1
|
Dehaene S, Sablé-Meyer M, Ciccione L. Origins of numbers: a shared language-of-thought for arithmetic and geometry? Trends Cogn Sci 2025:S1364-6613(25)00059-2. [PMID: 40234140 DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2025.03.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/02/2024] [Revised: 02/07/2025] [Accepted: 03/06/2025] [Indexed: 04/17/2025]
Abstract
Concepts of exact number are often thought to originate from counting and the successor function, or from a refinement of the approximate number system (ANS). We argue here for a third origin: a shared language-of-thought (LoT) for geometry and arithmetic that involves primitives of repetition, concatenation, and recursive embedding. Applied to sets, those primitives engender concepts of exact integers through recursive applications of additions and multiplications. Links between geometry and arithmetic also explain the emergence of higher-level notions (squares, primes, etc.). Under our hypothesis, understanding a number means having one or several mental expressions for it, and their minimal description length (MDL) determines how easily they can be mentally manipulated. Several historical, developmental, linguistic, and brain imaging phenomena provide preliminary support for our proposal.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Stanislas Dehaene
- Cognitive Neuroimaging Unit, Commissariat à l'Energie Atomique (CEA), Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM), NeuroSpin Center, Université Paris-Saclay, 91191 Gif-sur-Yvette, France; Collège de France, Université Paris-Sciences-Lettres (PSL), 11 Place Marcelin Berthelot, 75005 Paris, France
| | - Mathias Sablé-Meyer
- Cognitive Neuroimaging Unit, Commissariat à l'Energie Atomique (CEA), Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM), NeuroSpin Center, Université Paris-Saclay, 91191 Gif-sur-Yvette, France; Collège de France, Université Paris-Sciences-Lettres (PSL), 11 Place Marcelin Berthelot, 75005 Paris, France
| | - Lorenzo Ciccione
- Cognitive Neuroimaging Unit, Commissariat à l'Energie Atomique (CEA), Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM), NeuroSpin Center, Université Paris-Saclay, 91191 Gif-sur-Yvette, France; Collège de France, Université Paris-Sciences-Lettres (PSL), 11 Place Marcelin Berthelot, 75005 Paris, France.
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Matsumoto D, Nakai T. Syntactic theory of mathematical expressions. Cogn Psychol 2023; 146:101606. [PMID: 37748253 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101606] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/10/2023] [Revised: 06/28/2023] [Accepted: 09/16/2023] [Indexed: 09/27/2023]
Abstract
Mathematical expressions consist of recursive combinations of numbers, variables, and operators. According to theoretical linguists, the syntactic mechanisms of natural language also provide a basis for mathematics. To date, however, no theoretically rigorous investigation has been conducted to support such arguments. Therefore, this study uses a methodology based on theoretical linguistics to analyze the syntactic properties of mathematical expressions. Through a review of recent behavioral and neuroimaging studies on mathematical syntax, we report several inconsistencies with theoretical linguistics, such as the use of ternary structures. To address these, we propose that a syntactic category called Applicative plays a central role in analyzing mathematical expressions with seemingly ternary structures by combining binary structures. Besides basic arithmetic expressions, we also examine algebraic equations and complex expressions such as integral and differential calculi. This study is the first attempt at building a comprehensive framework for analyzing the syntactic structures of mathematical expressions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Daiki Matsumoto
- Graduate School of Human and Environmental Studies, Kyoto University, Kyoto, Japan; Department of Humanities, Kanazawa Seiryo University, Kanazawa, Japan
| | - Tomoya Nakai
- Lyon Neuroscience Research Center (CRNL), (INSERM/CNRS/University of Lyon), Bron, France; Araya Inc., Tokyo, Japan; Advanced Comprehensive Research Organization, Teikyo University, Tokyo, Japan.
