1
|
Marais AL, Roche-Labarbe N. Predictive coding and attention in developmental cognitive neuroscience and perspectives for neurodevelopmental disorders. Dev Cogn Neurosci 2025; 72:101519. [PMID: 39864185 PMCID: PMC11795830 DOI: 10.1016/j.dcn.2025.101519] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/23/2024] [Revised: 01/17/2025] [Accepted: 01/18/2025] [Indexed: 01/28/2025] Open
Abstract
Sensory prediction and repetition suppression are closely related cognitive mechanisms that allow the brain to form predictions about the environment, and guide perception in synergy with attention. Predictive coding is a theory of the fundamental role of predictive mechanisms in brain functions. Authors have proposed a central role of predictive impairments in autism and possibly other neurodevelopmental disorders. However, little is known about predictive mechanisms in typical development, and how they co-develop with attention. Here we review experimental support for predictive coding and its links with attention in healthy adults' brains, the first experimental works performed in typically developing children and infants, and theoretical accounts of neurodevelopmental disorders using a predictive coding framework. We propose future directions for predictive coding research in development. Finally, we describe the first predictive coding experiments in neonates and provide research perspectives for using this framework in searching for early markers of atypical neurodevelopment.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anne-Lise Marais
- Normandie Univ, UNICAEN, INSERM, COMETE, GIP CYCERON, Caen 14000, France
| | | |
Collapse
|
2
|
Kao C, Zhang Y. Age and Sex Differences in Infants' Neural Sensitivity to Emotional Prosodies in Spoken Words: A Multifeature Oddball Study. JOURNAL OF SPEECH, LANGUAGE, AND HEARING RESEARCH : JSLHR 2025; 68:332-348. [PMID: 39637290 DOI: 10.1044/2024_jslhr-23-00820] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/07/2024]
Abstract
PURPOSE This study aimed to investigate infants' neural responses to changes in emotional prosody in spoken words. The focus was on understanding developmental changes and potential sex differences, aspects that were not consistently observed in previous behavioral studies. METHOD A modified multifeature oddball paradigm was used with emotional deviants (angry, happy, and sad) presented against neutral prosody (standard) within varying spoken words during a single electroencephalography recording session. The reported data included 34 infants (18 males, 16 females; age range: 3-12 months, average age: 7 months 26 days). RESULTS Infants exhibited distinct patterns of mismatch responses (MMRs) to different emotional prosodies in both early (100-200 ms) and late (300-500 ms) time windows following the speech onset. While both happy and angry prosodies elicited more negative early MMRs than the sad prosody across all infants, older infants showed more negative early MMRs than their younger counterparts. The distinction between early MMRs to angry and sad prosodies was more pronounced in younger infants. In the late time window, angry prosody elicited a more negative late MMR than the sad prosody, with younger infants showing more distinct late MMRs to sad and angry prosodies compared to older infants. Additionally, a sex effect was observed as male infants displayed more negative early MMRs compared to females. CONCLUSIONS These findings demonstrate the feasibility of the modified multifeature oddball protocol in studying neural sensitivities to emotional speech in infancy. The observed age and sex effects on infants' auditory neural responses to vocal emotions underscore the need for further research to distinguish between acoustic and emotional processing and to understand their roles in early socioemotional and language development. SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.27914553.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chieh Kao
- Department of Speech-Language-Hearing Sciences, University of Minnesota, Twin Cities
- Center for Cognitive Sciences, University of Minnesota, Twin Cities
| | - Yang Zhang
- Department of Speech-Language-Hearing Sciences, University of Minnesota, Twin Cities
- Masonic Institute for the Developing Brain, University of Minnesota, Twin Cities
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Männel C, Ramos-Sanchez J, Obrig H, Ahissar M, Schaadt G. Perceptual anchoring: Children with dyslexia benefit less than controls from contextual repetitions in speech processing. Clin Neurophysiol 2024; 166:117-128. [PMID: 39153460 DOI: 10.1016/j.clinph.2024.07.016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/10/2023] [Revised: 07/23/2024] [Accepted: 07/24/2024] [Indexed: 08/19/2024]
Abstract
OBJECTIVES Individuals with dyslexia perceive and utilize statistical features in the auditory input deficiently. The present study investigates whether affected children also benefit less from repeating context tones as perceptual anchors for subsequent speech processing. METHODS In an event-related potential study, eleven-year-old children with dyslexia (n = 21) and without dyslexia (n = 20) heard syllable pairs, with the first syllable either receiving a constant pitch (anchor) or variable pitch (no-anchor), while second syllables were identical across conditions. RESULTS Children with and without dyslexia showed smaller auditory P2 responses to constant-pitch versus variable-pitch first syllables, while only control children additionally showed smaller N1 and faster P1 responses. This suggests less automatic processing of anchor repetitions in dyslexia. For the second syllables, both groups showed faster P2 responses following anchor than no-anchor first syllables, but only controls additionally showed smaller P2 responses. CONCLUSIONS Children with and without dyslexia show differences in anchor effects. While both groups seem to allocate less attention to speech stimuli after contextual repetitions, children with dyslexia display less facilitation in speech processing from acoustic anchors. SIGNIFICANCE Altered anchoring in the linguistic domain may contribute to the difficulties of individuals with dyslexia in establishing long-term representations of speech.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Claudia Männel
- Department of Audiology and Phoniatrics, Charité - Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Augustenburger Platz 1, 13353 Berlin, Germany; Department of Neuropsychology, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Stephanstr. 1a, 04103 Leipzig, Germany; Clinic for Cognitive Neurology, University of Leipzig, Liebigstr. 16, 04103 Leipzig, Germany.
