1
|
Chen Y, Blumstein DT, Boyle DM, Bartolotta N, Brown J, Kissui B, Waltert M, Kiffner C. Zebra Risk Perception in a Landscape of Fear. Ecol Evol 2025; 15:e71275. [PMID: 40370349 PMCID: PMC12077961 DOI: 10.1002/ece3.71275] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/20/2024] [Revised: 03/24/2025] [Accepted: 03/31/2025] [Indexed: 05/16/2025] Open
Abstract
Animals' assessments of predation risk are influenced by a variety of external and internal factors, including predator space use. However, it remains unclear what variables mediate prey species behavior within a landscape where predation risk is heterogeneous. To address this, we employed three assays to examine zebra (Equus quagga) responses to varying predation risk in a multiple-use area of northern Tanzania: (1) quantifying head-up posture as a proxy for vigilance through direct behavioral observation in areas of high and low likelihood of lion (Panthera leo) presence, (2) quantifying head-up posture as a proxy for vigilance when exposed to a lion roar playback, and (3) measuring flight initiation distances (FIDs) when approached by a person. Using generalized linear (mixed) models, we tested how lion space use and habitat type (as proxies for predation risk), normalized difference vegetation index (NDVI, as proxy for primary productivity), time of the day, and zebra-related variables (sex-age category, zebra herd size, group size including heterospecifics, and location within the herd) influenced vigilance and flight responses. We found that (1) neither vigilance nor FID were markedly influenced by estimated lion space use, habitat type, and NDVI; (2) vigilance decreased with group size, was lower for zebras positioned centrally in the herd, and during midday; (3) FID increased with a greater number of associated heterospecifics; and (4) zebras increased vigilance when exposed to lion roar playbacks, irrespective of lion space use. These findings suggest that zebra vigilance and flight behavior are not necessarily mediated by spatial variation in apparent predation risk but instead reflect a strategy of maintaining a consistent monitoring of possible threats across the landscape. Rather than relying on spatial clues alone, zebras primarily mitigate predation risk by increasing group size and associating with other species.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yuchen Chen
- Department of Ecology and Evolutionary BiologyUniversity of California Los AngelesLos AngelesCaliforniaUSA
| | - Daniel T. Blumstein
- Department of Ecology and Evolutionary BiologyUniversity of California Los AngelesLos AngelesCaliforniaUSA
| | - Diana M. Boyle
- Department of Conservation BiologyGeorg‐August Universität GöttingenGöttingenGermany
| | | | | | - Bernard Kissui
- Center for Wildlife Management StudiesThe School for Field StudiesKaratuTanzania
| | - Matthias Waltert
- Department of Conservation BiologyGeorg‐August Universität GöttingenGöttingenGermany
| | - Christian Kiffner
- Center for Wildlife Management StudiesThe School for Field StudiesKaratuTanzania
- Research Area 2 ‘Land Use and Governance’Leibniz Centre for Agricultural Landscape Research (ZALF)MünchebergGermany
- Thaer‐Institute of Agricultural and Horticultural SciencesHumboldt‐Universität zu BerlinBerlinGermany
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Owen‐Smith N. Reconciling Species Concepts: An Ecological Perspective. AMERICAN JOURNAL OF BIOLOGICAL ANTHROPOLOGY 2025; 186:e70047. [PMID: 40214099 PMCID: PMC11987482 DOI: 10.1002/ajpa.70047] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/20/2024] [Revised: 03/20/2025] [Accepted: 04/01/2025] [Indexed: 04/14/2025]
Abstract
