1
|
Kim M, Kim SP. Distraction impact of concurrent conversation on event-related potential based brain-computer interfaces. J Neural Eng 2024; 21:056004. [PMID: 39178898 DOI: 10.1088/1741-2552/ad731e] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/16/2024] [Accepted: 08/23/2024] [Indexed: 08/26/2024]
Abstract
Objective.This study investigates the impact of conversation on the performance of visual event-related potential (ERP)-based brain-computer interfaces (BCIs), considering distractions in real life environment. The research aims to understand how cognitive distractions from speaking and listening activities affect ERP-BCI performance.Approach.The experiment employs a dual-task paradigm where participants control a smart light using visual ERP-BCIs while simultaneously conducting speaking or listening tasks.Main results.The findings reveal that speaking notably degrades BCI accuracy and the amplitude of ERP components, while increases the latency variability of ERP components and occipital alpha power. In contrast, listening and simple syllable repetition tasks have a lesser impact on these variables. The results suggest that speaking activity significantly distracts visual attentional processes critical for BCI operationSignificance. This study highlights the need to take distractions by daily conversation into account of the design and implementation of ERP-BCIs.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Minju Kim
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Ulsan National Institute of Science and Technology, Ulsan, Republic of Korea
| | - Sung-Phil Kim
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Ulsan National Institute of Science and Technology, Ulsan, Republic of Korea
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Tian Y, Liu S, Wang J. A Corpus Study on the Difference of Turn-Taking in Online Audio, Online Video, and Face-to-Face Conversation. LANGUAGE AND SPEECH 2024; 67:593-616. [PMID: 37317824 DOI: 10.1177/00238309231176768] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/16/2023]
Abstract
Daily conversation is usually face-to-face and characterized by rapid and fluent exchange of turns between interlocutors. With the need to communicate across long distances, advances in communication media, online audio communication, and online video communication have become convenient alternatives for an increasing number of people. However, the fluency of turn-taking may be influenced when people communicate using these different modes. In this study, we conducted a corpus analysis of face-to-face, online audio, and online video conversations collected from the internet. The fluency of turn-taking in face-to-face conversations differed from that of online audio and video conversations. Namely, the timing of turn-taking was shorter and with more overlaps in face-to-face conversations compared with online audio and video conversations. This can be explained by the limited ability of online communication modes to transmit non-verbal cues and network latency. In addition, our study could not completely exclude the effect of formality of conversation. The present findings have implications for the rules of turn-taking in human online conversations, in that the traditional rule of no-gap-no-overlap may not be fully applicable to online conversations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ying Tian
- School of Psychology, Central China Normal University, China; Key Laboratory of Adolescent Cyberpsychology and Behavior (Central China Normal University), Ministry of Education, China; Key Laboratory of Human Development and Mental Health of Hubei Province, China
| | - Siyun Liu
- School of Psychology, Central China Normal University, China; Key Laboratory of Adolescent Cyberpsychology and Behavior (Central China Normal University), Ministry of Education, China; Key Laboratory of Human Development and Mental Health of Hubei Province, China
| | - Jianying Wang
- School of Psychology, Central China Normal University, China; Key Laboratory of Adolescent Cyberpsychology and Behavior (Central China Normal University), Ministry of Education, China; Key Laboratory of Human Development and Mental Health of Hubei Province, China
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Petersen EB, Parker D. Speak Up: How Hearing Loss and the Lack of Hearing Aids Affect Conversations in Quiet. JOURNAL OF SPEECH, LANGUAGE, AND HEARING RESEARCH : JSLHR 2024; 67:2782-2793. [PMID: 38995861 DOI: 10.1044/2024_jslhr-23-00667] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 07/14/2024]
Abstract
PURPOSE The study examines the effect of hearing loss and hearing aid (HA) amplification on the conversational dynamics between hearing-impaired (HI) and normal-hearing (NH) interlocutors. Combining data from the current and a prior study, we explore how the speech levels of both interlocutors correlate and relate to HI interlocutors' degree of hearing loss. METHOD Sixteen pairs of younger NH and elderly HI interlocutors conversed in quiet, with the HI interlocutor either unaided or wearing HAs. We analyzed the effect of hearing status and HA amplification on the conversational dynamics, including turn-taking times (floor-transfer offsets), utterance lengths, and speech levels. Furthermore, we conducted an in-depth analysis of the speech levels using combined data sets from the current and previously published data by Petersen, MacDonald, and Sørensen (2022). RESULTS Unaided HI interlocutors were slower and more variable at timing their turns, but wearing HAs reduced the differences between the HI and NH interlocutors. Conversations were less interactive, and pairs were slower at solving the conversational tasks when the HI interlocutor was unaided. Both interlocutors spoke louder when the HI interlocutor was unaided. The speech level of the NH interlocutors was related to that of the HI interlocutors, with the HI speech levels also correlating with their own degree of hearing loss. CONCLUSIONS Despite typically being unchallenging for HI individuals, one-on-one conversations in quiet were impacted by the HI interlocutor not wearing HAs. Additionally, combining data sets revealed that NH interlocutors adjusted their speech level to match that of HI interlocutors.
Collapse
|
4
|
Corps RE, Pickering MJ. The role of answer content and length when preparing answers to questions. Sci Rep 2024; 14:17110. [PMID: 39048617 PMCID: PMC11269693 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-024-68253-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/28/2023] [Accepted: 07/22/2024] [Indexed: 07/27/2024] Open
Abstract
Research suggests that interlocutors manage the timing demands of conversation by preparing what they want to say early. In three experiments, we used a verbal question-answering task to investigate what aspects of their response speakers prepare early. In all three experiments, participants answered more quickly when the critical content (here, barks) necessary for answer preparation occurred early (e.g., Which animal barks and is also a common household pet?) rather than late (e.g., Which animal is a common household pet and also barks?). In the individual experiments, we found no convincing evidence that participants were slower to produce longer answers, consisting of multiple words, than shorter answers, consisting of a single word. There was also no interaction between these two factors. A combined analysis of the first two experiments confirmed this lack of interaction, and demonstrated that participants were faster to answer questions when the critical content was available early rather than late and when the answer was short rather than long. These findings provide tentative evidence for an account in which interlocutors prepare the content of their answer as soon as they can, but sometimes do not prepare its length (and thus form) until they are ready to speak.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ruth Elizabeth Corps
- Department of Psychology, The University of Sheffield, ICOSS Building, 219 Portobello, Sheffield, S1 4DP, UK.
