1
|
Wang L, Pfordresher PQ, Jiang C, Liu F. Atypical vocal imitation of speech and song in autism spectrum disorder: Evidence from Mandarin speakers. AUTISM : THE INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF RESEARCH AND PRACTICE 2025; 29:408-423. [PMID: 39239838 PMCID: PMC11816480 DOI: 10.1177/13623613241275395] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 09/07/2024]
Abstract
LAY ABSTRACT Atypical vocal imitation has been identified in English-speaking autistic individuals, whereas the characteristics of vocal imitation in tone-language-speaking autistic individuals remain unexplored. By comparing speech and song imitation, the present study reveals a unique pattern of atypical vocal imitation across speech and music domains among Mandarin-speaking autistic individuals. The findings suggest that tone language experience does not compensate for difficulties in vocal imitation in autistic individuals and extends our understanding of vocal imitation in autism across different languages.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Li Wang
- The Chinese University of Hong Kong, China
- University of Reading, UK
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
2
|
Verde-Cagiao M, Nieto C, Campos R. Mother-infant co-regulation from 0 to 2 years: The role of copy behaviors. A systematic review. Infant Behav Dev 2022; 68:101749. [PMID: 35952425 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2022.101749] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/21/2021] [Revised: 04/19/2022] [Accepted: 07/16/2022] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
The purpose of this review was twofold: (1) to examine how copy behaviors (CB) have been studied in mother-infant natural interactions from 0 to 24 months, and (2) to determine to what extent they can be considered co-regulation processes between both members of the dyad. To do this, 36 studies published between 1975 and 2021 were systematically examined, classified and discussed. The analyzed evidence showed that CB in spontaneous mother-infant interactions have been examined under different perspectives, that such behaviors might be differentially classified as distinct modes of copying according to their traits and, lastly, that CB operate as social facilitators that foster the co-regulation of both affects and behaviors and direct mothers and infants, most of the times, towards a mutual sense of interpersonal matching that adds quality to their interactions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Carmen Nieto
- Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, Madrid, Spain
| | - Ruth Campos
- Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, Madrid, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Kondaurova MV, Zheng Q, VanDam M, Kinney K. Vocal Turn-Taking in Families With Children With and Without Hearing Loss. Ear Hear 2022; 43:883-898. [PMID: 34619686 PMCID: PMC8983796 DOI: 10.1097/aud.0000000000001135] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE Vocal turn-taking is an important predictor of language development in children with and without hearing loss. Most studies have examined vocal turn-taking in mother-child dyads without considering the multitalker context in a child's life. The present study investigates the quantity of vocal turns between deaf and hard-of-hearing children and multiple members of their social environment. DESIGN Participants were 52 families with children who used hearing aids (HA, mean age 26.3 mo) or cochlear implants (CI, mean age 63.2 mo) and 27 families with normal-hearing (NH, mean age 26.6 mo) children. The Language ENvironment Analysis system estimated the number of conversational turns per hour (CTC/hr) between all family members (i.e., adult female, adult male, target child, and other child) during full-day recordings over a period of about 1 year. RESULTS The CTC/hr was lower between the target child and the adult female or adult male in the CI compared with the HA and NH groups. Initially, CTC/hr was higher between the target child and the adult female than between the adult male or the other child. As the child's age increased, turn-taking between the target child and the adult female increased in comparison to that between the target child and the adult male. Over time, turn-taking between the target child and the other child increased and exceeded turn-taking between the target child and the adult caregivers. The increase was observed earlier in families with siblings compared with those without. CONCLUSIONS The quantity of vocal turn-taking depends on the degree of child hearing loss and the relationship between the children and the members of their social environment. Longitudinally, the positive effect of an assistive device on the quantity of turns between the children and their family members was found. The effect was stronger in families with siblings.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Maria V Kondaurova
- Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, University of Louisville, Louisville, Kentucky, USA
| | - Qi Zheng
- Department of Bioinformatics & Biostatistics, University of Louisville, Louisville, Kentucky, USA
| | - Mark VanDam
- Department of Speech and Hearing Sciences, Elson S. Floyd College of Medicine, Washington State University, Hearing Oral Program of Excellence (HOPE School) of Spokane, Spokane, Washington, USA
| | - Kaelin Kinney
- Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, University of Louisville, Louisville, Kentucky, USA