| | - Jessica Ramos-Sanchez
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition, and Behaviour, Thomas van Aquinostraat 4, 6525 GD Nijmegen, Netherlands
| | - Hellmuth Obrig
- Clinic for Cognitive Neurology, University of Leipzig, Liebigstr. 16, 04103 Leipzig, Germany; Department of Neurology, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Stephanstr. 1a, 04103 Leipzig, Germany
| | - Merav Ahissar
- ELSC Center for Brain Research, Hebrew University of Jerusalem
| | - Gesa Schaadt
- Department of Neuropsychology, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Stephanstr. 1a, 04103 Leipzig, Germany; Department of Education and Psychology, Freie Universität Berlin, Schwendenerstr. 33, 14195 Berlin, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Charlebois-Poirier AR, Lalancette E, Agbogba K, Fauteux AA, Knoth IS, Lippé S. Working memory and processing speed abilities are related to habituation and change detection in school-aged children: An ERP study. Neuropsychologia 2023; 187:108616. [PMID: 37339690 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108616] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/14/2022] [Revised: 04/24/2023] [Accepted: 06/08/2023] [Indexed: 06/22/2023]
Abstract
High cognitive performance is related to efficient brain processing while accomplishing complex cognitive tasks. This efficiency is observed through a rapid engagement of the brain regions and the cognitive processes required for task accomplishment. However, it is unclear if this efficiency is also present in basic sensory processes such as habituation and change detection. We recorded EEG with 85 healthy children (51 males) aged between 4 and 13 years old, while they listened to an auditory oddball paradigm. Cognitive functioning was evaluated using the Weschler Intelligence Scales for Children Fifth Edition and the Weschler Preschool & Primary School for Intelligence Fourth Edition. Auditory evoked potentials (AEPs) analyses and repeated measure analysis of covariance as well as regression models were performed. The analysis revealed that P1 and N1 repetition effects were observed across levels of cognitive functioning. Further, working memory abilities were related to repetition suppression on the auditory P2 component amplitude, while faster processing speed was related to repetition enhancement on the N2 component amplitude. Also, Late Discriminative Negativity (LDN) amplitude, a neural correlate of change detection, increased with working memory abilities. Our results confirm that efficient repetition suppression (i.e. greater reduction in amplitudes with greater levels of cognitive functioning) and more sensitive change detection (greater amplitude changes of the LDN) are related to the level of cognitive functioning in healthy children. More specifically, working memory and processing speed abilities are the cognitive domains related to efficient sensory habituation and change detection.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- A-R Charlebois-Poirier
- Research Center, Sainte-Justine Hospital, Université de Montréal, Montréal, QC, Canada; Department of Psychology, Université de Montréal Montréal, QC, Canada.
| | - E Lalancette
- Research Center, Sainte-Justine Hospital, Université de Montréal, Montréal, QC, Canada; Department of Psychology, Université de Montréal Montréal, QC, Canada
| | - K Agbogba
- Research Center, Sainte-Justine Hospital, Université de Montréal, Montréal, QC, Canada
| | - A-A Fauteux
- Research Center, Sainte-Justine Hospital, Université de Montréal, Montréal, QC, Canada; Department of Psychology, Université de Montréal Montréal, QC, Canada
| | - I S Knoth
- Research Center, Sainte-Justine Hospital, Université de Montréal, Montréal, QC, Canada
| | - S Lippé
- Research Center, Sainte-Justine Hospital, Université de Montréal, Montréal, QC, Canada; Department of Psychology, Université de Montréal Montréal, QC, Canada.