- Psychology of Language Department, Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen, Netherlands.
- Department of Psychology, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK.
| | | |
Collapse
|
5
|
Ter Bekke M, Levinson SC, van Otterdijk L, Kühn M, Holler J. Visual bodily signals and conversational context benefit the anticipation of turn ends. Cognition 2024; 248:105806. [PMID: 38749291 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105806] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/27/2022] [Revised: 03/04/2024] [Accepted: 04/24/2024] [Indexed: 05/26/2024]
Abstract
The typical pattern of alternating turns in conversation seems trivial at first sight. But a closer look quickly reveals the cognitive challenges involved, with much of it resulting from the fast-paced nature of conversation. One core ingredient to turn coordination is the anticipation of upcoming turn ends so as to be able to ready oneself for providing the next contribution. Across two experiments, we investigated two variables inherent to face-to-face conversation, the presence of visual bodily signals and preceding discourse context, in terms of their contribution to turn end anticipation. In a reaction time paradigm, participants anticipated conversational turn ends better when seeing the speaker and their visual bodily signals than when they did not, especially so for longer turns. Likewise, participants were better able to anticipate turn ends when they had access to the preceding discourse context than when they did not, and especially so for longer turns. Critically, the two variables did not interact, showing that visual bodily signals retain their influence even in the context of preceding discourse. In a pre-registered follow-up experiment, we manipulated the visibility of the speaker's head, eyes and upper body (i.e. torso + arms). Participants were better able to anticipate turn ends when the speaker's upper body was visible, suggesting a role for manual gestures in turn end anticipation. Together, these findings show that seeing the speaker during conversation may critically facilitate turn coordination in interaction.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marlijn Ter Bekke
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition & Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, the Netherlands; Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen, the Netherlands
| | | | - Lina van Otterdijk
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition & Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, the Netherlands
| | - Michelle Kühn
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition & Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, the Netherlands
| | - Judith Holler
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition & Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, the Netherlands; Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen, the Netherlands.
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Ter Bekke M, Drijvers L, Holler J. Hand Gestures Have Predictive Potential During Conversation: An Investigation of the Timing of Gestures in Relation to Speech. Cogn Sci 2024; 48:e13407. [PMID: 38279899 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13407] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/23/2022] [Revised: 07/09/2023] [Accepted: 01/10/2024] [Indexed: 01/29/2024]
Abstract
During face-to-face conversation, transitions between speaker turns are incredibly fast. These fast turn exchanges seem to involve next speakers predicting upcoming semantic information, such that next turn planning can begin before a current turn is complete. Given that face-to-face conversation also involves the use of communicative bodily signals, an important question is how bodily signals such as co-speech hand gestures play into these processes of prediction and fast responding. In this corpus study, we found that hand gestures that depict or refer to semantic information started before the corresponding information in speech, which held both for the onset of the gesture as a whole, as well as the onset of the stroke (the most meaningful part of the gesture). This early timing potentially allows listeners to use the gestural information to predict the corresponding semantic information to be conveyed in speech. Moreover, we provided further evidence that questions with gestures got faster responses than questions without gestures. However, we found no evidence for the idea that how much a gesture precedes its lexical affiliate (i.e., its predictive potential) relates to how fast responses were given. The findings presented here highlight the importance of the temporal relation between speech and gesture and help to illuminate the potential mechanisms underpinning multimodal language processing during face-to-face conversation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marlijn Ter Bekke
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University
- Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics
| | - Linda Drijvers
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University
- Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics
| | - Judith Holler
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University
- Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Bögels S, Levinson SC. Ultrasound measurements of interactive turn-taking in question-answer sequences: Articulatory preparation is delayed but not tied to the response. PLoS One 2023; 18:e0276470. [PMID: 37405982 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0276470] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/07/2022] [Accepted: 06/16/2023] [Indexed: 07/07/2023] Open
Abstract
We know that speech planning in conversational turn-taking can happen in overlap with the previous turn and research suggests that it starts as early as possible, that is, as soon as the gist of the previous turn becomes clear. The present study aimed to investigate whether planning proceeds all the way up to the last stage of articulatory preparation (i.e., putting the articulators in place for the first phoneme of the response) and what the timing of this process is. Participants answered pre-recorded quiz questions (being under the illusion that they were asked live), while their tongue movements were measured using ultrasound. Planning could start early for some quiz questions (i.e., midway during the question), but late for others (i.e., only at the end of the question). The results showed no evidence for a difference between tongue movements in these two types of questions for at least two seconds after planning could start in early-planning questions, suggesting that speech planning in overlap with the current turn proceeds more slowly than in the clear. On the other hand, when time-locking to speech onset, tongue movements differed between the two conditions from up to two seconds before this point. This suggests that articulatory preparation can occur in advance and is not fully tied to the overt response itself.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sara Bögels
- Department of Communication and Cognition, Tilburg University, Tilburg, The Netherlands
- Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition, and Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | | |
Collapse
|
8
|
Meyer AS. Timing in Conversation. J Cogn 2023; 6:20. [PMID: 37033404 PMCID: PMC10077995 DOI: 10.5334/joc.268] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/09/2022] [Accepted: 03/17/2023] [Indexed: 04/08/2023] Open
Abstract
Turn-taking in everyday conversation is fast, with median latencies in corpora of conversational speech often reported to be under 300 ms. This seems like magic, given that experimental research on speech planning has shown that speakers need much more time to plan and produce even the shortest of utterances. This paper reviews how language scientists have combined linguistic analyses of conversations and experimental work to understand the skill of swift turn-taking and proposes a tentative solution to the riddle of fast turn-taking.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Antje S. Meyer
- Radboud University Nijmegen and Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics and Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Rann JC, Almor A. Effects of verbal tasks on driving simulator performance. Cogn Res Princ Implic 2022; 7:12. [PMID: 35119569 PMCID: PMC8817015 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-022-00357-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/11/2021] [Accepted: 01/08/2022] [Indexed: 11/10/2022] Open
Abstract
We report results from a driving simulator paradigm we developed to test the fine temporal effects of verbal tasks on simultaneous tracking performance. A total of 74 undergraduate students participated in two experiments in which they controlled a cursor using the steering wheel to track a moving target and where the dependent measure was overall deviation from target. Experiment 1 tested tracking performance during slow and fast target speeds under conditions involving either no verbal input or output, passive listening to spoken prompts via headphones, or responding to spoken prompts. Experiment 2 was similar except that participants read written prompts overlain on the simulator screen instead of listening to spoken prompts. Performance in both experiments was worse during fast speeds and worst overall during responding conditions. Most significantly, fine scale time-course analysis revealed deteriorating tracking performance as participants prepared and began speaking and steadily improving performance while speaking. Additionally, post-block survey data revealed that conversation recall was best in responding conditions, and perceived difficulty increased with task complexity. Our study is the first to track temporal changes in interference at high resolution during the first hundreds of milliseconds of verbal production and comprehension. Our results are consistent with load-based theories of multitasking performance and show that language production, and, to a lesser extent, language comprehension tap resources also used for tracking. More generally, our paradigm provides a useful tool for measuring dynamical changes in tracking performance during verbal tasks due to the rapidly changing resource requirements of language production and comprehension.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jonathan C Rann
- Department of Psychology, University of South Carolina, 1512 Pendelton Street, Columbia, SC, 29208, USA. .,Institute for Mind and Brain, University of South Carolina, Columbia, SC, 29208, USA.
