1
|
Iveson MH, Altschul D, Deary I. Do childhood socioeconomic circumstances moderate the association between childhood cognitive ability and all-cause mortality across the life course? Prospective observational study of the 36-day sample of the Scottish Mental Survey 1947. BMJ Open 2020; 10:e037847. [PMID: 33268399 PMCID: PMC7712420 DOI: 10.1136/bmjopen-2020-037847] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/18/2020] [Revised: 11/02/2020] [Accepted: 11/17/2020] [Indexed: 11/08/2022] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND There is growing evidence that higher childhood cognitive ability predicts lower all-cause mortality risk across the life course. Whereas this association does not appear to be mediated by childhood socioeconomic circumstances, it is unclear whether socioeconomic circumstances moderate this association. METHODS The moderating role of childhood socioeconomic circumstances was assessed in 5318 members of the 36-day sample of the Scottish Mental Survey 1947. Univariate, sex-adjusted and age-adjusted, and mutually adjusted Cox models predicting all-cause mortality risk up to age 79 years were created using childhood IQ scores and childhood social class as predictors. Moderation was assessed by adding an interaction term between IQ scores and social class and comparing model fit. RESULTS An SD advantage in childhood IQ scores (HR=0.83, 95% CI 0.79 to 0.86, p<0.001) and a single-class advantage in childhood social class (HR=0.92, 95% CI 0.88 to 0.97, p<0.001) independently predicted lower mortality risk. Adding the IQ-social class interaction effect did not improve model fit (χ2Δ=1.36, p=0.24), and the interaction effect did not predict mortality risk (HR=1.03, 95% CI 0.98 to 1.07, p=0.25). CONCLUSIONS The present study demonstrated that the association between higher childhood cognitive ability and lower all-cause mortality risk is not conditional on childhood social class. Whereas other measures of socioeconomic circumstances may play a moderating role, these findings suggest that the benefits of higher childhood cognitive ability for longevity apply regardless of the material socioeconomic circumstances experienced in childhood.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Matthew Henry Iveson
- Centre for Clinical Brain Sciences, The University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK
- Department of Psychology, The University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK
| | - Drew Altschul
- Centre for Clinical Brain Sciences, The University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK
- Department of Psychology, The University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK
| | - Ian Deary
- Department of Psychology, The University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK
- Lothian Birth Cohorts, The University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Bakrania K, Edwardson CL, Khunti K, Bandelow S, Davies MJ, Yates T. Associations Between Sedentary Behaviors and Cognitive Function: Cross-Sectional and Prospective Findings From the UK Biobank. Am J Epidemiol 2018; 187:441-454. [PMID: 28992036 DOI: 10.1093/aje/kwx273] [Citation(s) in RCA: 77] [Impact Index Per Article: 11.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/04/2017] [Accepted: 07/06/2017] [Indexed: 12/12/2022] Open
Abstract
We investigated the cross-sectional and prospective associations between different sedentary behaviors and cognitive function in a large sample of adults with data stored in the UK Biobank. Baseline data were available for 502,643 participants (2006-2010, United Kingdom). Cognitive tests included prospective memory (baseline only: n = 171,585), visual-spatial memory (round 1: n = 483,832; round 2: n = 482,762), fluid intelligence (n = 165,492), and short-term numeric memory (n = 50,370). After a mean period of 5.3 years, participants (numbering from 12,091 to 114,373, depending on the test) also provided follow-up cognitive data. Sedentary behaviors (television viewing, driving, and nonoccupational computer-use time) were measured at baseline. At baseline, both television viewing and driving time were inversely associated with cognitive function across all outcomes (e.g., for each additional hour spent watching television, the total number of correct answers in the fluid intelligence test was 0.15 (99% confidence interval: 0.14, 0.16) lower. Computer-use time was positively associated with cognitive function across all outcomes. Both television viewing and driving time at baseline were positively associated with the odds of having cognitive decline at follow-up across most outcomes. Conversely, computer-use time at baseline was inversely associated with the odds of having cognitive decline at follow-up across most outcomes. This study supports health policies designed to reduce television viewing and driving in adults.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kishan Bakrania
- Department of Health Sciences, University of Leicester, Leicester General Hospital, Leicester, United Kingdom
- Diabetes Research Centre, University of Leicester, Leicester General Hospital, Leicester, United Kingdom
- Leicester Diabetes Centre, University Hospitals of Leicester, Leicester General Hospital, Leicester, United Kingdom
- National Institute for Health Research Leicester Biomedical Research Centre, Leicester General Hospital, Leicester, United Kingdom
- National Institute for Health Research Collaboration for Leadership in Applied Health Research and Care—East Midlands, Diabetes Research Centre, Leicester General Hospital, Leicester, United Kingdom
| | - Charlotte L Edwardson
- Diabetes Research Centre, University of Leicester, Leicester General Hospital, Leicester, United Kingdom