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Dotan D. Top-Down Number Reading: Language Affects the Visual Identification of Digit Strings. Cogn Sci 2023; 47:e13368. [PMID: 37864833 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13368] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/14/2023] [Revised: 09/29/2023] [Accepted: 10/09/2023] [Indexed: 10/23/2023]
Abstract
Reading numbers aloud involves visual processes that analyze the digit string and verbal processes that produce the number words. Cognitive models of number reading assume that information flows from the visual input to the verbal production processes-a feed-forward processing mode in which the verbal production depends on the visual input but not vice versa. Here, I show that information flows also in the opposite direction, from verbal production to the visual input processes. Participants read aloud briefly presented multi-digit strings in Hebrew, in which the order of words is congruent with the order of digits (21 = twenty-and-one), and in Arabic, in which the ones word precedes the tens word (one-and-twenty). The error-by-digit-position curve was affected by language: relative to Hebrew, in Arabic the error rate was slightly lower for the unit digit and slightly higher for the decade digit, indicating that in Arabic the unit digit was processed earlier and the decade digit later, in accord with the Arabic word order. This language-dependent processing order originated in the visual level and was not a verbal confound, because it persisted even when I controlled for the serial position of the decade/unit word in the verbal number by using numbers with 0 (two hundred three/two hundred thirty). I conclude that the visual analyzer's digit scanning order, decade-first or unit-first, is not fixed but affected by the language in which the number is produced-a top-down, verbal-to-visual information flow.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dror Dotan
- Mathematical Thinking Lab, School of Education, Tel Aviv University
- Sagol School of Neuroscience, Tel Aviv University
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Guerrero D, Park J. Arithmetic thinking as the basis of children's generative number concepts. DEVELOPMENTAL REVIEW 2023. [DOI: 10.1016/j.dr.2022.101062] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/12/2022]
|
5
|
Bulut T. Meta-analytic connectivity modeling of the left and right inferior frontal gyri. Cortex 2022; 155:107-131. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2022.07.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/03/2022] [Revised: 05/21/2022] [Accepted: 07/15/2022] [Indexed: 11/03/2022]
|
6
|
Dotan D, Brutman N. Syntactic chunking reveals a core syntactic representation of multi-digit numbers, which is generative and automatic. Cogn Res Princ Implic 2022; 7:58. [PMID: 35792977 PMCID: PMC9259776 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-022-00409-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/14/2022] [Accepted: 06/13/2022] [Indexed: 12/05/2022] Open
Abstract
Representing the base-10 structure of numbers is a challenging cognitive ability, unique to humans, but it is yet unknown how precisely this is done. Here, we examined whether and how literate adults represent a number’s full syntactic structure. In 5 experiments, participants repeated number-word sequences and we systematically varied the order of words within each sequence. Repetition on grammatical sequences (e.g., two hundred ninety-seven) was better than on non-grammatical ones (hundred seven two ninety). We conclude that the participants represented the number’s full syntactic structure and used it to merge number words into chunks in short-term memory. Accuracy monotonously improved for sequences with increasingly longer grammatical segments, up to a limit of ~ 4 words per segment, irrespectively of the number of digits, and worsened thereafter. Namely, short chunks improved memorization, whereas oversized chunks disrupted memorization. This chunk size limit suggests that the chunks are not based on predefined structures, whose size limit is not expected to be so low, but are created ad hoc by a generative process, such as the hierarchical syntactic representation hypothesized in Michael McCloskey’s number-processing model. Chunking occurred even when it disrupted performance, as in the oversized chunks, and even when external cues for chunking were controlled for or were removed. We conclude that the above generative process operates automatically rather than voluntarily. To date, this is the most detailed account of the core representation of the syntactic structure of numbers—a critical aspect of numerical literacy and of the ability to read and write numbers.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dror Dotan
- Mathematical Thinking Lab, School of Education and School of Neuroscience, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel.