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Serrat-Sellabona E, Aguilar-Mediavilla E, Sanz-Torrent M, Andreu L, Amadó A, Serra M. Sociodemographic and Pre-Linguistic Factors in Early Vocabulary Acquisition. CHILDREN (BASEL, SWITZERLAND) 2021; 8:206. [PMID: 33803169 PMCID: PMC8001358 DOI: 10.3390/children8030206] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/29/2020] [Revised: 02/27/2021] [Accepted: 03/03/2021] [Indexed: 12/17/2022]
Abstract
Here, we studied the beginnings of language development, jointly assessing two groups of precursors, sociodemographic and pre-linguistic, that have previously been studied separately. Thus, the general objective of this study was to explore which factors best explained the acquisition of initial expressive vocabulary. The sample consisted of 504 participants from Catalan-speaking homes with ages ranging between 10 and 18 months. The data were obtained through the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories (MCB-CDIs). Vocabulary development shows a lexical spurt at 17 months. Regression analyses show that pre-linguistic factors have more explanatory power of than sociodemographic ones. Within the sociodemographic variables, age, birth order and birth weight explain part of the vocabulary variance. With respect to pre-linguistic variables, imitation, late gestures and phrase comprehension are predictors of the initial vocabulary acquisition. Specifically, imitation and late gestures were the pre-linguistic behaviours that made it possible to distinguish between children with higher and lower levels of vocabulary. We discussed these findings in relation to their relevance for language acquisition and for the early assessment of linguistic competence.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Eva Aguilar-Mediavilla
- Applied Pedagogy and Educational Psychology, Institute of Research and Innovation in Education (IRIE), Universitat de les Illes Balears, 07122 Palma, Spain
| | - Mònica Sanz-Torrent
- Psychology Faculty, Universitat de Barcelona, 08035 Barcelona, Spain; (M.S.-T.); (M.S.)
| | - Llorenç Andreu
- Psychology and Education Science Studies, Universitat Oberta de Catalunya, 08018 Barcelona, Spain;
| | - Anna Amadó
- Psychology Department, Universitat de Girona, 17004 Girona, Spain;
| | - Miquel Serra
- Psychology Faculty, Universitat de Barcelona, 08035 Barcelona, Spain; (M.S.-T.); (M.S.)
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Kondaurova MV, Fagan MK, Zheng Q. Vocal imitation between mothers and their children with cochlear implants. INFANCY 2020; 25:827-850. [DOI: 10.1111/infa.12363] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/12/2019] [Revised: 06/12/2020] [Accepted: 07/13/2020] [Indexed: 01/11/2023]
Affiliation(s)
- Maria V. Kondaurova
- Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences University of Louisville Louisville KY USA
| | - Mary K. Fagan
- Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders Chapman University Orange CA USA
| | - Qi Zheng
- Department of Bioinformatics & Biostatistics University of Louisville Louisville KY USA
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Laubscher E, Light J. Core vocabulary lists for young children and considerations for early language development: a narrative review. Augment Altern Commun 2020; 36:43-53. [PMID: 32172598 DOI: 10.1080/07434618.2020.1737964] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/24/2022] Open
Abstract
For early symbolic communicators, acquisition of an initial vocabulary is a critically important achievement that sets the stage for future language development. Children who require augmentative and alternative communication (AAC) rely on others to select and provide these important first words for them. One resource to help guide this process includes published lists of words that may contribute to a core vocabulary for individuals who require AAC. Although some clinicians report that they consider or prioritize core words during the vocabulary selection process, it is not known whether an emphasis on core words will best meet the expressive vocabulary needs of early symbolic communicators. The purposes of this narrative review were to (a) review studies that have developed word lists to inform selection of a core vocabulary for young children who require AAC, (b) compare the words on these lists to the early words used by children with typical development, and (c) consider the implications for vocabulary selection and language development for early symbolic communicators who require AAC. Results suggest that core word lists may under-emphasize many of the types of words that predominate in early expressive vocabulary; these lists may not be the most appropriate resources to guide AAC system design and instruction for early symbolic communicators.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Emily Laubscher
- Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, USA
| | - Janice Light
- Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Increasing Vocalizations and Echoics in Infants at Risk of Autism Spectrum Disorder. Behav Anal Pract 2020; 13:467-472. [PMID: 32647604 DOI: 10.1007/s40617-020-00413-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/24/2022] Open
Abstract
Infant siblings of children with an autism spectrum disorder (ASD) diagnosis (i.e., infants at risk of ASD) are excellent candidates for early interventions based on the principles of applied behavior analysis. This study replicates and extends behavioral research using contingent social reinforcement procedures (i.e., vocal imitation and motherese speech) to increase both vocalizations and echoics among 3 infants at risk of ASD with their mothers in the natural environment. Results confirmed earlier findings that contingent reinforcement, specifically vocal imitation, reliably produces high rates of vocalizations, echoic approximations, and emerging pure echoic repertoires in at risk infants.