Species concepts remain contentious, both in paleoanthropology and in modern taxonomy. The lineage-based concept emphasizing evolutionary independence is most fundamental, but in practice is generally represented by proxy evidence of morphological or genetic divergence. This has resulted in a troubling proliferation of species names in the hominin fossil record. Pseudo-extinctions where lineages persisted under a new species name need to be distinguished from cases where lineages ended terminally-the implications for ecological adaptability are diametrically opposed. Furthermore, the ecological criterion for species coexistence is widely overlooked. The competitive exclusion principle holds that species sharing closely similar niches cannot continue to coexist in the same place at the same time. Notably, the largely vegetarian Paranthropus lineage remained distinct from the diverging, more versatile Homo lineage until fading from the fossil record during the later Pleistocene. Claims that additional hominin species existed are ecologically suspect unless supported by evidence of adequate niche separation. Modern examples where there has been equivocation in lineage recognition are illustrated for bovids, giraffids, baboons, and elephants. Furthermore, the mechanisms that resulted in the displacement of Neanderthals by modern humans are reappraised from an ecological perspective. Representations of evolutionary divergence as a bushy tree need to be superseded by the emerging paradigm of reticulate lineages diverging and coalescing through time and space.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Norman Owen‐Smith
- Centre for African Ecology, School of Animal, Plant and Environmental Sciences, University of the WitwatersrandJohannesburgSouth Africa
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Krivopalova A, Mikula P, Cukor J, Ševčík R, Brynychová K, Šálek M. Adaptation of farmland mammalian specialist to urban life: Escape behavior of European hare along the urban-rural gradient. THE SCIENCE OF THE TOTAL ENVIRONMENT 2024; 951:175779. [PMID: 39191323 DOI: 10.1016/j.scitotenv.2024.175779] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/13/2024] [Revised: 08/18/2024] [Accepted: 08/23/2024] [Indexed: 08/29/2024]
Abstract
The European hare Lepus europaeus is an iconic but rapidly declining farmland specialist with recently confirmed populations in urban areas. However, their behavioral responses and adaptability to urbanization and life in human-dominated areas are fully unexplored. Here, using infrared cameras, we explored escape behavior (measured as the flight initiation distance) using 965 hare observations in urban and farmland areas in the Czech Republic and Austria (Central Europe) and its association with habitat type, distance to the city center, patch size, season, hare age and initial behavior. We found that European hares adjusted their escape behavior to habitat type and escaped significantly earlier in farmland (rural) habitats than in urban habitats. However, escape distances of hares did not differ between farmland types with different degree of habitat heterogeneity. We also revealed that urban hares escaped earlier when located further from the city center or in a larger study patch. Moreover, adult hares escaped earlier than subadults and foraging individuals escaped earlier than resting hares but both only in rural areas. Our results support high behavioral adaptability of European hares to human-dominated urban areas.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Aleksandra Krivopalova
- Faculty of Forestry and Wood Sciences, Czech University of Life Sciences Prague, Kamýcká 129, 16500 Prague, Czech Republic; Forestry and Game Management Research Institute, Strnady 136, 25202 Jíloviště, Czech Republic
| | - Peter Mikula
- Faculty of Environmental Sciences, Czech University of Life Sciences Prague, Kamýcká 129, 16500 Prague, Czech Republic; TUM School of Life Sciences, Ecoclimatology, Technical University of Munich, Hans-Carl-von-Carlowitz-Platz 2, 85354 Freising, Germany; Institute for Advanced Study, Technical University of Munich, Lichtenbergstraße 2a, 85748 Garching, Germany
| | - Jan Cukor
- Faculty of Forestry and Wood Sciences, Czech University of Life Sciences Prague, Kamýcká 129, 16500 Prague, Czech Republic; Forestry and Game Management Research Institute, Strnady 136, 25202 Jíloviště, Czech Republic.