| | - Amit Almor
- Department of Psychology, University of South Carolina, 1512 Pendelton Street, Columbia, SC, 29208, USA.,Institute for Mind and Brain, University of South Carolina, Columbia, SC, 29208, USA.,Linguistics Program, University of South Carolina, Columbia, SC, 29208, USA
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Corps RE, Knudsen B, Meyer AS. Overrated gaps: Inter-speaker gaps provide limited information about the timing of turns in conversation. Cognition 2022; 223:105037. [PMID: 35123218 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2022.105037] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/30/2021] [Revised: 01/18/2022] [Accepted: 01/20/2022] [Indexed: 11/03/2022]
Abstract
Corpus analyses have shown that turn-taking in conversation is much faster than laboratory studies of speech planning would predict. To explain fast turn-taking, Levinson and Torreira (2015) proposed that speakers are highly proactive: They begin to plan a response to their interlocutor's turn as soon as they have understood its gist, and launch this planned response when the turn-end is imminent. Thus, fast turn-taking is possible because speakers use the time while their partner is talking to plan their own utterance. In the present study, we asked how much time upcoming speakers actually have to plan their utterances. Following earlier psycholinguistic work, we used transcripts of spoken conversations in Dutch, German, and English. These transcripts consisted of segments, which are continuous stretches of speech by one speaker. In the psycholinguistic and phonetic literature, such segments have often been used as proxies for turns. We found that in all three corpora, large proportions of the segments comprised of only one or two words, which on our estimate does not give the next speaker enough time to fully plan a response. Further analyses showed that speakers indeed often did not respond to the immediately preceding segment of their partner, but continued an earlier segment of their own. More generally, our findings suggest that speech segments derived from transcribed corpora do not necessarily correspond to turns, and the gaps between speech segments therefore only provide limited information about the planning and timing of turns.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ruth E Corps
- Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen, The Netherlands.
| | - Birgit Knudsen
- Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | | |
Collapse
|
11
|
Petersen EB, MacDonald EN, Josefine Munch Sørensen A. The Effects of Hearing-Aid Amplification and Noise on Conversational Dynamics Between Normal-Hearing and Hearing-Impaired Talkers. Trends Hear 2022; 26:23312165221103340. [PMID: 35862280 PMCID: PMC9310272 DOI: 10.1177/23312165221103340] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/31/2021] [Revised: 04/25/2022] [Accepted: 04/26/2022] [Indexed: 11/16/2022] Open
Abstract
There is a long-standing tradition to assess hearing-aid benefits using lab-based speech intelligibility tests. Towards a more everyday-like scenario, the current study investigated the effects of hearing-aid amplification and noise on face-to-face communication between two conversational partners. Eleven pairs, consisting of a younger normal-hearing (NH) and an older hearing-impaired (HI) participant, solved spot-the-difference tasks while their conversations were recorded. In a two-block randomized design, the tasks were solved in quiet or noise, both with and without the HI participant receiving hearing-aid amplification with active occlusion cancellation. In the presence of 70 dB SPL babble noise, participants had fewer, slower, and less well-timed turn-starts, while speaking louder with longer inter-pausal units (IPUs, stretches of continuous speech surrounded by silence) and reducing their articulation rates. All these changes are indicative of increased communication effort. The timing of turn-starts by the HI participants exhibited more variability than that of their NH conversational partners. In the presence of background noise, the timing of turn-starts by the HI participants became even more variable, and their NH partners spoke louder. When the HI participants were provided with hearing-aid amplification, their timing of turn-starts became faster, they increased their articulation rate, and they produced shorter IPUs, all indicating reduced communication effort. In conclusion, measures of the conversational dynamics showed that background noise increased the communication effort, especially for the HI participants, and that providing hearing-aid amplification caused the HI participant to behave more like their NH conversational partner, especially in quiet situations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Ewen N. MacDonald
- Hearing Systems Group, Dept. of Health Technology, Technical University of Denmark, Kongens Lyngby, Denmark
- Department of Systems Design Engineering, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, ON, Canada
| | - A. Josefine Munch Sørensen
- Hearing Systems Group, Dept. of Health Technology, Technical University of Denmark, Kongens Lyngby, Denmark
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Bögels S, Torreira F. Turn-end Estimation in Conversational Turn-taking: The Roles of Context and Prosody. DISCOURSE PROCESSES 2021. [DOI: 10.1080/0163853x.2021.1986664] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Sara Bögels
- Department of Communication and Cognition, Tilburg University
- Language and Cognition Department, Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition, and Behaviour, Radboud University
| | - Francisco Torreira
- Language and Cognition Department, Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics
- Department of Linguistics, McGill University
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Holler J, Alday PM, Decuyper C, Geiger M, Kendrick KH, Meyer AS. Competition Reduces Response Times in Multiparty Conversation. Front Psychol 2021; 12:693124. [PMID: 34603124 PMCID: PMC8481383 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.693124] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/09/2021] [Accepted: 08/04/2021] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Natural conversations are characterized by short transition times between turns. This holds in particular for multi-party conversations. The short turn transitions in everyday conversations contrast sharply with the much longer speech onset latencies observed in laboratory studies where speakers respond to spoken utterances. There are many factors that facilitate speech production in conversational compared to laboratory settings. Here we highlight one of them, the impact of competition for turns. In multi-party conversations, speakers often compete for turns. In quantitative corpus analyses of multi-party conversation, the fastest response determines the recorded turn transition time. In contrast, in dyadic conversations such competition for turns is much less likely to arise, and in laboratory experiments with individual participants it does not arise at all. Therefore, all responses tend to be recorded. Thus, competition for turns may reduce the recorded mean turn transition times in multi-party conversations for a simple statistical reason: slow responses are not included in the means. We report two studies illustrating this point. We first report the results of simulations showing how much the response times in a laboratory experiment would be reduced if, for each trial, instead of recording all responses, only the fastest responses of several participants responding independently on the trial were recorded. We then present results from a quantitative corpus analysis comparing turn transition times in dyadic and triadic conversations. There was no significant group size effect in question-response transition times, where the present speaker often selects the next one, thus reducing competition between speakers. But, as predicted, triads showed shorter turn transition times than dyads for the remaining turn transitions, where competition for the floor was more likely to arise. Together, these data show that turn transition times in conversation should be interpreted in the context of group size, turn transition type, and social setting.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Judith Holler
- Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen, Netherlands.,Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition, and Behavior, Radboud University, Nijmegen, Netherlands
| | - Phillip M Alday
- Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen, Netherlands
| | - Caitlin Decuyper
- Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen, Netherlands
| | - Mareike Geiger
- Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen, Netherlands.,Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition, and Behavior, Radboud University, Nijmegen, Netherlands
| | | | - Antje S Meyer
- Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen, Netherlands.,Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition, and Behavior, Radboud University, Nijmegen, Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Jongman SR. The attentional demands of combining comprehension and production in conversation. PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING AND MOTIVATION 2021. [DOI: 10.1016/bs.plm.2021.02.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
|
15
|
Knudsen B, Creemers A, Meyer AS. Forgotten Little Words: How Backchannels and Particles May Facilitate Speech Planning in Conversation? Front Psychol 2020; 11:593671. [PMID: 33240183 PMCID: PMC7677452 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.593671] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/11/2020] [Accepted: 10/15/2020] [Indexed: 11/15/2022] Open
Abstract
In everyday conversation, turns often follow each other immediately or overlap in time. It has been proposed that speakers achieve this tight temporal coordination between their turns by engaging in linguistic dual-tasking, i.e., by beginning to plan their utterance during the preceding turn. This raises the question of how speakers manage to co-ordinate speech planning and listening with each other. Experimental work addressing this issue has mostly concerned the capacity demands and interference arising when speakers retrieve some content words while listening to others. However, many contributions to conversations are not content words, but backchannels, such as “hm”. Backchannels do not provide much conceptual content and are therefore easy to plan and respond to. To estimate how much they might facilitate speech planning in conversation, we determined their frequency in a Dutch and a German corpus of conversational speech. We found that 19% of the contributions in the Dutch corpus, and 16% of contributions in the German corpus were backchannels. In addition, many turns began with fillers or particles, most often translation equivalents of “yes” or “no,” which are likewise easy to plan. We proposed that to generate comprehensive models of using language in conversation psycholinguists should study not only the generation and processing of content words, as is commonly done, but also consider backchannels, fillers, and particles.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Birgit Knudsen
- Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen, Netherlands
| | - Ava Creemers
- Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen, Netherlands
| | - Antje S Meyer
- Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen, Netherlands.,Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Bögels S. Neural correlates of turn-taking in the wild: Response planning starts early in free interviews. Cognition 2020; 203:104347. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104347] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/18/2019] [Revised: 05/20/2020] [Accepted: 05/30/2020] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
|
17
|
Making oneself predictable in linguistic interactions. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2020; 209:103125. [PMID: 32603913 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2020.103125] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/31/2020] [Revised: 04/22/2020] [Accepted: 06/17/2020] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
While language production is a highly demanding task, conversational partners are known to coordinate their turns with striking precision. Among the mechanisms that allow them to do so is listeners' ability to predict what the speaker will say, and thus to prepare their response in advance. But do speakers also play a role in facilitating coordination? We hypothesized that speakers contribute by using coordination smoothers - in particular by making their turns easier to predict. To test this, we asked participants to type definitions for common English words, either on their own (n = 26 individuals) or interacting with a partner (n = 18 pairs), and we measured the timing with which they produced the definitions. In a post-test, additional participants (n = 55) attempted to predict the final word of these definitions and rated them for quality. We found that interacting speakers initiated their turns with less variable delays than solo individuals. In contrast, our post-test measures suggested that jointly produced definitions were in fact of lower predictability and quality than those produced by individuals, but the analysis revealed these findings were likely confounded by task difficulty. We propose that the reduction in temporal variability observed for interacting speakers may facilitate prediction and thus act as a coordination smoother in linguistic interactions.
Collapse
|
18
|
Aruffo C. Reading Scripted Dialogue: Pretending to Take Turns. DISCOURSE PROCESSES 2020. [DOI: 10.1080/0163853x.2019.1651588] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
|
19
|
Sjerps MJ, Decuyper C, Meyer AS. Initiation of utterance planning in response to pre-recorded and "live" utterances. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2019; 73:357-374. [PMID: 31544625 DOI: 10.1177/1747021819881265] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
In everyday conversation, interlocutors often plan their utterances while listening to their conversational partners, thereby achieving short gaps between their turns. Important issues for current psycholinguistics are how interlocutors distribute their attention between listening and speech planning and how speech planning is timed relative to listening. Laboratory studies addressing these issues have used a variety of paradigms, some of which have involved using recorded speech to which participants responded, whereas others have involved interactions with confederates. This study investigated how this variation in the speech input affected the participants' timing of speech planning. In Experiment 1, participants responded to utterances produced by a confederate, who sat next to them and looked at the same screen. In Experiment 2, they responded to recorded utterances of the same confederate. Analyses of the participants' speech, their eye movements, and their performance in a concurrent tapping task showed that, compared with recorded speech, the presence of the confederate increased the processing load for the participants, but did not alter their global sentence planning strategy. These results have implications for the design of psycholinguistic experiments and theories of listening and speaking in dyadic settings.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Matthias J Sjerps
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | - Caitlin Decuyper
- Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | - Antje S Meyer
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands.,Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Barthel M, Sauppe S. Speech Planning at Turn Transitions in Dialog Is Associated With Increased Processing Load. Cogn Sci 2019; 43:e12768. [DOI: 10.1111/cogs.12768] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/25/2018] [Revised: 02/13/2019] [Accepted: 05/21/2019] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Mathias Barthel
- Language and Cognition Department Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics
| | | |
Collapse
|
21
|
Holler J, Levinson SC. Multimodal Language Processing in Human Communication. Trends Cogn Sci 2019; 23:639-652. [PMID: 31235320 DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2019.05.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 129] [Impact Index Per Article: 21.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/07/2019] [Revised: 05/17/2019] [Accepted: 05/21/2019] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
The natural ecology of human language is face-to-face interaction comprising the exchange of a plethora of multimodal signals. Trying to understand the psycholinguistic processing of language in its natural niche raises new issues, first and foremost the binding of multiple, temporally offset signals under tight time constraints posed by a turn-taking system. This might be expected to overload and slow our cognitive system, but the reverse is in fact the case. We propose cognitive mechanisms that may explain this phenomenon and call for a multimodal, situated psycholinguistic framework to unravel the full complexities of human language processing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Judith Holler
- Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen, The Netherlands; Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen, The Netherlands.