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Alzubaidi M, Agus M, Alyafei K, Althelaya KA, Shah U, Abd-Alrazaq AA, Anbar M, Makhlouf M, Househ M. Towards deep observation: A systematic survey on artificial intelligence techniques to monitor fetus via Ultrasound Images. iScience 2022; 25:104713. [PMID: 35856024 PMCID: PMC9287600 DOI: 10.1016/j.isci.2022.104713] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/30/2022] [Revised: 06/09/2022] [Accepted: 06/28/2022] [Indexed: 11/26/2022] Open
Abstract
Several reviews have been conducted regarding artificial intelligence (AI) techniques to improve pregnancy outcomes. But they are not focusing on ultrasound images. This survey aims to explore how AI can assist with fetal growth monitoring via ultrasound image. We reported our findings using the guidelines for PRISMA. We conducted a comprehensive search of eight bibliographic databases. Out of 1269 studies 107 are included. We found that 2D ultrasound images were more popular (88) than 3D and 4D ultrasound images (19). Classification is the most used method (42), followed by segmentation (31), classification integrated with segmentation (16) and other miscellaneous methods such as object-detection, regression, and reinforcement learning (18). The most common areas that gained traction within the pregnancy domain were the fetus head (43), fetus body (31), fetus heart (13), fetus abdomen (10), and the fetus face (10). This survey will promote the development of improved AI models for fetal clinical applications. Artificial intelligence studies to monitor fetal development via ultrasound images Fetal issues categorized based on four categories — general, head, heart, face, abdomen The most used AI techniques are classification, segmentation, object detection, and RL The research and practical implications are included.
Collapse
|
6
|
Sicard‐Cras I, Rioualen S, Pellae E, Misery L, Sizun J, Roué J. A review of the characteristics, mechanisms and clinical significance of habituation in foetuses and newborn infants. Acta Paediatr 2022; 111:245-258. [PMID: 34537978 DOI: 10.1111/apa.16115] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/08/2021] [Revised: 09/15/2021] [Accepted: 09/16/2021] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
Habituation has been a topic of interest since the early 20th century. We summarise the characteristics of habituation, the proposed habituation mechanisms, the associated cortical responses and the link between habituation and cognitive development. Behavioural and neuroimaging studies have highlighted the early sensory abilities of foetuses and newborn infants, with preterm newborn infants exhibiting decreased habituation and dishabituation capabilities that increase their environmental vulnerability. Habituation provides a foundation for the learning and cognition on which higher functions are constructed. It has been suggested that it is efficient for predicting cognitive developmental outcomes in term and preterm newborn infants.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Iona Sicard‐Cras
- Department of Neonatal Medicine University Hospital of Brest Brest France
- Laboratory LIEN University of Brest Brest France
| | - Stéphane Rioualen
- Department of Neonatal Medicine University Hospital of Brest Brest France
- Laboratory LIEN University of Brest Brest France
| | - Elisabeth Pellae
- Department of Neonatal Medicine University Hospital of Brest Brest France
- Laboratory LIEN University of Brest Brest France
| | | | - Jacques Sizun
- Department of Neonatal Medicine University Hospital of Brest Brest France
- Laboratory LIEN University of Brest Brest France
| | - Jean‐Michel Roué
- Department of Neonatal Medicine University Hospital of Brest Brest France
- Laboratory LIEN University of Brest Brest France
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Côté V, Lalancette È, Knoth IS, Côté L, Agbogba K, Vannasing P, Major P, Barlaam F, Michaud J, Lippé S. Distinct patterns of repetition suppression in Fragile X syndrome, down syndrome, tuberous sclerosis complex and mutations in SYNGAP1. Brain Res 2020; 1751:147205. [PMID: 33189692 DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2020.147205] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/30/2020] [Revised: 10/31/2020] [Accepted: 11/08/2020] [Indexed: 12/29/2022]