- Leicester Diabetes Centre, University Hospitals of Leicester, Leicester General Hospital, Leicester, United Kingdom
- National Institute for Health Research Leicester Biomedical Research Centre, Leicester General Hospital, Leicester, United Kingdom
| | - Kamlesh Khunti
- Diabetes Research Centre, University of Leicester, Leicester General Hospital, Leicester, United Kingdom
- Leicester Diabetes Centre, University Hospitals of Leicester, Leicester General Hospital, Leicester, United Kingdom
- National Institute for Health Research Collaboration for Leadership in Applied Health Research and Care—East Midlands, Diabetes Research Centre, Leicester General Hospital, Leicester, United Kingdom
| | - Stephan Bandelow
- School of Sport, Exercise, and Health Sciences, Loughborough University, Loughborough, United Kingdom
| | - Melanie J Davies
- Diabetes Research Centre, University of Leicester, Leicester General Hospital, Leicester, United Kingdom
- Leicester Diabetes Centre, University Hospitals of Leicester, Leicester General Hospital, Leicester, United Kingdom
- National Institute for Health Research Leicester Biomedical Research Centre, Leicester General Hospital, Leicester, United Kingdom
| | - Thomas Yates
- Diabetes Research Centre, University of Leicester, Leicester General Hospital, Leicester, United Kingdom
- Leicester Diabetes Centre, University Hospitals of Leicester, Leicester General Hospital, Leicester, United Kingdom
- National Institute for Health Research Leicester Biomedical Research Centre, Leicester General Hospital, Leicester, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Iveson MH, Cukic I, Der G, Batty GD, Deary IJ. Intelligence and all-cause mortality in the 6-Day Sample of the Scottish Mental Survey 1947 and their siblings: testing the contribution of family background. Int J Epidemiol 2018; 47:89-96. [PMID: 29025063 PMCID: PMC5837228 DOI: 10.1093/ije/dyx168] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 07/31/2017] [Indexed: 11/14/2022] Open
Abstract
Background Higher early-life intelligence is associated with a reduced risk of mortality in adulthood, though this association is apparently hardly attenuated when accounting for early-life socio-economic status (SES). However, the use of proxy measures of SES means that residual confounding may underestimate this attenuation. In the present study, the potential confounding effect of early-life SES was instead accounted for by examining the intelligence-mortality association within families. Methods The association between early-life intelligence and mortality in adulthood was assessed in 727 members of the 6-Day Sample of the Scottish Mental Survey 1947 and, for the first time, 1580 of their younger siblings. These individuals were born between 1936 and 1958, and were followed up into later life, with deaths recorded up to 2015. Cox regression was used to estimate the relative risk of mortality associated with higher IQ scores after adjusting for shared family factors. Results A standard-deviation advantage in IQ score was associated with a significantly reduced mortality risk [hazard ratio = 0.76, p < 0.001, 95% confidence interval (CI) (0.68-0.84)]. This reduction in hazard was only slightly attenuated by adjusting for sex and shared family factors [hazard ratio = 0.79, p = 0.002, 95% CI (0.68-0.92)]. Conclusions Although somewhat conservative, adjusting for all variance shared by a family avoids any potential residual confounding of the intelligence-mortality association arising from the use of proxy measures of early-life SES. The present study demonstrates that the longevity associated with higher early-life intelligence cannot be explained by early-life SES or within-family factors.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Matthew H Iveson
- The Centre for Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive Epidemiology, The University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK
- The Administrative Data Research Centre Scotland, Edinburgh, UK
| | - Iva Cukic
- The Centre for Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive Epidemiology, The University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK
| | - Geoff Der
- MRC Social and Public Health Sciences Unit, University of Glasgow, Glasgow, UK
| | - G David Batty
- Department of Epidemiology and Public Health, University College, London, UK
| | - Ian J Deary
- The Centre for Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive Epidemiology, The University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
|
5
|
Andrews SJ, Das D, Anstey KJ, Easteal S. Association of AKAP6 and MIR2113 with cognitive performance in a population-based sample of older adults. GENES BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR 2017; 16:472-478. [PMID: 28067462 DOI: 10.1111/gbb.12368] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/26/2016] [Revised: 12/21/2016] [Accepted: 01/04/2017] [Indexed: 01/12/2023]
Abstract