Collapse
|
8
|
Stenberg G. Does contingency in adults' responding influence 12-month-old infants' social referencing? Infant Behav Dev 2017; 49:9-20. [PMID: 28686883 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2017.06.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/24/2015] [Revised: 04/26/2016] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
Abstract
In two experiments we examined the influence of contingent versus non-contingent responding on infant social referencing behavior. EXPERIMENT 1: Forty 12-month-old infants were exposed to an ambiguous toy in a social referencing situation. In one condition an unfamiliar adult who in a previous play situation had responded contingently to the infant's looks gave the infant positive information about the toy. In the other condition an unfamiliar adult who previously had not responded contingently delivered the positive information. EXPERIMENT 2: Forty-eight 12-month-old infants participated in Experiment 2. In this experiment it was examined whether the familiarity of the adult influences infants' reactions to contingency in responding. In one condition a parent who previously had responded contingently to the infant's looks provided positive information about the ambiguous toy, and in the other condition a parent who previously had not responded contingently provided the positive information. The infants looked more at the contingent experimenter in Experimenter 1, and also played more with the toy after receiving positive information from the contingent experimenter. No differences in looking at the parent and in playing with the toy were found in Experiment 2. The results indicate that contingency in responding, as well as the familiarity of the adult, influence infants' social referencing behavior.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gunilla Stenberg
- Psychology, Uppsala University, Box 1225, 751 42 Uppsala, Sweden.
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Miller JL, Gros-Louis J. The Effect of Social Responsiveness on Infants' Object-Directed Imitation. INFANCY 2017; 22:344-361. [PMID: 33158357 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12156] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
Previous research has demonstrated that social interactions underlie the development of object-directed imitation. For example, infants differentially learn object action sequences from a live social partner compared to a social partner over a video monitor; however, what is not well understood is what aspects of social interactions influence social learning. Previous studies have found variable influences of different types of caregiver responsiveness on attention, language, and cognitive development. Therefore, the purpose of this study was to examine how the responsive style of a social partner influenced the learning of object-directed action sequences. Infants interacted with either a sensitive or redirective experimenter before the learning trial. Results revealed infants changed their patterns of engagement; infants interacting with a sensitive experimenter had longer periods of attentional engagement than infants interacting with a redirective experimenter. Furthermore, during the learning trial, the amount of sensitivity during interaction with the social partner predicted learning scores. These findings suggest that infants' attention is influenced by social partners' interactive style during ongoing interaction, which subsequently affects how infants learn from these social partners.
Collapse
|
10
|
Stenberg G. Does contingency in adults' responding influence 12-month-old infants' social referencing? Infant Behav Dev 2016; 46:67-79. [PMID: 27940087 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2016.11.013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/29/2016] [Accepted: 11/30/2016] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
Abstract
In two experiments we examined the influence of contingent versus non-contingent responding on infant social referencing behavior. EXPERIMENT 1: Forty 12-month-old infants were exposed to an ambiguous toy in a social referencing situation. In one condition an unfamiliar adult who in a previous play situation had responded contingently to the infant's looks gave the infant positive information about the toy. In the other condition an unfamiliar adult who previously had not responded contingently delivered the positive information. EXPERIMENT 2: Forty-eight 12-month-old infants participated in Experiment 2. In this experiment it was examined whether the familiarity of the adult influences infants' reactions to contingency in responding. In one condition a parent who previously had responded contingently to the infant's looks provided positive information about the ambiguous toy, and in the other condition a parent who previously had not responded contingently provided the positive information. The infants looked more at the contingent experimenter in Experimenter 1, and also played more with the toy after receiving positive information from the contingent experimenter. No differences in looking at the parent and in playing with the toy were found in Experiment 2. The results indicate that contingency in responding, as well as the familiarity of the adult, influence infants' social referencing behavior.