| | - Stephen C Levinson
- Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen, The Netherlands; Centre for Language Studies, Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Abstract
Even when speakers are not actively doing another task, they can be interfered in their speech planning by concurrent auditory stimuli. In this study, we used picture naming with passive hearing, or active listening, combined to high-density electroencephalographic (EEG) recordings to investigate the locus and origin of interference on speech production. Participants named pictures while ignoring (or paying attention to) auditory syllables presented at different intervals (+150 ms, +300 ms or +450 ms). Interference of passive hearing was observed at all positive stimulus onset asynchronies (SOA) including when distractors appeared 450 ms after picture onset. Analyses of ERPs and microstates revealed modulations appearing in a time-window close to verbal response onset likely relating to post-lexical planning processes. A shift of latency of the N1 auditory component for syllables displayed 450 ms after picture onset relative to hearing in isolation was also observed. Data from picture naming with active listening to auditory syllables also pointed to post-lexical interference. The present study suggests that, beyond the lexical stage, post-lexical processes can be interfered and that the reciprocal interference between utterance planning and hearing relies on attentional demand and possibly competing neural substrates.
Collapse
|
23
|
Lindsay L, Gambi C, Rabagliati H. Preschoolers Optimize the Timing of Their Conversational Turns Through Flexible Coordination of Language Comprehension and Production. Psychol Sci 2019; 30:504-515. [DOI: 10.1177/0956797618822802] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022] Open
Abstract
Conversation is the natural setting for language learning and use, and a key property of conversation is the smooth taking of turns. In adult conversations, delays between turns are minimal (typically 200 ms or less) because listeners display a striking ability to predict what their partner will say, and they formulate a response before their partner’s turn ends. Here, we tested how this ability to coordinate comprehension and production develops in preschool children. In an interactive paradigm, 106 children (ages 3–5 years) and 48 adults responded to questions that varied in predictability but were controlled for linguistic complexity. Using a novel distributional approach to data analysis, we found that when children can predict a question’s ending, they leave shorter gaps before responding, suggesting that they can optimize the timing of their conversational turns like adults do. In line with a recent ethological theory of turn taking, this early competency helps explain how conversational contexts support language development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Laura Lindsay
- Department of Psychology, School of Philosophy, Psychology and Language Sciences, The University of Edinburgh
| | - Chiara Gambi
- Department of Psychology, School of Philosophy, Psychology and Language Sciences, The University of Edinburgh
- School of Psychology, Cardiff University
| | - Hugh Rabagliati
- Department of Psychology, School of Philosophy, Psychology and Language Sciences, The University of Edinburgh
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Fairs A, Bögels S, Meyer AS. Dual-tasking with simple linguistic tasks: Evidence for serial processing. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2018; 191:131-148. [PMID: 30268022 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2018.09.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/05/2017] [Revised: 09/06/2018] [Accepted: 09/10/2018] [Indexed: 11/29/2022] Open
Abstract
In contrast to the large amount of dual-task research investigating the coordination of a linguistic and a non-linguistic task, little research has investigated how two linguistic tasks are coordinated. However, such research would greatly contribute to our understanding of how interlocutors combine speech planning and listening in conversation. In three dual-task experiments we studied how participants coordinated the processing of an auditory stimulus (S1), which was either a syllable or a tone, with selecting a name for a picture (S2). Two SOAs, of 0 ms and 1000 ms, were used. To vary the time required for lexical selection and to determine when lexical selection took place, the pictures were presented with categorically related or unrelated distractor words. In Experiment 1 participants responded overtly to both stimuli. In Experiments 2 and 3, S1 was not responded to overtly, but determined how to respond to S2, by naming the picture or reading the distractor aloud. Experiment 1 yielded additive effects of SOA and distractor type on the picture naming latencies. The presence of semantic interference at both SOAs indicated that lexical selection occurred after response selection for S1. With respect to the coordination of S1 and S2 processing, Experiments 2 and 3 yielded inconclusive results. In all experiments, syllables interfered more with picture naming than tones. This is likely because the syllables activated phonological representations also implicated in picture naming. The theoretical and methodological implications of the findings are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Amie Fairs
- Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, P.O. Box 310, 6500AH Nijmegen, the Netherlands; International Max Planck Research School for Language Sciences, P.O. Box 310, 6500AH Nijmegen, the Netherlands.
