1
|
Clement L, Schwarz S, Mahot-Castaing B, Wystrach A. Is this scenery worth exploring? Insight into the visual encoding of navigating ants. J Exp Biol 2025; 228:JEB249935. [PMID: 39882691 DOI: 10.1242/jeb.249935] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/04/2024] [Accepted: 01/25/2025] [Indexed: 01/31/2025]
Abstract
Solitary foraging insects such as desert ants rely heavily on vision for navigation. Although ants can learn visual scenes, it is unclear what cues they use to decide whether a scene is worth exploring at the first place. To investigate this, we recorded the motor behaviour of Cataglyphis velox ants navigating in a virtual reality setup and measured their lateral oscillations in response to various unfamiliar visual scenes under both closed-loop and open-loop conditions. In naturalistic-looking panorama, ants display regular oscillations as observed outdoors, allowing them to efficiently scan the scenery. Manipulations of the virtual environment revealed distinct functions served by dynamic and static cues. Dynamic cues, mainly rotational optic flow, regulated the amplitude of oscillations but not their regularity. Conversely, static cues had little impact on the amplitude but were essential for producing regular oscillations. Regularity of oscillations decreased in scenes with only horizontal, only vertical or no edges, but was restored in scenes with both edge types together. The actual number of edges, the visual pattern heterogeneity across azimuths, the light intensity or the relative elevation of brighter regions did not affect oscillations. We conclude that ants use a simple but functional heuristic to determine whether the visual world is worth exploring, relying on the presence of at least two different edge orientations in the scene.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Leo Clement
- Centre de Recherches sur la Cognition Animale, CNRS, Université Paul Sabatier, Toulouse 31062 cedex 09, France
| | - Sebastian Schwarz
- Centre de Recherches sur la Cognition Animale, CNRS, Université Paul Sabatier, Toulouse 31062 cedex 09, France
- Department of Biology, University of Graz, 8010 Graz, Austria
| | - Blandine Mahot-Castaing
- Centre de Recherches sur la Cognition Animale, CNRS, Université Paul Sabatier, Toulouse 31062 cedex 09, France
| | - Antoine Wystrach
- Centre de Recherches sur la Cognition Animale, CNRS, Université Paul Sabatier, Toulouse 31062 cedex 09, France
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Mota T, Paffhausen B, Menzel R. Chromatic processing and receptive-field structure in neurons of the anterior optic tract of the honeybee brain. PLoS One 2024; 19:e0310282. [PMID: 39264932 PMCID: PMC11392409 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0310282] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/21/2024] [Accepted: 08/28/2024] [Indexed: 09/14/2024] Open
Abstract
Color vision in honeybees is a well-documented perceptual phenomenon including multiple behavioral tests of trichromaticity and color opponency. Data on the combined color/space properties of high order visual neurons in the bee brain is however limited. Here we fill this gap by analyzing the activity of neurons in the anterior optic tract (AOT), a high order brain region suggested to be involved in chromatic processing. The spectral response properties of 72 units were measured using UV, blue and green light stimuli presented in 266 positions of the visual field. The majority of these units comprise combined chromatic-spatial processing properties. We found eight different neuron categories in terms of their spectral, spatial and temporal response properties. Color-opponent neurons, the most abundant neural category in the AOT, present large receptive fields and activity patterns that were typically opponent between UV and blue or green, particularly during the on-tonic response phase. Receptive field shapes and activity patterns of these color processing neurons are more similar between blue and green, than between UV and blue or green. We also identified intricate spatial antagonism and double spectral opponency in some receptive fields of color-opponent units. Stimulation protocols with different color combinations applied to 21 AOT units allowed us to uncover additional levels of spectral antagonism and hidden inhibitory inputs, even in some units that were initially classified as broad-band neurons based in their responses to single spectral lights. The results are discussed in the context of floral color discrimination and celestial spectral gradients.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Theo Mota