| | - Richard Ševčík
- Forestry and Game Management Research Institute, Strnady 136, 25202 Jíloviště, Czech Republic
| | - Kateřina Brynychová
- Forestry and Game Management Research Institute, Strnady 136, 25202 Jíloviště, Czech Republic
| | - Martin Šálek
- Forestry and Game Management Research Institute, Strnady 136, 25202 Jíloviště, Czech Republic; Faculty of Environmental Sciences, Czech University of Life Sciences Prague, Kamýcká 129, 16500 Prague, Czech Republic; Czech Academy of Sciences, Institute of Vertebrate Biology, Květná 8, 60365 Brno, Czech Republic
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Zeller KA, Ditmer MA, Squires JR, Rice WL, Wilder J, DeLong D, Egan A, Pennington N, Wang CA, Plucinski J, Barber JR. Experimental recreationist noise alters behavior and space use of wildlife. Curr Biol 2024; 34:2997-3004.e3. [PMID: 38876101 DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2024.05.030] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/04/2024] [Revised: 04/13/2024] [Accepted: 05/15/2024] [Indexed: 06/16/2024]
Abstract
Providing outdoor recreational opportunities to people and protecting wildlife are dual goals of many land managers. However, recreation is associated with negative effects on wildlife, ranging from increased stress hormones1,2 to shifts in habitat use3,4,5 to lowered reproductive success.6,7 Noise from recreational activities can be far reaching and have similar negative effects on wildlife, yet the impacts of these auditory encounters are less studied and are often unobservable. We designed a field-based experiment to both isolate and quantify the effects of recreation noise on several mammal species and test the effects of different recreation types and group sizes. Animals entering our sampling arrays triggered cameras to record video and broadcast recreation noise from speakers ∼20 m away. Our design allowed us to observe and classify behaviors of wildlife as they were exposed to acoustic stimuli. We found wildlife were 3.1-4.7 times more likely to flee and were vigilant for 2.2-3.0 times longer upon hearing recreation noise compared with controls (natural sounds and no noise). Wildlife abundance at our sampling arrays was 1.5 times lower the week following recreation noise deployments. Noise from larger groups of vocal hikers and mountain bikers caused the highest probability of fleeing (6-8 times more likely to flee). Elk were the most sensitive species to recreation noise, and large carnivores were the least sensitive. Our findings indicate that recreation noise alone caused anti-predator responses in wildlife, and as outdoor recreation continues to increase in popularity and geographic extent,8,9 noise from recreation may result in degraded or indirect wildlife habitat loss.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Katherine A Zeller
- USDA Forest Service, Rocky Mountain Research Station, Aldo Leopold Wilderness Research Institute, 790 E. Beckwith Avenue, Missoula, MT 59801, USA.
| | - Mark A Ditmer
- USDA Forest Service, Rocky Mountain Research Station, 240 Prospect Road, Fort Collins, CO 80526, USA; Department of Fish, Wildlife, and Conservation Biology, Colorado State University, 400 University Avenue, Fort Collins, CO 80523, USA
| | - John R Squires
- USDA Forest Service, Rocky Mountain Research Station, 800 E. Beckwith Avenue, Missoula, MT 59801, USA
| | - William L Rice
- Parks, Tourism, and Recreation Management Program, University of Montana, 32 Campus Drive, Missoula, MT 59812, USA
| | - James Wilder
- USDA Forest Service, Bridger-Teton National Forest, 390 N. Cache, Jackson, WY 83001, USA
| | - Don DeLong
- USDA Forest Service, Bridger-Teton National Forest, 671 Washington Street, Afton, WY 83110, USA
| | - Ashley Egan
- USDA Forest Service, Bridger-Teton National Forest, 390 N. Cache, Jackson, WY 83001, USA
| | - Niah Pennington
- Warner College of Natural Resources, Colorado State University, 400 University Avenue, Fort Collins, CO 80523, USA
| | - Chris A Wang
- Freaklabs, 40 Kozuka, Kamogawa, Chiba 296-0233, Japan
| | | | - Jesse R Barber
- Department of Biological Sciences, Boise State University, 1910 University Drive, Boise, ID 83725, USA
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
McGann K, Johnson CN, Clinchy M, Zanette LY, Cunningham CX. Fear of the human 'super predator' in native marsupials and introduced deer in Australia. Proc Biol Sci 2024; 291:20232849. [PMID: 38775542 DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2023.2849] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/01/2023] [Accepted: 03/29/2024] [Indexed: 07/31/2024] Open
Abstract
Recent experiments have demonstrated that carnivores and ungulates in Africa, Asia, Europe and North America fear the human 'super predator' far more than other predators. Australian mammals have been a focus of research on predator naiveté because it is suspected they show atypical antipredator responses. To experimentally test if mammals in Australia also most fear humans, we quantified the responses of four native marsupials (eastern grey kangaroo, Bennett's wallaby, Tasmanian pademelon, common brushtail possum) and introduced fallow deer to playbacks of predator (human, dog, Tasmanian devil, wolf) or non-predator control (sheep) vocalizations. Native marsupials most feared the human 'super predator', fleeing humans 2.4 times more often than the next most frightening predator (dogs), and being most, and significantly, vigilant to humans. These results demonstrate that native marsupials are not naïve to the peril humans pose, substantially expanding the taxonomic and geographic scope of the growing experimental evidence that wildlife worldwide generally perceive humans as the planet's most frightening predator. Introduced fallow deer fled humans, but not more than other predators, which we suggest may result from their being introduced. Our results point to both challenges concerning marsupial conservation and opportunities for exploiting fear of humans as a wildlife management tool.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Katherine McGann