Genetic factors make a substantial contribution to inter-individual variability in cognitive function. A recent meta-analysis of genome-wide association studies identified two loci, AKAP6 and MIR2113, that are associated with general cognitive function. Here, we extend this previous research by investigating the association of MIR2113 and AKAP6 with baseline and longitudinal non-linear change across a broad spectrum of cognitive domains in a community-based cohort of older adults without dementia. Two single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs), MIR211-rs10457441 and AKAP6-rs17522122 were genotyped in 1570 non-demented older Australians of European ancestry, who were examined up to 4 times over 12 years. Linear mixed effects models were used to examine the association between AKAP6 and MIR2113 with cognitive performance in episodic memory, working memory, vocabulary, perceptual speed and reaction time at baseline and with linear and quadratic rates of change. AKAP6-rs17522122*T was associated with worse baseline performance in episodic memory, working memory, vocabulary and perceptual speed, but it was not associated with cognitive change in any domain. MIR2113-rs10457441*T was associated with accelerated decline in episodic memory. No other associations with baseline cognitive performance or with linear or quadratic rate or cognitive changes were observed for this SNP. These results confirm the previous finding that AKAP6 is associated with performance across multiple cognitive domains at baseline but not with cognitive decline, while MIR2113 primarily affects the rate at which memory declines over time.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- S J Andrews
- John Curtin School of Medical Research, Australian National University, Canberra, Australia
| | - D Das
- John Curtin School of Medical Research, Australian National University, Canberra, Australia
| | - K J Anstey
- Centre for Research on Ageing, Health and Wellbeing, Australian National University, Canberra, Australia
| | - S Easteal
- John Curtin School of Medical Research, Australian National University, Canberra, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Beaver KM, Schwartz JA, Connolly EJ, Al-Ghamdi MS, Kobeisy AN, Barnes JC, Boutwell BB. Intelligence and early life mortality: Findings from a longitudinal sample of youth. DEATH STUDIES 2016; 40:298-304. [PMID: 26765521 DOI: 10.1080/07481187.2015.1137994] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/05/2023]
Abstract
The current study examined whether adolescent IQ predicted risk for mortality by the age of 32. Analyses of data from the Add Health revealed that IQ was related to mortality risk, such that respondents with relatively lower IQs were significantly more likely to experience early life mortality when compared to respondents with relatively higher IQs. This association remained statistically significant even after controlling for a host of covariates such as race, gender, involvement in violent behaviors, levels of self-control, and poverty. The average IQ of deceased respondents was approximately 95, whereas the average IQ of living respondents was about 100.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kevin M Beaver
- a College of Criminology and Criminal Justice , Florida State University , Tallahassee , Florida , USA
- b Center for Social and Humanities Research , King Abdulaziz University , Jeddah , Saudi Arabia
| | - Joseph A Schwartz
- c School of Criminology and Criminal Justice , University of Nebraska at Omaha , Lincoln , Nebraska , USA
| | - Eric J Connolly
- d Department of Criminal Justice , Pennsylvania State University , Abington , Pennsylvania , USA
| | - Mohammed Said Al-Ghamdi
- b Center for Social and Humanities Research , King Abdulaziz University , Jeddah , Saudi Arabia
| | - Ahmed Nezar Kobeisy
- b Center for Social and Humanities Research , King Abdulaziz University , Jeddah , Saudi Arabia
| | - J C Barnes
- e School of Criminal Justice , University of Cincinnati , Cincinnati , Ohio , USA
| | - Brian B Boutwell
- f Criminology & Criminal Justice, School of Social Work and Department of Epidemiology , Saint Louis University , St. Louis , Missouri , USA
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Hagger-Johnson G, Deary IJ, Davies CA, Weiss A, Batty GD. Reaction time and mortality from the major causes of death: the NHANES-III study. PLoS One 2014; 9:e82959. [PMID: 24489645 PMCID: PMC3906008 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0082959] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/28/2013] [Accepted: 10/30/2013] [Indexed: 02/03/2023] Open
Abstract
OBJECTIVE Studies examining the relation of information processing speed, as measured by reaction time, with mortality are scarce. We explored these associations in a representative sample of the US population. METHODS Participants were 5,134 adults (2,342 men) aged 20-59 years from the Third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES III, 1988-94). RESULTS Adjusted for age, sex, and ethnic minority status, a 1 SD slower reaction time was associated with a raised risk of mortality from all-causes (HR = 1.25, 95% CI 1.12, 1.39) and cardiovascular disease (CVD) (HR = 1.36, 95% CI 1.17, 1.58). Having 1 SD more variable reaction time was also associated with greater risk of all-cause (HR = 1.36, 95% CI 1.19, 1.55) and CVD (HR = 1.50, 95% CI 1.33, 1.70) mortality. No associations were observed for cancer mortality. The magnitude of the relationships was comparable in size to established risk factors in this dataset, such as smoking. INTERPRETATION Alongside better-established risk factors, reaction time is associated with increased risk of premature death and cardiovascular disease. It is a candidate risk factor for all-cause and cause-specific mortality.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gareth Hagger-Johnson
- Department of Epidemiology and Public Health, University College London (UCL), London, United Kingdom
| | - Ian J. Deary
- Department of Psychology, Centre for Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive Epidemiology, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, United Kingdom
| | - Carolyn A. Davies
- Medical Research Council (MRC)/Chief Scientist Office (CSO) Social and Public Health Sciences Unit, University of Glasgow, Glasgow, United Kingdom
| | - Alexander Weiss
- Department of Psychology, Centre for Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive Epidemiology, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, United Kingdom