| | - Nadin Brutman
- Mathematical Thinking Lab, School of Education and School of Neuroscience, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Dotan D, Breslavskiy I, Copty-Diab H, Yousefi V. Syntactic priming reveals an explicit syntactic representation of multi-digit verbal numbers. Cognition 2021; 215:104821. [PMID: 34224979 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104821] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/22/2021] [Revised: 06/16/2021] [Accepted: 06/21/2021] [Indexed: 01/01/2023]
Abstract
When we say or understand verbal numbers, a major challenge to the cognitive system is the need to process the number's syntactic structure. Several studies showed that number syntax is handled by dedicated processes, however, it is still unclear how precisely these processes operate, whether the number's syntactic structure is represented explicitly, and if it is - what this representation looks like. Here, we used a novel experimental paradigm, syntactic priming of numbers, which can examine in detail the syntactic representation of multi-digit verbal numbers. In each trial, the participants - Arabic-Hebrew bilinguals and Hebrew monolinguals - heard a multi-digit number and responded orally with a random number. The syntactic structure of their responses was similar to that of the targets, showing that they represented the verbal number's syntax. This priming effect was genuinely syntactic, and could not be explained as lexical - repeating words from the target; as phonological - responding with words phonologically-similar to the target; or as a numerical distance effect - producing responses numerically close to the target. The syntactic priming effect was stronger for earlier words in the verbal number and weaker for later words, suggesting that the syntactic representation is capped by working-memory limits. We propose that syntactic priming could become a useful method to examine various aspects of the syntactic representation of numbers.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dror Dotan
- Mathematical Thinking Lab, School of Education and the Sagol School of Neuroscience, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv 6997801, Israel.
| | - Ilya Breslavskiy
- Mathematical Thinking Lab, School of Education and the Sagol School of Neuroscience, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv 6997801, Israel
| | - Haneen Copty-Diab
- Mathematical Thinking Lab, School of Education and the Sagol School of Neuroscience, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv 6997801, Israel
| | - Vivian Yousefi
- Mathematical Thinking Lab, School of Education and the Sagol School of Neuroscience, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv 6997801, Israel
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Baker JM, Gillam RB, Jordan KE. Children's neural activity during number line estimations assessed by functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS). Brain Cogn 2020; 144:105601. [PMID: 32739744 PMCID: PMC7855273 DOI: 10.1016/j.bandc.2020.105601] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/25/2020] [Revised: 07/10/2020] [Accepted: 07/17/2020] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
Abstract
Number line estimation (NLE) is an educational task in which children estimate the location of a value (e.g., 25) on a blank line that represents a numerical range (e.g., 0-100). NLE performance is a strong predictor of success in mathematics, and error patterns on this task help provide a glimpse into how children may represent number internally. However, a missing and fundamental element of this puzzle is the identification of neural correlates of NLE in children. That is, understanding possible neural signatures related to NLE performance will provide valuable insight into the cognitive processes that underlie children's development of NLE ability. Using functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS), we provide the first investigation of concurrent behavioral and cortical signatures of NLE performance in children. Specifically, our results highlight significant fronto-parietal changes in cortical activation in response to increases in NLE scale (e.g., 0-100 vs. 0-100,000). Furthermore, our results demonstrate that NLE performance feedback (auditory, visual, or audiovisual), as well as children's grade (2nd vs. 3rd) influence cortical responding during an NLE task.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Joseph M Baker
- Center for Interdisciplinary Brain Sciences Research, Division of Interdisciplinary Brain Sciences, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, School of Medicine, Stanford University, United States.