Collapse
|
11
|
Ishizuka Y, Yamamoto JI. Contingent imitation increases verbal interaction in children with autism spectrum disorders. AUTISM : THE INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF RESEARCH AND PRACTICE 2016; 20:1011-1020. [PMID: 27095832 DOI: 10.1177/1362361315622856] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Several studies have suggested that contingent adult imitation increase nonverbal communication, such as attention and proximity to adults, in children with autism spectrum disorders. However, few studies have shown the effect of contingent imitation on verbal communication. This study examined whether children with autism were able to promote verbal interaction such as vocal imitation, vocalization, and vocal turn-taking via contingent imitation. We used an alternating treatment design composed of the conditions of contingent imitation and control for six children with autism (aged 33-63 months). For contingent imitation condition, adults imitated children's vocalization immediately. For control condition, adults did not imitate but gave a vocal response immediately. Results showed that in contingent imitation condition, all children increased the number of vocal imitations and vocal turn-takings compared with control condition. The number of vocalizations increased in both condition for all children. Overall, it is suggested that all children promote verbal interaction via contingent imitation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yuka Ishizuka
- Keio University, Japan Japan Society for the Promotion of Science, Japan
| | | |
Collapse
|
12
|
|
13
|
Brink KA, Lane JD, Wellman HM. Developmental pathways for social understanding: linking social cognition to social contexts. Front Psychol 2015; 6:719. [PMID: 26074859 PMCID: PMC4447997 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00719] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/04/2014] [Accepted: 05/14/2015] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Contemporary research, often with looking-time tasks, reveals that infants possess foundational understandings of their social worlds. However, few studies have examined how these early social cognitions relate to the child’s social interactions and behavior in early development. Does an early understanding of the social world relate to how an infant interacts with his or her parents? Do early social interactions along with social-cognitive understandings in infancy predict later preschool social competencies? In the current paper, we propose a theory in which children’s later social behaviors and their understanding of the social world depend on the integration of early social understanding and experiences in infancy. We review several of our studies, as well as other research, that directly examine the pathways between these competencies to support a hypothesized network of relations between social-cognitive development and social-interactive behaviors in the development from infancy to childhood. In total, these findings reveal differences in infant social competences that both track the developmental trajectory of infants’ understanding of people over the first years of life and provide external validation for the large body of social-cognitive findings emerging from laboratory looking-time paradigms.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kimberly A Brink
- Department of Psychology, University of Michigan , Ann Arbor, MI, USA
| | - Jonathan D Lane
- Peabody College of Education and Human Development, Vanderbilt University , Nashville, TN, USA
| | - Henry M Wellman
- Department of Psychology, University of Michigan , Ann Arbor, MI, USA
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
The Effects of Contingent Caregiver Imitation of Infant Vocalizations: a Comparison of Multiple Caregivers. Anal Verbal Behav 2014; 30:20-8. [PMID: 27274970 DOI: 10.1007/s40616-014-0008-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/25/2022] Open
Abstract
The present study replicated and extended the Pelaez et al. (Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis 44:33-40, 2011) study, which examined the reinforcing effects of mothers' contingent imitation of their infants' vocalizations. Three infants aged 7-12 months who could vocalize sounds but not words participated with two caregivers for each infant (i.e., triads). During the intervention phase, the caregivers were asked to immediately imitate all vocalizations emitted by the child for a 3-min period. During the yoked control phase, the caregivers listened to an audio recording from the preceding condition and provided vocalizations non-contingently on the infants' responses. The procedures yielded different results across participants; one infant emitted a higher frequency of vocalizations during the contingent imitation phases over the control phases, and the other two infants showed higher rates of responding during the control phases. However, all infants emitted more imitative return vocalizations during contingent reinforcement conditions compared with the yoked control condition.
Collapse
|
15
|
Dunphy-Lelii S, Labounty J, Lane JD, Wellman HM. The Social Context of Infant Intention Understanding. JOURNAL OF COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT 2014; 15:60-77. [PMID: 24778577 DOI: 10.1080/15248372.2012.710863] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
Abstract
Traditional looking-time paradigms are often used to assess infants' attention to socio-cognitive phenomena, but the link between these laboratory scenarios and real-world interactions is unclear. The current study investigated hypothesized relations between traditional social-cognitive looking-time paradigms and their real-world counterparts in caregiver-infant social interaction. Seventy-five 10- to 12-month-old infants participated in a structured play session with their caregiver, as well as a traditional looking-time paradigm targeting intentional action. Infants' ability to quickly parse intentional displays correlated with several key qualities of their everyday interactions. In particular, caregiver and infant interaction quality, maternal supportiveness, caregiver and infant joint engagement skill, and social attentiveness in infants correlated with faster habituation to looking-time displays. These results support a linkage between social-cognitive looking-time laboratory paradigms and more naturalistic partner interaction, at this key age. The data both provide external validation for the large body of social-cognitive findings emerging from laboratory looking-time paradigms, and contribute to a growing literature tracking the developmental trajectory of infants' understanding of people over the first two years.