- Institute of Biology, Neurobiology, Freie Universität Berlin, Berlin, Germany
- Department of Physiology and Biophysics, Institute of Biological Sciences, Federal University of Minas Gerais, Belo Horizonte, Brazil
| | - Benjamin Paffhausen
- Institute of Biology, Neurobiology, Freie Universität Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Randolf Menzel
- Institute of Biology, Neurobiology, Freie Universität Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Qu X, Huang Q, Li H, Lou F. Comparative transcriptomics revealed the ecological trap effect of linearly polarized light on Oratosquilla oratoria. COMPARATIVE BIOCHEMISTRY AND PHYSIOLOGY. PART D, GENOMICS & PROTEOMICS 2024; 50:101234. [PMID: 38631126 DOI: 10.1016/j.cbd.2024.101234] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/03/2024] [Revised: 03/24/2024] [Accepted: 04/03/2024] [Indexed: 04/19/2024]
Abstract
Although polarized light can assist many animals in performing special visual tasks, current polarized light pollution (PLP) caused by urban construction has been shown to induce maladaptive behaviors of PL-sensitive animals and change ecological interactions. However, the underlying mechanisms remain unclear. Our previous work hypothesized that linearly polarized light (LPL) is an ecological trap for Oratosquilla oratoria, a common Stomatopoda species in the China Sea. Here we explored the underlying negative effects of artificially LPL on O. oratoria based on comparative transcriptomics. We identified 3616 differentially expressed genes (DEGs) in O. oratoria compound eyes continuous exposed to natural light (NL) and LPL scenarios. In comparison with the NL scenario, a total of 1972 up- and 1644 down- regulated genes were obtained from the O. oratoria compound eyes under LPL scenario, respectively. Furthermore, we performed functional annotation of those DEGs described above and identified 65 DEGs related to phototransduction, reproduction, immunity, and synapse. Based on the functional information, we suspected that continuous LPL exposure could block the light transmission, disrupt the reproductive process, and lead to the progressive failure of the immune response of O. oratoria. In conclusion, this study is the first to systematically describe the negative effects of artificial LPL exposure on O. oratoria at the genetic level, and it can improve the biological conservation theory behind PLP.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Xiuyu Qu
- School of Ocean, Yantai University, Yantai 264003, Shandong, China
| | - Qi Huang
- School of Food Science and Bioengineering, Yantai Institute of Technology, Yantai 264003, Shandong, China
| | - Huanjun Li
- Shandong Marine Resource and Environment Research Institute, Yantai 264003, Shandong, China
| | - Fangrui Lou
- School of Ocean, Yantai University, Yantai 264003, Shandong, China.
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Xue J, Qiu S, Jin W, Wang X. Self-calibration algorithm for installation angle deviation of bionic polarization compound eyes. OPTICS EXPRESS 2023; 31:25446-25466. [PMID: 37710431 DOI: 10.1364/oe.494842] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/05/2023] [Accepted: 07/05/2023] [Indexed: 09/16/2023]
Abstract
A self-calibration algorithm based on unsupervised optimization for polarizer installation angle deviation is proposed and used in a multi-aperture bionic polarization compound eye system. To simplify calibration operation, under the condition that the calibration-polarized light information is unknown, this algorithm fully exploits redundancy and random polarization information in the scene, and uses a non-convex multi-objective discrete parameter sorting optimization method to achieve angle self-calibration. Compared with ordinary calibration procedures, the algorithm requires less stringent conditions, achieves online calibration and is more accurate. It also can be applied to camera polarization arrays, division-of-focal-plane polarization cameras, and other polarization devices.