| | - Ronald B Gillam
- Department of Communicative Disorders and Deaf Education, Utah State University, United States
| | - Kerry E Jordan
- Department of Psychology, Utah State University, United States
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Nakai T, Okanoya K. Cortical collateralization induced by language and arithmetic in non-right-handers. Cortex 2019; 124:154-166. [PMID: 31901561 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.11.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/28/2019] [Revised: 08/26/2019] [Accepted: 11/20/2019] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
Abstract
The functional overlap of language and arithmetic is debatable. Although some studies have reported independent representations of arithmetic and language in the brain, other studies have reported shared activity of the two cognitive domains in the inferior frontal gyrus. Although most previous studies have evaluated right-handed individuals, variability of hemispheric dominance in non-right-handed individuals should provide important information on the functional collateralization of these two cognitive domains. The present study evaluated the cortical lateralization patterns of the two cognitive domains using functional magnetic resonance imaging in 30 non-right-handed participants who performed language and arithmetic tasks. We found that language and arithmetic tasks demonstrated shared activity in the bilateral inferior frontal gyrus (IFG). Furthermore, the lateralization patterns of language and arithmetic tasks were correlated with each other. Most participants with language dominance in the left hemisphere also exhibited dominance of arithmetic tasks in the left hemisphere; similarly, most participants with language dominance in the right hemisphere exhibited dominance of arithmetic tasks in the right hemisphere. Among all the brain regions, the precentral gyrus, which is located slightly posterior to the IFG, exhibited the highest correlation coefficient between laterality indices of language and arithmetic tasks. These results suggest a shared functional property between language and arithmetic in the brain.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tomoya Nakai
- Center for Information and Neural Networks (CiNet), National Institute of Information and Communication Technology, Osaka University, Osaka, Japan; The University of Tokyo, Graduate School of Arts and Sciences, Tokyo, Japan; National Rehabilitation Center For Persons with Disabilities, Saitama, Japan
| | - Kazuo Okanoya
- The University of Tokyo, Graduate School of Arts and Sciences, Tokyo, Japan; Center for Evolutionary Cognitive Science, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan.
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Shared neural resources of rhythm and syntax: An ALE meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia 2019; 137:107284. [PMID: 31783081 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.107284] [Citation(s) in RCA: 35] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/27/2019] [Accepted: 11/25/2019] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
A growing body of evidence has highlighted behavioral connections between musical rhythm and linguistic syntax, suggesting that these abilities may be mediated by common neural resources. Here, we performed a quantitative meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies using activation likelihood estimate (ALE) to localize the shared neural structures engaged in a representative set of musical rhythm (rhythm, beat, and meter) and linguistic syntax (merge movement, and reanalysis) operations. Rhythm engaged a bilateral sensorimotor network throughout the brain consisting of the inferior frontal gyri, supplementary motor area, superior temporal gyri/temporoparietal junction, insula, intraparietal lobule, and putamen. By contrast, syntax mostly recruited the left sensorimotor network including the inferior frontal gyrus, posterior superior temporal gyrus, premotor cortex, and supplementary motor area. Intersections between rhythm and syntax maps yielded overlapping regions in the left inferior frontal gyrus, left supplementary motor area, and bilateral insula-neural substrates involved in temporal hierarchy processing and predictive coding. Together, this is the first neuroimaging meta-analysis providing detailed anatomical overlap of sensorimotor regions recruited for musical rhythm and linguistic syntax.
Collapse
|
11
|
Amalric M, Dehaene S. Cortical circuits for mathematical knowledge: evidence for a major subdivision within the brain's semantic networks. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2018; 373:rstb.2016.0515. [PMID: 29292362 DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0515] [Citation(s) in RCA: 46] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 06/17/2017] [Indexed: 01/29/2023] Open
Abstract