| | - Sara Bögels
- Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, P.O. Box 310, 6500AH Nijmegen, the Netherlands; Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, P.O. Box 9010, 6500GL Nijmegen, the Netherlands
| | - Antje S Meyer
- Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, P.O. Box 310, 6500AH Nijmegen, the Netherlands; Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, P.O. Box 9010, 6500GL Nijmegen, the Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Corps RE, Crossley A, Gambi C, Pickering MJ. Early preparation during turn-taking: Listeners use content predictions to determine what to say but not when to say it. Cognition 2018; 175:77-95. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.01.015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/29/2017] [Revised: 01/26/2018] [Accepted: 01/29/2018] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
|
26
|
Meyer AS, Alday PM, Decuyper C, Knudsen B. Working Together: Contributions of Corpus Analyses and Experimental Psycholinguistics to Understanding Conversation. Front Psychol 2018; 9:525. [PMID: 29706919 PMCID: PMC5906713 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00525] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/08/2017] [Accepted: 03/27/2018] [Indexed: 11/25/2022] Open
Abstract
As conversation is the most important way of using language, linguists and psychologists should combine forces to investigate how interlocutors deal with the cognitive demands arising during conversation. Linguistic analyses of corpora of conversation are needed to understand the structure of conversations, and experimental work is indispensable for understanding the underlying cognitive processes. We argue that joint consideration of corpus and experimental data is most informative when the utterances elicited in a lab experiment match those extracted from a corpus in relevant ways. This requirement to compare like with like seems obvious but is not trivial to achieve. To illustrate this approach, we report two experiments where responses to polar (yes/no) questions were elicited in the lab and the response latencies were compared to gaps between polar questions and answers in a corpus of conversational speech. We found, as expected, that responses were given faster when they were easy to plan and planning could be initiated earlier than when they were harder to plan and planning was initiated later. Overall, in all but one condition, the latencies were longer than one would expect based on the analyses of corpus data. We discuss the implication of this partial match between the data sets and more generally how corpus and experimental data can best be combined in studies of conversation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Antje S Meyer
- Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Radboud University, Nijmegen, Netherlands
| | - Phillip M Alday
- Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Radboud University, Nijmegen, Netherlands
| | - Caitlin Decuyper
- Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Radboud University, Nijmegen, Netherlands
| | - Birgit Knudsen
- Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Radboud University, Nijmegen, Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
AlignTool: The automatic temporal alignment of spoken utterances in German, Dutch, and British English for psycholinguistic purposes. Behav Res Methods 2018; 50:466-489. [PMID: 29380301 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-017-1002-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
In language production research, the latency with which speakers produce a spoken response to a stimulus and the onset and offset times of words in longer utterances are key dependent variables. Measuring these variables automatically often yields partially incorrect results. However, exact measurements through the visual inspection of the recordings are extremely time-consuming. We present AlignTool, an open-source alignment tool that establishes preliminarily the onset and offset times of words and phonemes in spoken utterances using Praat, and subsequently performs a forced alignment of the spoken utterances and their orthographic transcriptions in the automatic speech recognition system MAUS. AlignTool creates a Praat TextGrid file for inspection and manual correction by the user, if necessary. We evaluated AlignTool's performance with recordings of single-word and four-word utterances as well as semi-spontaneous speech. AlignTool performs well with audio signals with an excellent signal-to-noise ratio, requiring virtually no corrections. For audio signals of lesser quality, AlignTool still is highly functional but its results may require more frequent manual corrections. We also found that audio recordings including long silent intervals tended to pose greater difficulties for AlignTool than recordings filled with speech, which AlignTool analyzed well overall. We expect that by semi-automatizing the temporal analysis of complex utterances, AlignTool will open new avenues in language production research.
Collapse
|
28
|
Knowing when to respond: the role of visual information in conversational turn exchanges. Atten Percept Psychophys 2018; 80:27-41. [DOI: 10.3758/s13414-017-1428-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
29
|
Planning versus comprehension in turn-taking: Fast responders show reduced anticipatory processing of the question. Neuropsychologia 2017; 109:295-310. [PMID: 29269305 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.12.028] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/29/2017] [Revised: 11/13/2017] [Accepted: 12/15/2017] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
Rapid response latencies in conversation suggest that responders start planning before the ongoing turn is finished. Indeed, an earlier EEG study suggests that listeners start planning their responses to questions as soon as they can (Bögels et al., 2015a). The present study aimed to (1) replicate this early planning effect and (2) investigate whether such early response planning incurs a cost on participants' concurrent comprehension of the ongoing turn. During the experiment participants answered questions from a confederate partner. To address aim (1), the questions were designed such that response planning could start either early or late in the turn. Our results largely replicate Bögels et al. (2015a), showing a large positive ERP effect and an oscillatory alpha/beta reduction right after participants could have first started planning their verbal response, again suggesting an early start of response planning. To address aim (2), the confederate's questions also contained either an expected word or an unexpected one to elicit a differential N400 effect, either before or after the start of response planning. We hypothesized an attenuated N400 effect after response planning had started. In contrast, the N400 effects before and after planning did not differ. There was, however, a positive correlation between participants' response time and their N400 effect size after planning had started; quick responders showed a smaller N400 effect, suggesting reduced attention to comprehension and possibly reduced anticipatory processing. We conclude that early response planning can indeed impact comprehension processing.
Collapse
|
30
|
To plan or not to plan: Does planning for production remove facilitation from associative priming? Acta Psychol (Amst) 2017; 181:40-50. [PMID: 29045888 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2017.10.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/06/2017] [Revised: 08/25/2017] [Accepted: 10/08/2017] [Indexed: 11/23/2022] Open
Abstract
Theories of conversation propose that in order to have smooth transitions from one turn to the next, speakers already plan their response while listening to their interlocutor. Moreover, it has been argued that speakers align their linguistic representations (i.e. prime each other), thereby reducing the processing costs associated with concurrent listening and speaking. In two experiments, we assessed how identity and associative priming from spoken words onto picture naming were affected by a concurrent speech planning task. In a baseline (no name) condition, participants heard prime words that were identical, associatively related, or unrelated to target pictures presented two seconds after prime onset. Each prime was accompanied by a non-target picture and followed by its recorded name. The participant did not name the non-target picture. In the plan condition, the participants first named the non-target picture, instead of listening to the recording, and then the target. In Experiment 1, where the plan- and no-plan conditions were tested between participants, priming effects of equal strength were found in the plan and no-plan condition. In Experiment 2, where the two conditions were tested within participants, the identity priming effect was maintained, but the associative priming effect was only seen in the no-plan but not in the plan condition. In this experiment, participant had to decide at the onset of each trial whether or not to name the non-target picture, rendering the task more complex than in Experiment 1. These decision processes may have interfered with the processing of the primes. Thus, associative priming can take place during speech planning, but only if the cognitive load is not too high.