Collapse
|
5
|
Wilson RI. Neural Networks for Navigation: From Connections to Computations. Annu Rev Neurosci 2023; 46:403-423. [PMID: 37428603 DOI: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-110920-032645] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 07/12/2023]
Abstract
Many animals can navigate toward a goal they cannot see based on an internal representation of that goal in the brain's spatial maps. These maps are organized around networks with stable fixed-point dynamics (attractors), anchored to landmarks, and reciprocally connected to motor control. This review summarizes recent progress in understanding these networks, focusing on studies in arthropods. One factor driving recent progress is the availability of the Drosophila connectome; however, it is increasingly clear that navigation depends on ongoing synaptic plasticity in these networks. Functional synapses appear to be continually reselected from the set of anatomical potential synapses based on the interaction of Hebbian learning rules, sensory feedback, attractor dynamics, and neuromodulation. This can explain how the brain's maps of space are rapidly updated; it may also explain how the brain can initialize goals as stable fixed points for navigation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rachel I Wilson
- Department of Neurobiology, Harvard Medical School, Cambridge, Massachusetts, USA;
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Li T, Jiang Y, Yang X, Li H, Gong Z, Qin Y, Zhang J, Lu R, Wei G, Wu Y, Lu C. The effects of circularly polarized light on mating behavior and gene expression in Anomala corpulenta (Coleoptera: Scarabaeidae). Front Physiol 2023; 14:1172542. [PMID: 37064909 PMCID: PMC10102372 DOI: 10.3389/fphys.2023.1172542] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/23/2023] [Accepted: 03/15/2023] [Indexed: 04/03/2023] Open
Abstract
Light is an important abiotic factor affecting insect behavior. In nature, linearly polarized light is common, but circularly polarized light is rare. Left circularly polarized (LCP) light is selectively reflected by the exocuticle of most scarab beetles, including Anomala corpulenta. Despite our previous research showing that this visual signal probably mediates their mating behavior, the way in which it does so is not well elucidated. In this study, we investigated how LCP light affects not only mating behavior but also gene expression in this species using RNA-seq. The results indicated that disruption of LCP light reflection by females of A. corpulenta probably affects the process by which males of A. corpulenta search for mates. Furthermore, the RNA-seq results showed that genes of the environmental signaling pathways and also of several insect reproduction-related amino acid metabolic pathways were differentially expressed in groups exposed and not exposed to LCP light. This implies that A. corpulenta reproduction is probably regulated by LCP light-induced stress. Herein, the results show that LCP light is probably perceived by males of the species, further mediating their mating behavior. However, this hypothesis needs future verification with additional samples.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tong Li
- Henan Key Laboratory of Crop Pest Control, Key Laboratory of Integrated Pest Management on Crops in Southern Region of North China, Institute of Plant Protection, Henan Academy of Agricultural Sciences, Zhengzhou, China
| | - Yueli Jiang
- Henan Key Laboratory of Crop Pest Control, Key Laboratory of Integrated Pest Management on Crops in Southern Region of North China, Institute of Plant Protection, Henan Academy of Agricultural Sciences, Zhengzhou, China
| | - Xiaofan Yang
- Plant Protection Institute, Hebei Academy of Agricultural and Forestry Sciences, Baoding, China
| | - Huiling Li
- Henan Key Laboratory of Crop Pest Control, Key Laboratory of Integrated Pest Management on Crops in Southern Region of North China, Institute of Plant Protection, Henan Academy of Agricultural Sciences, Zhengzhou, China
| | - Zhongjun Gong
- Henan Key Laboratory of Crop Pest Control, Key Laboratory of Integrated Pest Management on Crops in Southern Region of North China, Institute of Plant Protection, Henan Academy of Agricultural Sciences, Zhengzhou, China
| | - Yifan Qin
- Henan Key Laboratory of Crop Pest Control, Key Laboratory of Integrated Pest Management on Crops in Southern Region of North China, Institute of Plant Protection, Henan Academy of Agricultural Sciences, Zhengzhou, China
| | - Jing Zhang
- Henan Key Laboratory of Crop Pest Control, Key Laboratory of Integrated Pest Management on Crops in Southern Region of North China, Institute of Plant Protection, Henan Academy of Agricultural Sciences, Zhengzhou, China
| | - Ruijie Lu
- Henan Key Laboratory of Crop Pest Control, Key Laboratory of Integrated Pest Management on Crops in Southern Region of North China, Institute of Plant Protection, Henan Academy of Agricultural Sciences, Zhengzhou, China
| | - Guoshu Wei
- College of Plant Protection, Hebei Agricultural University, Baoding, Hebei, China
| | - Yuqing Wu
- Henan Key Laboratory of Crop Pest Control, Key Laboratory of Integrated Pest Management on Crops in Southern Region of North China, Institute of Plant Protection, Henan Academy of Agricultural Sciences, Zhengzhou, China
- *Correspondence: Yuqing Wu, ; Chuantao Lu,
| | - Chuantao Lu
- Henan Key Laboratory of Crop Pest Control, Key Laboratory of Integrated Pest Management on Crops in Southern Region of North China, Institute of Plant Protection, Henan Academy of Agricultural Sciences, Zhengzhou, China
- *Correspondence: Yuqing Wu, ; Chuantao Lu,