| | - G. David Batty
- Department of Epidemiology and Public Health, University College London (UCL), London, United Kingdom
- Department of Psychology, Centre for Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive Epidemiology, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Batterham PJ, Bunce D, Mackinnon AJ, Christensen H. Intra-individual reaction time variability and all-cause mortality over 17 years: a community-based cohort study. Age Ageing 2014; 43:84-90. [PMID: 23934546 DOI: 10.1093/ageing/aft116] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/14/2022] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND very few studies have examined the association between intra-individual reaction time variability and subsequent mortality. Furthermore, the ability of simple measures of variability to predict mortality has not been compared with more complex measures. METHOD a prospective cohort study of 896 community-based Australian adults aged 70+ were interviewed up to four times from 1990 to 2002, with vital status assessed until June 2007. From this cohort, 770-790 participants were included in Cox proportional hazards regression models of survival. Vital status and time in study were used to conduct survival analyses. The mean reaction time and three measures of intra-individual reaction time variability were calculated separately across 20 trials of simple and choice reaction time tasks. Models were adjusted for a range of demographic, physical health and mental health measures. RESULTS greater intra-individual simple reaction time variability, as assessed by the raw standard deviation (raw SD), coefficient of variation (CV) or the intra-individual standard deviation (ISD), was strongly associated with an increased hazard of all-cause mortality in adjusted Cox regression models. The mean reaction time had no significant association with mortality. CONCLUSION intra-individual variability in simple reaction time appears to have a robust association with mortality over 17 years. Health professionals such as neuropsychologists may benefit in their detection of neuropathology by supplementing neuropsychiatric testing with the straightforward process of testing simple reaction time and calculating raw SD or CV.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Philip J Batterham
- Centre for Mental Health Research, The Australian National University, Building 63 Eggleston Road, Canberra, Australian Capital Territory 0200, Australia
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
9
|
Average county-level IQ predicts county-level disadvantage and several county-level mortality risk rates. INTELLIGENCE 2013. [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2012.10.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
|
10
|
Kajantie E, Räikkönen K, Henriksson M, Leskinen JT, Forsén T, Heinonen K, Pesonen AK, Osmond C, Barker DJP, Eriksson JG. Stroke is predicted by low visuospatial in relation to other intellectual abilities and coronary heart disease by low general intelligence. PLoS One 2012; 7:e46841. [PMID: 23144789 PMCID: PMC3492363 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0046841] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/05/2010] [Accepted: 09/09/2012] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Background Low intellectual ability is associated with an increased risk of coronary heart disease and stroke. Most studies have used a general intelligence score. We studied whether three different subscores of intellectual ability predict these disorders. Methods We studied 2,786 men, born between 1934 and 1944 in Helsinki, Finland, who as conscripts at age 20 underwent an intellectual ability test comprising verbal, visuospatial (analogous to Raven's progressive matrices) and arithmetic reasoning subtests. We ascertained the later occurrence of coronary heart disease and stroke from validated national hospital discharge and death registers. Results 281 men (10.1%) had experienced a coronary heart disease event and 131 (4.7%) a stroke event. Coronary heart disease was predicted by low scores in all subtests, hazard ratios for each standard deviation (SD) lower score ranging from 1.21 to 1.30 (confidence intervals 1.08 to 1.46). Stroke was predicted by a low visuospatial reasoning score, the corresponding hazard ratio being 1.23 (95% confidence interval 1.04 to 1.46), adjusted for year and age at testing. Adjusted in addition for the two other scores, the hazard ratio was 1.40 (1.10 to 1.79). This hazard ratio was little affected by adjustment for socioeconomic status in childhood and adult life, whereas the same adjustments attenuated the associations between intellectual ability and coronary heart disease. The associations with stroke were also unchanged when adjusted for systolic blood pressure at 20 years and reimbursement for adult antihypertensive medication. Conclusions Stroke is predicted by low visuospatial reasoning scores in relation to scores in the two other subtests. This association may be mediated by common underlying causes such as impaired brain development, rather than by mechanisms associated with risk factors shared by stroke and coronary heart disease, such as socio-economic status, hypertension and atherosclerosis.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Eero Kajantie
- National Institute for Health and Welfare, Helsinki, Finland.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
11
|
Murray C, Pattie A, Starr JM, Deary IJ. Does cognitive ability predict mortality in the ninth decade? The Lothian Birth Cohort 1921. INTELLIGENCE 2012. [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2012.05.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/28/2022]
|
12
|
|