Collapse
|
31
|
Corps RE, Gambi C, Pickering MJ. Coordinating Utterances During Turn-Taking: The Role of Prediction, Response Preparation, and Articulation. DISCOURSE PROCESSES 2017. [DOI: 10.1080/0163853x.2017.1330031] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Ruth E. Corps
- Department of Psychology University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK
| | - Chiara Gambi
- Department of Psychology University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK
| | | |
Collapse
|
32
|
Barthel M, Meyer AS, Levinson SC. Next Speakers Plan Their Turn Early and Speak after Turn-Final "Go-Signals". Front Psychol 2017; 8:393. [PMID: 28443035 PMCID: PMC5387091 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00393] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/12/2016] [Accepted: 03/01/2017] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
In conversation, turn-taking is usually fluid, with next speakers taking their turn right after the end of the previous turn. Most, but not all, previous studies show that next speakers start to plan their turn early, if possible already during the incoming turn. The present study makes use of the list-completion paradigm (Barthel et al., 2016), analyzing speech onset latencies and eye-movements of participants in a task-oriented dialogue with a confederate. The measures are used to disentangle the contributions to the timing of turn-taking of early planning of content on the one hand and initiation of articulation as a reaction to the upcoming turn-end on the other hand. Participants named objects visible on their computer screen in response to utterances that did, or did not, contain lexical and prosodic cues to the end of the incoming turn. In the presence of an early lexical cue, participants showed earlier gaze shifts toward the target objects and responded faster than in its absence, whereas the presence of a late intonational cue only led to faster response times and did not affect the timing of participants' eye movements. The results show that with a combination of eye-movement and turn-transition time measures it is possible to tease apart the effects of early planning and response initiation on turn timing. They are consistent with models of turn-taking that assume that next speakers (a) start planning their response as soon as the incoming turn's message can be understood and (b) monitor the incoming turn for cues to turn-completion so as to initiate their response when turn-transition becomes relevant.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mathias Barthel
- Language and Cognition Department, Max Planck Institute for PsycholinguisticsNijmegen, Netherlands
| | - Antje S Meyer
- Psychology of Language Department, Max Planck Institute for PsycholinguisticsNijmegen, Netherlands.,Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud UniversityNijmegen, Netherlands
| | - Stephen C Levinson
- Language and Cognition Department, Max Planck Institute for PsycholinguisticsNijmegen, Netherlands.,Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud UniversityNijmegen, Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
33
|
Magyari L, De Ruiter JP, Levinson SC. Temporal Preparation for Speaking in Question-Answer Sequences. Front Psychol 2017; 8:211. [PMID: 28270782 PMCID: PMC5318421 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00211] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/14/2016] [Accepted: 02/03/2017] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
In every-day conversations, the gap between turns of conversational partners is most frequently between 0 and 200 ms. We were interested how speakers achieve such fast transitions. We designed an experiment in which participants listened to pre-recorded questions about images presented on a screen and were asked to answer these questions. We tested whether speakers already prepare their answers while they listen to questions and whether they can prepare for the time of articulation by anticipating when questions end. In the experiment, it was possible to guess the answer at the beginning of the questions in half of the experimental trials. We also manipulated whether it was possible to predict the length of the last word of the questions. The results suggest when listeners know the answer early they start speech production already during the questions. Speakers can also time when to speak by predicting the duration of turns. These temporal predictions can be based on the length of anticipated words and on the overall probability of turn durations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lilla Magyari
- Department of General Psychology, Faculty of Humanities and Social Sciences, Pázmány Péter Catholic University Budapest, Hungary
| | - Jan P De Ruiter
- Department of Psychology, School of Arts and Sciences, Tufts University, Medford MA, USA
| | - Stephen C Levinson
- Language and Cognition Department, Max Planck Institute for PsycholinguisticsNijmegen, Netherlands; Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud UniversityNijmegen, Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
34
|
Barthel M, Sauppe S, Levinson SC, Meyer AS. The Timing of Utterance Planning in Task-Oriented Dialogue: Evidence from a Novel List-Completion Paradigm. Front Psychol 2016; 7:1858. [PMID: 27990127 PMCID: PMC5131015 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01858] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/18/2016] [Accepted: 11/09/2016] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
In conversation, interlocutors rarely leave long gaps between turns, suggesting that next speakers begin to plan their turns while listening to the previous speaker. The present experiment used analyses of speech onset latencies and eye-movements in a task-oriented dialogue paradigm to investigate when speakers start planning their responses. German speakers heard a confederate describe sets of objects in utterances that either ended in a noun [e.g., Ich habe eine Tür und ein Fahrrad ("I have a door and a bicycle")] or a verb form [e.g., Ich habe eine Tür und ein Fahrrad besorgt ("I have gotten a door and a bicycle")], while the presence or absence of the final verb either was or was not predictable from the preceding sentence structure. In response, participants had to name any unnamed objects they could see in their own displays with utterances such as Ich habe ein Ei ("I have an egg"). The results show that speakers begin to plan their turns as soon as sufficient information is available to do so, irrespective of further incoming words.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mathias Barthel
- Language and Cognition Department, Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics Nijmegen, Netherlands
| | - Sebastian Sauppe
- Language and Cognition Department, Max Planck Institute for PsycholinguisticsNijmegen, Netherlands; Department of Comparative Linguistics, University of ZurichZurich, Switzerland
| | - Stephen C Levinson
- Language and Cognition Department, Max Planck Institute for PsycholinguisticsNijmegen, Netherlands; Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud UniversityNijmegen, Netherlands
| | - Antje S Meyer
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud UniversityNijmegen, Netherlands; Psychology of Language Department, Max Planck Institute for PsycholinguisticsNijmegen, Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
35
|
Levinson SC. Turn-taking in Human Communication--Origins and Implications for Language Processing. Trends Cogn Sci 2015; 20:6-14. [PMID: 26651245 DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.10.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 270] [Impact Index Per Article: 27.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/04/2015] [Revised: 10/26/2015] [Accepted: 10/28/2015] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Most language usage is interactive, involving rapid turn-taking. The turn-taking system has a number of striking properties: turns are short and responses are remarkably rapid, but turns are of varying length and often of very complex construction such that the underlying cognitive processing is highly compressed. Although neglected in cognitive science, the system has deep implications for language processing and acquisition that are only now becoming clear. Appearing earlier in ontogeny than linguistic competence, it is also found across all the major primate clades. This suggests a possible phylogenetic continuity, which may provide key insights into language evolution.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Stephen C Levinson
- Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Wundtlaan 1, NL-6525 XD Nijmegen, The Netherlands; Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands.