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Freas CA, Spetch ML. Varieties of visual navigation in insects. Anim Cogn 2023; 26:319-342. [PMID: 36441435 PMCID: PMC9877076 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-022-01720-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/20/2022] [Revised: 11/10/2022] [Accepted: 11/15/2022] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
The behaviours and cognitive mechanisms animals use to orient, navigate, and remember spatial locations exemplify how cognitive abilities have evolved to suit a number of different mobile lifestyles and habitats. While spatial cognition observed in vertebrates has been well characterised in recent decades, of no less interest are the great strides that have also been made in characterizing and understanding the behavioural and cognitive basis of orientation and navigation in invertebrate models and in particular insects. Insects are known to exhibit remarkable spatial cognitive abilities and are able to successfully migrate over long distances or pinpoint known locations relying on multiple navigational strategies similar to those found in vertebrate models-all while operating under the constraint of relatively limited neural architectures. Insect orientation and navigation systems are often tailored to each species' ecology, yet common mechanistic principles can be observed repeatedly. Of these, reliance on visual cues is observed across a wide number of insect groups. In this review, we characterise some of the behavioural strategies used by insects to solve navigational problems, including orientation over short-distances, migratory heading maintenance over long distances, and homing behaviours to known locations. We describe behavioural research using examples from a few well-studied insect species to illustrate how visual cues are used in navigation and how they interact with non-visual cues and strategies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Cody A. Freas
- Department of Psychology, University of Alberta, Edmonton, AB Canada ,School of Natural Sciences, Macquarie University, Sydney, NSW Australia
| | - Marcia L. Spetch
- Department of Psychology, University of Alberta, Edmonton, AB Canada
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Yuan X, Liebelt MJ, Shi P, Phillips BJ. Cognitive decisions based on a rule-based fuzzy system. Inf Sci (N Y) 2022. [DOI: 10.1016/j.ins.2022.03.089] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
|
9
|
Sung JY, Harris OK, Hensley NM, Chemero AP, Morehouse NI. Beyond cognitive templates: re-examining template metaphors used for animal recognition and navigation. Integr Comp Biol 2021; 61:825-841. [PMID: 33970266 DOI: 10.1093/icb/icab040] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/31/2023] Open
Abstract
The term 'cognitive template' originated from work in human-based cognitive science to describe a literal, stored, neural representation used in recognition tasks. As the study of cognition has expanded to non-human animals, the term has diffused to describe a wider range of animal cognitive tools and strategies that guide action through the recognition of and discrimination between external states. One potential reason for this non-standardized meaning and variable employment is that researchers interested in the broad range of animal recognition tasks enjoy the simplicity of the cognitive template concept and have allowed it to become shorthand for many dissimilar or unknown neural processes without deep scrutiny of how this metaphor might comport with underlying neurophysiology. We review the functional evidence for cognitive templates in fields such as perception, navigation, communication, and learning, highlighting any neural correlates identified by these studies. We find that the concept of cognitive templates has facilitated valuable exploration at the interface between animal behavior and cognition, but the quest for a literal template has failed to attain mechanistic support at the level of neurophysiology. This may be the result of a misled search for a single physical locus for the 'template' itself. We argue that recognition and discrimination processes are best treated as emergent and, as such, may not be physically localized within single structures of the brain. Rather, current evidence suggests that such tasks are accomplished through synergies between multiple distributed processes in animal nervous systems. We thus advocate for researchers to move towards a more ecological, process-oriented conception, especially when discussing the neural underpinnings of recognition-based cognitive tasks.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jenny Y Sung
- Department of Biological Sciences, University of Cincinnati
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
10
|
Li D, Xu C, Zhang M, Wang X, Guo K, Sun Y, Gao J, Guo Z. Measuring glucose concentration in a solution based on the indices of polarimetric purity. BIOMEDICAL OPTICS EXPRESS 2021; 12:2447-2459. [PMID: 33996240 PMCID: PMC8086474 DOI: 10.1364/boe.414850] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/11/2020] [Revised: 03/19/2021] [Accepted: 03/23/2021] [Indexed: 05/20/2023]
Abstract
Polarization imaging is a powerful tool, which can be applied in biomedical diagnosis and many research fields. Here, we propose a new application of the indices of polarimetric purity (IPPs) composed of P1, P2, P3, to describe the glucose concentrations (GC) changes in the scattering system. The results suggest that P1 of the IPPs is a better indicator to GC in the solution than the degree of polarization (DoP) for the forward scattering detection. Meanwhile, the fitting relation among radius of scattering particle, GCs and P1 parameter has also been calculated, in which the error of inversion is no more than 4.73%. In the backscattering detection, the fitted frequency statistical histogram of the IPPs is used to measure the GCs, and their modes can represent changing trend of GCs.