Is mathematical language similar to natural language? Are language areas used by mathematicians when they do mathematics? And does the brain comprise a generic semantic system that stores mathematical knowledge alongside knowledge of history, geography or famous people? Here, we refute those views by reviewing three functional MRI studies of the representation and manipulation of high-level mathematical knowledge in professional mathematicians. The results reveal that brain activity during professional mathematical reflection spares perisylvian language-related brain regions as well as temporal lobe areas classically involved in general semantic knowledge. Instead, mathematical reflection recycles bilateral intraparietal and ventral temporal regions involved in elementary number sense. Even simple fact retrieval, such as remembering that 'the sine function is periodical' or that 'London buses are red', activates dissociated areas for math versus non-math knowledge. Together with other fMRI and recent intracranial studies, our results indicated a major separation between two brain networks for mathematical and non-mathematical semantics, which goes a long way to explain a variety of facts in neuroimaging, neuropsychology and developmental disorders.This article is part of a discussion meeting issue 'The origins of numerical abilities'.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marie Amalric
- Cognitive Neuroimaging Unit, CEA DSV/I2BM, INSERM, Université Paris-Sud, Université Paris-Saclay, NeuroSpin center, 91191 Gif/Yvette, France .,Collège de France, Paris, France.,Sorbonne Universités, UPMC Univ Paris 06, IFD, 4 place Jussieu, Paris, France
| | - Stanislas Dehaene
- Cognitive Neuroimaging Unit, CEA DSV/I2BM, INSERM, Université Paris-Sud, Université Paris-Saclay, NeuroSpin center, 91191 Gif/Yvette, France .,Collège de France, Paris, France
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Jeon HA, Friederici AD. What Does "Being an Expert" Mean to the Brain? Functional Specificity and Connectivity in Expertise. Cereb Cortex 2018; 27:5603-5615. [PMID: 27797834 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhw329] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/16/2016] [Indexed: 12/15/2022] Open
Abstract
To what extent is varying cognitive expertise reflected in the brain's functional specificity and connectivity? We addressed this question by examining expertise in mathematics based on the fact that mathematical skills are one of the most critical cognitive abilities known to be a good predictor of academic achievement. We investigated processing of hierarchical structures, which is a fundamental process for building complex cognitive architecture. Experts and nonexperts in mathematics participated in processing hierarchical structures using algebraic expressions. Results showed that a modulating effect depending on expertise was observed specifically in nonexperts in the left inferior frontal gyrus around pars triangularis and frontal sulcus, the left intraparietal sulcus, and the right inferior parietal lobule. This expertise-dependent pattern of activation led to a crucial dissociation within the left prefrontal cortex. More interestingly, task-related functional networks were also modulated differently in the frontoparietal network for relatively good performance and in the frontostriatal network for poor performance. The present study indicates that a high level of expertise is evident in a small number of specific brain regions, whereas a low level of expertise is reflected by broadly distributed brain areas, along with divergent functional connectivity between experts and nonexperts.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hyeon-Ae Jeon
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Daegu Gyeongbuk Institute of Science and Technology (DGIST), Daegu 42988, Korea.,Partner Group of the Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences at the Department for Brain and Cognitive Sciences, DGIST, Daegu 42988, Korea
| | - Angela D Friederici
- Department of Neuropsychology, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Stephanstraße 1a, 04103Leipzig, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Hung YH, Frost SJ, Molfese P, Malins JG, Landi N, Mencl WE, Rueckl JG, Bogaerts L, Pugh KR. Common neural basis of motor sequence learning and word recognition and its relation with individual differences in reading skill. SCIENTIFIC STUDIES OF READING : THE OFFICIAL JOURNAL OF THE SOCIETY FOR THE SCIENTIFIC STUDY OF READING 2018; 23:89-100. [PMID: 31105422 PMCID: PMC6521955 DOI: 10.1080/10888438.2018.1451533] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/07/2023]
Abstract
To investigate the neural basis of a common statistical learning mechanism involved in motor sequence learning and decoding, we recorded same participants' brain activation in a serial reaction time (SRT) and word reading task using functional magnetic resonance imaging. In the SRT, a manual response was made depending on the location of a visual cue, and the order of the locations was either fixed or random. In the word reading task, visual words were passively presented. Compared to less skilled readers, more skilled readers showed greater differences in activation in the inferior frontal gyrus pars triangularis (IFGpTr) and the insula between the ordered and random condition in the SRT task and greater activation in those regions in the word reading task. It suggests that extraction of statistically predictable patterns in the IFGpTr and insula contributes to both motor sequence learning and orthographic learning, and therefore predicts individual differences in decoding skill.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yi-Hui Hung