Collapse
|
16
|
Olson J, Masur EF. Mothers respond differently to infants' familiar versus non-familiar verbal imitations. JOURNAL OF CHILD LANGUAGE 2012; 39:731-752. [PMID: 21910953 DOI: 10.1017/s0305000911000262] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/31/2023]
Abstract
Mothers' verbal responses to their infants' spontaneous imitations of familiar and non-familiar words during naturally occurring interactions were examined in a longitudinal sample observed at 1 ; 1, 1 ; 5 and 1 ; 9. Maternal responses to both familiar and non-familiar imitations exhibited structural characteristics likely to be facilitative of early word learning, including shorter and single-word utterances and reproductions of imitated words in sentence-final position. Mothers also responded differentially to infants' non-familiar versus familiar imitations. Mothers produced more return imitations and more exact repetitions, providing an extra exemplar, following infants' imitations of non-familiar words. The familiar words infants imitated were more likely to receive the more complex expanded and reduced+expanded return imitations. Results suggest mothers' responses to infants' verbal imitations could serve as a mechanism for facilitating language acquisition.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Janet Olson
- Northern Illinois University, DeKalb, IL, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
17
|
Wong TP, Moran C, Foster-Cohen S. The effects of expansions, questions and cloze procedures on children's conversational skills. CLINICAL LINGUISTICS & PHONETICS 2012; 26:273-287. [PMID: 21967501 DOI: 10.3109/02699206.2011.614717] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/31/2023]
Abstract
The effectiveness of expansion as a technique for facilitating children's language and conversational skills is well known (Scherer and Olswang, 1984). Expansion, however, can appear alone or in combination with other techniques. Using a repeated measures design, this study aimed to compare the effects of expansion alone (EA); expansion combined with wh-questions (EQ); and expansion followed by a cloze procedure (EC) on the conversational skills of eight preschool children with conversational difficulties. Results showed that while there were no significant differences in child verbal topic maintaining responses across all techniques, EA elicited a significantly higher number of topic extensions, more non-verbal topic maintaining responses and fewer 'non-relevant responses' from the children, than either EQ or EC. The positive effects of each technique on the pragmatic appropriateness in conversations suggest that they could be used strategically in language intervention to ensure greater therapeutic effect.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tze-Peng Wong
- Department of Communication Disorders, New Zealand Institute of Language, Brain and Behavior, University of Canterbury, Christchurch, New Zealand.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
18
|
Cecchini M, Baroni E, Di Vito C, Piccolo F, Lai C. Newborn preference for a new face vs. a previously seen communicative or motionless face. Infant Behav Dev 2011; 34:424-33. [PMID: 21550665 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2011.04.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/18/2009] [Revised: 04/01/2010] [Accepted: 04/01/2011] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
|
19
|
Pelaez M, Virues-Ortega J, Gewirtz JL. Reinforcement of vocalizations through contingent vocal imitation. J Appl Behav Anal 2011; 44:33-40. [PMID: 21541136 PMCID: PMC3050463 DOI: 10.1901/jaba.2011.44-33] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/30/2009] [Accepted: 05/25/2010] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
Maternal vocal imitation of infant vocalizations is highly prevalent during face-to-face interactions of infants and their caregivers. Although maternal vocal imitation has been associated with later verbal development, its potentially reinforcing effect on infant vocalizations has not been explored experimentally. This study examined the reinforcing effect of maternal vocal imitation of infant vocalizations using a reversal probe BAB design. Eleven 3- to 8-month-old infants at high risk for developmental delays experienced contingent maternal vocal imitation during reinforcement conditions. Differential reinforcement of other behavior served as the control condition. The behavior of 10 infants showed evidence of a reinforcement effect. Results indicated that vocal imitations can serve to reinforce early infant vocalizations.
Collapse
|