Collapse
|
11
|
Homing in the arachnid taxa Araneae and Amblypygi. Anim Cogn 2020; 23:1189-1204. [PMID: 32894371 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-020-01424-w] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/22/2019] [Revised: 08/19/2020] [Accepted: 08/28/2020] [Indexed: 01/06/2023]
Abstract
Adequate homing is essential for the survival of any animal when it leaves its home to find prey or a mate. There are several strategies by which homing can be carried out: (a) retrace the outbound path; (b) use a 'cognitive map'; or (c) use path integration (PI). Here, I review the state of the art of research on spiders (Araneae) and whip spiders (Amblypygi) homing behaviour. The main strategy described in the literature as being used by these arachnids is PI. Behavioural and neural substrates of PI are described in a small group of spider families (Agelenidae, Lycosidae, Gnaphosidae, Ctenidae and Theraphosidae) and a whip spider family (Phrynidae). In spiders, the cues used to detect the position of the animal relative to its home are the position of the sun, polarized light patterns, web elasticity and landmarks. In whip spiders, the cues used are olfactory, tactile and, with a more minor role, visual. The use of a magnetic field in whip spiders has been rejected both with field and laboratory studies. Concerning the distance walked in PI, the possibility of using optic flow and idiothetic information in spiders is considered. The studies about outbound and inbound paths in whip spiders seem to suggest they do not follow the PI rules. As a conclusion, these arachnids' navigation relies on multimodal cues. We have detailed knowledge about the sensory origin (visual, olfactory, mechanosensory receptors) of neural information, but we are far from knowing the central neural structures where sensory information is integrated.
Collapse
|
12
|
Abstract
Preference for spatial locations to maximize favorable outcomes and minimize aversive experiences helps animals survive and adapt to the changing environment. Both visual and non-visual cues play a critical role in spatial navigation and memory of a place supports and guides these strategies. Here we present the neural, genetic and behavioral processes involved in place memory formation using Drosophila melanogaster with a focus on non-visual cue based spatial memories. The work presented here highlights the work done by Dr. Troy Zars and his colleagues with an emphasis on role of biogenic amines in learning, cell biological mechanisms of neural systems and behavioral plasticity of place conditioning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Divya Sitaraman
- Department of Psychology, College of Science, California State University-East Bay, Hayward, CA, USA
| | - Holly LaFerriere
- Department of Biology, Bemidji State University, Bemidji, MN, USA
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Shen F, Zhang M, Guo K, Zhou H, Peng Z, Cui Y, Wang F, Gao J, Guo Z. The depolarization performances of scattering systems based on the Indices of Polarimetric Purity (IPPs). OPTICS EXPRESS 2019; 27:28337-28349. [PMID: 31684587 DOI: 10.1364/oe.27.028337] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/02/2023]
Abstract
In this paper, the Indices of Polarimetric Purity (IPPs) [1-3] have been proposed to analyze the depolarization performances of mono-dispersion and poly-dispersion scattering systems. Here, we mainly investigate the influences of the particles' density, Refractive Index (RI) of the medium, incident wavelengths, the mixing ratio of bi-dispersion scattering particles and particle-size distributions of poly-dispersion scattering system on the depolarization performances for the backscattering detection. For the mono-dispersion scattering system under same incident wavelength, if the relative RI ratios (m) increase linearly, the depolarization performances of the system will first weaken and then strengthen, and of course, the incident wavelength and density of scattering particles will also influence the depolarization performances of the scattering system. For the bi-dispersion scattering system, the proportion of small particles will be negatively correlated with the depolarization property of the dispersion system, and meanwhile, the particle-size distributions will also affect the depolarization performances greatly in the poly-dispersion scattering system. The results demonstrate that the IPPs can be used to describe the depolarization performances of dispersion systems effectively.