- School of Natural Resources, University of Tasmania, Private Bag 55 , Hobart, Tasmania 7001, Australia
| | - Christopher N Johnson
- School of Natural Resources, University of Tasmania, Private Bag 55 , Hobart, Tasmania 7001, Australia
| | - Michael Clinchy
- Department of Biology, Western University , London, Ontario N6A 5B7, Canada
| | - Liana Y Zanette
- Department of Biology, Western University , London, Ontario N6A 5B7, Canada
| | - Calum X Cunningham
- School of Natural Resources, University of Tasmania, Private Bag 55 , Hobart, Tasmania 7001, Australia
- School of Environmental and Forest Sciences, College of the Environment, University of Washington , Seattle, WA 98195-2100, USA
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Spottiswoode CN, Wood BM. Culturally determined interspecies communication between humans and honeyguides. Science 2023; 382:1155-1158. [PMID: 38060656 DOI: 10.1126/science.adh4129] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/02/2023] [Accepted: 10/25/2023] [Indexed: 12/18/2023]
Abstract
Species interactions that vary across environments can create geographical mosaics of genetic coevolution. However, traits mediating species interactions are sometimes culturally inherited. Here we show that traditions of interspecies communication between people and wild birds vary in a culturally determined geographical mosaic. Honey hunters in different parts of Africa use different calls to communicate with greater honeyguides (Indicator indicator) that lead them to bees' nests. We show experimentally that honeyguides in Tanzania and Mozambique discriminate among honey hunters' calls, responding more readily to local than to foreign calls. This was not explained by variation in sound transmission and instead suggests that honeyguides learn local human signals. We discuss the forces stabilizing and diversifying interspecies communication traditions, and the potential for cultural coevolution between species.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Claire N Spottiswoode
- Department of Zoology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
- FitzPatrick Institute of African Ornithology, Department of Biological Sciences, University of Cape Town, Cape Town, South Africa
| | - Brian M Wood
- Department of Anthropology, University of California Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, USA
- Department of Human Behavior, Ecology, and Culture, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Feistel R. Self-Organisation of Prediction Models. ENTROPY (BASEL, SWITZERLAND) 2023; 25:1596. [PMID: 38136476 PMCID: PMC10743227 DOI: 10.3390/e25121596] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/29/2023] [Revised: 11/21/2023] [Accepted: 11/24/2023] [Indexed: 12/24/2023]
Abstract
Living organisms are active open systems far from thermodynamic equilibrium. The ability to behave actively corresponds to dynamical metastability: minor but supercritical internal or external effects may trigger major substantial actions such as gross mechanical motion, dissipating internally accumulated energy reserves. Gaining a selective advantage from the beneficial use of activity requires a consistent combination of sensual perception, memorised experience, statistical or causal prediction models, and the resulting favourable decisions on actions. This information processing chain originated from mere physical interaction processes prior to life, here denoted as structural information exchange. From there, the self-organised transition to symbolic information processing marks the beginning of life, evolving through the novel purposivity of trial-and-error feedback and the accumulation of symbolic information. The emergence of symbols and prediction models can be described as a ritualisation transition, a symmetry-breaking kinetic phase transition of the second kind previously known from behavioural biology. The related new symmetry is the neutrally stable arbitrariness, conventionality, or code invariance of symbols with respect to their meaning. The meaning of such symbols is given by the structural effect they ultimately unleash, directly or indirectly, by deciding on which actions to take. The early genetic code represents the first symbols. The genetically inherited symbolic information is the first prediction model for activities sufficient for survival under the condition of environmental continuity, sometimes understood as the "final causality" property of the model.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rainer Feistel
- Leibniz Institute for Baltic Sea Research (IOW), 18119 Rostock, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Shrader AM. Ecology: Humans are scarier than lions. Curr Biol 2023; 33:R1158-R1160. [PMID: 37935131 DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2023.09.059] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2023]
Abstract
In Africa, nothing inspires fear more than lions. They are large, hunt in groups and kill prey much larger than themselves. Yet, evidence suggests that African wildlife are more afraid of humans than anything else that moves across the savanna.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Adrian M Shrader
- Mammal Research Institute, Department of Zoology and Entomology, University of Pretoria, Pretoria, South Africa.
| |
Collapse
|