- Haskins Laboratories
- Yale University
- National Yang-Ming University
| | | | - Peter Molfese
- Haskins Laboratories
- National Institutes of Mental Health
| | | | - Nicole Landi
- Haskins Laboratories
- Yale University
- University of Connecticut
| | | | | | | | - Kenneth R Pugh
- Haskins Laboratories
- Yale University
- University of Connecticut
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Bulut T, Hung YH, Tzeng O, Wu DH. Neural correlates of processing sentences and compound words in Chinese. PLoS One 2017; 12:e0188526. [PMID: 29194453 PMCID: PMC5711016 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0188526] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/14/2017] [Accepted: 11/08/2017] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
Sentence reading involves multiple linguistic operations including processing of lexical and compositional semantics, and determining structural and grammatical relationships among words. Previous studies on Indo-European languages have associated left anterior temporal lobe (aTL) and left interior frontal gyrus (IFG) with reading sentences compared to reading unstructured word lists. To examine whether these brain regions are also involved in reading a typologically distinct language with limited morphosyntax and lack of agreement between sentential arguments, an FMRI study was conducted to compare passive reading of Chinese sentences, unstructured word lists and disconnected character lists that are created by only changing the order of an identical set of characters. Similar to previous findings from other languages, stronger activation was found in mainly left-lateralized anterior temporal regions (including aTL) for reading sentences compared to unstructured word and character lists. On the other hand, stronger activation was identified in left posterior temporal sulcus for reading unstructured words compared to unstructured characters. Furthermore, reading unstructured word lists compared to sentences evoked stronger activation in left IFG and left inferior parietal lobule. Consistent with the literature on Indo-European languages, the present results suggest that left anterior temporal regions subserve sentence-level integration, while left IFG supports restoration of sentence structure. In addition, left posterior temporal sulcus is associated with morphological compounding. Taken together, reading Chinese sentences engages a common network as reading other languages, with particular reliance on integration of semantic constituents.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Talat Bulut
- Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, National Central University, Zhongli, Taiwan
- Department of Speech and Language Therapy, Istanbul Medipol University, Istanbul, Turkey
| | - Yi-Hui Hung
- Haskins Laboratories, Yale University, New Haven, Connecticut, United States of America
- Institute of Neuroscience, National Yang Ming University, Taipei, Taiwan
| | - Ovid Tzeng
- The Institute of Linguistics, Academia Sinica, Taipei, Taiwan
| | - Denise H. Wu
- Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, National Central University, Zhongli, Taiwan
- * E-mail:
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Blank IA, Kiran S, Fedorenko E. Can neuroimaging help aphasia researchers? Addressing generalizability, variability, and interpretability. Cogn Neuropsychol 2017; 34:377-393. [PMID: 29188746 PMCID: PMC6157596 DOI: 10.1080/02643294.2017.1402756] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/02/2023]
Abstract
Neuroimaging studies of individuals with brain damage seek to link brain structure and activity to cognitive impairments, spontaneous recovery, or treatment outcomes. To date, such studies have relied on the critical assumption that a given anatomical landmark corresponds to the same functional unit(s) across individuals. However, this assumption is fallacious even across neurologically healthy individuals. Here, we discuss the severe implications of this issue, and argue for an approach that circumvents it, whereby: (i) functional brain regions are defined separately for each subject using fMRI, allowing for inter-individual variability in their precise location; (ii) the response profile of these subject-specific regions are characterized using various other tasks; and (iii) the results are averaged across individuals, guaranteeing generalizabliity. This method harnesses the complementary strengths of single-case studies and group studies, and it eliminates the need for post hoc "reverse inference" from anatomical landmarks back to cognitive operations, thus improving data interpretability.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Idan A Blank
- a McGovern Institute for Brain Research , Massachusetts Institute of Technology , Cambridge , MA , USA
| | - Swathi Kiran
- b Department of Speech Language and Hearing Sciences, Aphasia Research Laboratory , Sargent College, Boston University , Boston , MA , USA
| | - Evelina Fedorenko
- c Department of Psychiatry , Massachusetts General Hospital , Charlestown , MA , USA
- d Department of Psychiatry , Harvard Medical School , Boston , MA , USA
| |
Collapse
|