Abstract
Sensory processing is the gateway to information processing and more complex processes such as learning. Alterations in sensory processing is a common phenotype of many genetic syndromes associated with intellectual disability (ID). It is currently unknown whether sensory processing alterations converge or diverge on brain responses between syndromes. Here, we compare for the first time four genetic conditions with ID using the same basic sensory learning paradigm. One hundred and five participants, aged between 3 and 30 years old, composing four clinical ID groups and one control group, were recruited: Fragile X syndrome (FXS; n = 14), tuberous sclerosis complex (TSC; n = 9), Down syndrome (DS; n = 19), SYNGAP1 mutations (n = 8) and Neurotypical controls (NT; n = 55)). All groups included female and male participants. Brain responses were recorded using electroencephalography (EEG) during an audio-visual task that involved three repetitions of the pronunciation of the phoneme /a/. Event Related Potentials (ERP) were used to: 1) compare peak-to-peak amplitudes between groups, 2) evaluate the presence of repetition suppression within each group and 3) compare the relative repetition suppression between groups. Our results revealed larger overall amplitudes in FXS. A repetition suppression (RS) pattern was found in the NT group, FXS and DS, suggesting spared repetition suppression in a multimodal task in these two ID syndromes. Interestingly, FXS presented a stronger RS on one peak-to-peak value in comparison with the NT. The results of our study reveal the distinctiveness of ERP and RS brain responses in ID syndromes. Further studies should be conducted to understand the molecular mechanisms involved in these patterns of responses.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Valérie Côté
- Psychology Departement, Université de Montréal, Pavillon Marie-Victorin, 90, Avenue Vincent d'Indy, Montréal, QC H2V 2S9, Canada; NED Laboratory, Office 5.2.43, 3175 Chemin de la Côte-Sainte-Catherine, Montréal, QC H3T 1C5, Canada; Research Center UHC Sainte-Justine, 3175 Chemin de la Côte-Sainte-Catherine, Montréal, QC H3T 1C5, Canada.
| | - Ève Lalancette
- Psychology Departement, Université de Montréal, Pavillon Marie-Victorin, 90, Avenue Vincent d'Indy, Montréal, QC H2V 2S9, Canada; NED Laboratory, Office 5.2.43, 3175 Chemin de la Côte-Sainte-Catherine, Montréal, QC H3T 1C5, Canada; Research Center UHC Sainte-Justine, 3175 Chemin de la Côte-Sainte-Catherine, Montréal, QC H3T 1C5, Canada
| | - Inga S Knoth
- NED Laboratory, Office 5.2.43, 3175 Chemin de la Côte-Sainte-Catherine, Montréal, QC H3T 1C5, Canada; Research Center UHC Sainte-Justine, 3175 Chemin de la Côte-Sainte-Catherine, Montréal, QC H3T 1C5, Canada
| | - Lucie Côté
- Neurology Program, CHU Sainte-Justine, Montréal, 3175 Chemin de la Côte-Sainte-Catherine, QC H3T 1C5, Canada.
| | - Kristian Agbogba
- NED Laboratory, Office 5.2.43, 3175 Chemin de la Côte-Sainte-Catherine, Montréal, QC H3T 1C5, Canada; Research Center UHC Sainte-Justine, 3175 Chemin de la Côte-Sainte-Catherine, Montréal, QC H3T 1C5, Canada.
| | - Phetsamone Vannasing
- Research Center UHC Sainte-Justine, 3175 Chemin de la Côte-Sainte-Catherine, Montréal, QC H3T 1C5, Canada.
| | - Philippe Major
- Neurology Program, CHU Sainte-Justine, Montréal, 3175 Chemin de la Côte-Sainte-Catherine, QC H3T 1C5, Canada; Research Center UHC Sainte-Justine, 3175 Chemin de la Côte-Sainte-Catherine, Montréal, QC H3T 1C5, Canada.
| | - Fanny Barlaam
- NED Laboratory, Office 5.2.43, 3175 Chemin de la Côte-Sainte-Catherine, Montréal, QC H3T 1C5, Canada; Research Center UHC Sainte-Justine, 3175 Chemin de la Côte-Sainte-Catherine, Montréal, QC H3T 1C5, Canada
| | - Jacques Michaud
- Research Center UHC Sainte-Justine, 3175 Chemin de la Côte-Sainte-Catherine, Montréal, QC H3T 1C5, Canada.
| | - Sarah Lippé
- Psychology Departement, Université de Montréal, Pavillon Marie-Victorin, 90, Avenue Vincent d'Indy, Montréal, QC H2V 2S9, Canada; NED Laboratory, Office 5.2.43, 3175 Chemin de la Côte-Sainte-Catherine, Montréal, QC H3T 1C5, Canada; Research Center UHC Sainte-Justine, 3175 Chemin de la Côte-Sainte-Catherine, Montréal, QC H3T 1C5, Canada.
| |
Collapse
|