Trends Survey was analyzed. Participants reported current psychological distress, race/ethnicity, and current fruit and vegetable consumption. Linear regression analyses were used to examine the association between race/ethnicity, distress, and their interaction and fruit and vegetable consumption. RESULTS There was a significant interaction between race/ethnicity and psychological distress in predicting fruit and vegetable consumption. Follow-up analyses indicated that distress was related to fruit and vegetable consumption for White and Hispanic but not for African American respondents. CONCLUSION The association between psychological distress and fruit/vegetable consumption differs as a function of race/ethnicity. The findings have implications for understanding the role of distress in eating behavior regulation and for developing interventions to address fruit/vegetable consumption targeted to members of different race/ethnic groups.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marc T Kiviniemi
- Department of Community Health and Health Behavior, School of Public Health and Health Professions, University at Buffalo, 314 Kimball Tower, 3435 Main Street, Buffalo, NY 14214, USA.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
35
|
Kiviniemi MT, Hay JL, James AS, Lipkus IM, Meissner HI, Stefanek M, Studts JL, Bridges JFP, Close DR, Erwin DO, Jones RM, Kaiser K, Kash KM, Kelly KM, Craddock Lee SJ, Purnell JQ, Siminoff LA, Vadaparampil ST, Wang C. Decision making about cancer screening: an assessment of the state of the science and a suggested research agenda from the ASPO Behavioral Oncology and Cancer Communication Special Interest Group. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev 2009; 18:3133-7. [PMID: 19900944 PMCID: PMC2810193 DOI: 10.1158/1055-9965.epi-18-11-aspo] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/14/2022] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Marc T Kiviniemi
- Department of Health Behavior, University at Buffalo, Buffalo, New York 14222, USA.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|