| |
Collapse
|
36
|
Turn-taking: From perception to speech preparation. Neurosci Lett 2015; 609:147-51. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neulet.2015.10.033] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/03/2015] [Revised: 10/05/2015] [Accepted: 10/12/2015] [Indexed: 11/21/2022]
|
37
|
Bögels S, Magyari L, Levinson SC. Neural signatures of response planning occur midway through an incoming question in conversation. Sci Rep 2015; 5:12881. [PMID: 26242909 PMCID: PMC4525376 DOI: 10.1038/srep12881] [Citation(s) in RCA: 79] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/11/2015] [Accepted: 07/13/2015] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
A striking puzzle about language use in everyday conversation is that turn-taking latencies are usually very short, whereas planning language production takes much longer. This implies overlap between language comprehension and production processes, but the nature and extent of such overlap has never been studied directly. Combining an interactive quiz paradigm with EEG measurements in an innovative way, we show that production planning processes start as soon as possible, that is, within half a second after the answer to a question can be retrieved (up to several seconds before the end of the question). Localization of ERP data shows early activation even of brain areas related to late stages of production planning (e.g., syllabification). Finally, oscillation results suggest an attention switch from comprehension to production around the same time frame. This perspective from interactive language use throws new light on the performance characteristics that language competence involves.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sara Bögels
- Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | - Lilla Magyari
- 1] Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen, The Netherlands [2] Pazmany Peter Catholic University, Faculty of Humanities and Social Sciences, Department of General Psychology, Budapest, Hungary
| | - Stephen C Levinson
- 1] Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen, The Netherlands [2] Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
38
|
Hintz F, Meyer AS. Prediction and Production of Simple Mathematical Equations: Evidence from Visual World Eye-Tracking. PLoS One 2015; 10:e0130766. [PMID: 26154431 PMCID: PMC4496095 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0130766] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/23/2015] [Accepted: 05/23/2015] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
The relationship between the production and the comprehension systems has recently become a topic of interest for many psycholinguists. It has been argued that these systems are tightly linked and in particular that listeners use the production system to predict upcoming content. In this study, we tested how similar production and prediction processes are in a novel version of the visual world paradigm. Dutch speaking participants (native speakers in Experiment 1; German-Dutch bilinguals in Experiment 2) listened to mathematical equations while looking at a clock face featuring the numbers 1 to 12. On alternating trials, they either heard a complete equation ("three plus eight is eleven") or they heard the first part ("three plus eight is") and had to produce the result ("eleven") themselves. Participants were encouraged to look at the relevant numbers throughout the trial. Their eye movements were recorded and analyzed. We found that the participants' eye movements in the two tasks were overall very similar. They fixated the first and second number of the equations shortly after they were mentioned, and fixated the result number well before they named it on production trials and well before the recorded speaker named it on comprehension trials. However, all fixation latencies were shorter on production than on comprehension trials. These findings suggest that the processes involved in planning to say a word and anticipating hearing a word are quite similar, but that people are more aroused or engaged when they intend to respond than when they merely listen to another person.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Florian Hintz
- Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen, The Netherlands; International Max Planck Research School for Language Sciences, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | - Antje S Meyer
- Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen, The Netherlands; Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition, and Behavior, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
39
|
Levinson SC, Torreira F. Timing in turn-taking and its implications for processing models of language. Front Psychol 2015; 6:731. [PMID: 26124727 PMCID: PMC4464110 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00731] [Citation(s) in RCA: 151] [Impact Index Per Article: 15.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/28/2015] [Accepted: 05/16/2015] [Indexed: 12/03/2022] Open
Abstract
The core niche for language use is in verbal interaction, involving the rapid exchange of turns at talking. This paper reviews the extensive literature about this system, adding new statistical analyses of behavioral data where they have been missing, demonstrating that turn-taking has the systematic properties originally noted by Sacks et al. (1974; hereafter SSJ). This system poses some significant puzzles for current theories of language processing: the gaps between turns are short (of the order of 200 ms), but the latencies involved in language production are much longer (over 600 ms). This seems to imply that participants in conversation must predict (or 'project' as SSJ have it) the end of the current speaker's turn in order to prepare their response in advance. This in turn implies some overlap between production and comprehension despite their use of common processing resources. Collecting together what is known behaviorally and experimentally about the system, the space for systematic explanations of language processing for conversation can be significantly narrowed, and we sketch some first model of the mental processes involved for the participant preparing to speak next.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Stephen C. Levinson
- Language and Cognition Department, Max Planck Institute for PsycholinguisticsNijmegen, Netherlands
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud UniversityNijmegen, Netherlands
| | - Francisco Torreira
- Language and Cognition Department, Max Planck Institute for PsycholinguisticsNijmegen, Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
40
|
Garrod S, Pickering MJ. The use of content and timing to predict turn transitions. Front Psychol 2015; 6:751. [PMID: 26124728 PMCID: PMC4463931 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00751] [Citation(s) in RCA: 32] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/29/2014] [Accepted: 05/20/2015] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
For addressees to respond in a timely fashion, they cannot simply process the speaker's utterance as it occurs and wait till it finishes. Instead, they predict both when the speaker will conclude and what linguistic forms will be used. While doing this, they must also prepare their own response. To explain this, we draw on the account proposed by Pickering and Garrod (2013a), in which addressees covertly imitate the speaker's utterance and use this to determine the intention that underlies their upcoming utterance. They use this intention to predict when and how the utterance will end, and also to drive their own production mechanisms for preparing their response. Following Arnal and Giraud (2012), we distinguish between mechanisms that predict timing and content. In particular, we propose that the timing mechanism relies on entrainment of low-frequency oscillations between speech envelope and brain. This constrains the context that feeds into the determination of the speaker's intention and hence the timing and form of the upcoming utterance. This approach typically leads to well-timed contributions, but also provides a mechanism for resolving conflicts, for example when there is unintended speaker overlap.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Simon Garrod
- Institute of Neuroscience and Psychology, University of Glasgow Glasgow, UK
| | | |
Collapse
|
41
|
Kendrick KH. The intersection of turn-taking and repair: the timing of other-initiations of repair in conversation. Front Psychol 2015; 6:250. [PMID: 25814968 PMCID: PMC4357221 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00250] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/16/2014] [Accepted: 02/18/2015] [Indexed: 11/16/2022] Open
Abstract
The transitions between turns at talk in conversation tend to occur quickly, with only a slight gap of ∼100–300 ms between them. This estimate of central tendency, however, hides a wealth of complex variation, as a number of factors, such as the type of turns involved, have been shown to influence the timing of turn transitions. This article considers one specific type of turn that does not conform to the statistical trend, namely turns that deal with troubles of speaking, hearing, and understanding, known as other-initiations of repair (OIR). The results of a quantitative analysis of 169 OIRs in face-to-face conversation reveal that the most frequent cases occur after gaps of ∼700 ms. Furthermore, OIRs that locate a source of trouble in a prior turn specifically tend to occur after shorter gaps than those that do not, and those that correct errors in a prior turn, while rare, tend to occur without delay. An analysis of the transitions before OIRs, using methods of conversation analysis, suggests that speakers use the extra time (i) to search for a late recognition of the problematic turn, (ii) to provide an opportunity for the speaker of the problematic turn to resolve the trouble independently, and (iii) to produce visual signals, such as facial gestures. In light of these results, it is argued that OIRs take priority over other turns at talk in conversation and therefore are not subject to the same rules and constraints that motivate fast turn transitions in general.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kobin H Kendrick
- Language and Cognition Department, Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|