Collapse
|
14
|
Le Moël F, Stone T, Lihoreau M, Wystrach A, Webb B. The Central Complex as a Potential Substrate for Vector Based Navigation. Front Psychol 2019; 10:690. [PMID: 31024377 PMCID: PMC6460943 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00690] [Citation(s) in RCA: 38] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/29/2018] [Accepted: 03/12/2019] [Indexed: 12/20/2022] Open
Abstract
Insects use path integration (PI) to maintain a home vector, but can also store and recall vector-memories that take them from home to a food location, and even allow them to take novel shortcuts between food locations. The neural circuit of the Central Complex (a brain area that receives compass and optic flow information) forms a plausible substrate for these behaviors. A recent model, grounded in neurophysiological and neuroanatomical data, can account for PI during outbound exploratory routes and the control of steering to return home. Here, we show that minor, hypothetical but neurally plausible, extensions of this model can additionally explain how insects could store and recall PI vectors to follow food-ward paths, take shortcuts, search at the feeder and re-calibrate their vector-memories with experience. In addition, a simple assumption about how one of multiple vector-memories might be chosen at any point in time can produce the development and maintenance of efficient routes between multiple locations, as observed in bees. The central complex circuitry is therefore well-suited to allow for a rich vector-based navigational repertoire.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Florent Le Moël
- Research Centre on Animal Cognition, Centre for Integrative Biology, CNRS, University of Toulouse, Toulouse, France
| | - Thomas Stone
- School of Informatics, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, United Kingdom
| | - Mathieu Lihoreau
- Research Centre on Animal Cognition, Centre for Integrative Biology, CNRS, University of Toulouse, Toulouse, France
| | - Antoine Wystrach
- Research Centre on Animal Cognition, Centre for Integrative Biology, CNRS, University of Toulouse, Toulouse, France
| | - Barbara Webb
- School of Informatics, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Marshall NJ, Powell SB, Cronin TW, Caldwell RL, Johnsen S, Gruev V, Chiou THS, Roberts NW, How MJ. Polarisation signals: a new currency for communication. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2019; 222:222/3/jeb134213. [PMID: 30733259 DOI: 10.1242/jeb.134213] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/25/2023]
Abstract
Most polarisation vision studies reveal elegant examples of how animals, mainly the invertebrates, use polarised light cues for navigation, course-control or habitat selection. Within the past two decades it has been recognised that polarised light, reflected, blocked or transmitted by some animal and plant tissues, may also provide signals that are received or sent between or within species. Much as animals use colour and colour signalling in behaviour and survival, other species additionally make use of polarisation signalling, or indeed may rely on polarisation-based signals instead. It is possible that the degree (or percentage) of polarisation provides a more reliable currency of information than the angle or orientation of the polarised light electric vector (e-vector). Alternatively, signals with specific e-vector angles may be important for some behaviours. Mixed messages, making use of polarisation and colour signals, also exist. While our knowledge of the physics of polarised reflections and sensory systems has increased, the observational and behavioural biology side of the story needs more (and more careful) attention. This Review aims to critically examine recent ideas and findings, and suggests ways forward to reveal the use of light that we cannot see.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- N Justin Marshall
- Queensland Brain Institute, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Queensland, 4072, Australia
| | - Samuel B Powell
- Queensland Brain Institute, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Queensland, 4072, Australia
| | - Thomas W Cronin
- Department of Biological Sciences, University of Maryland Baltimore County, MD 21250, USA
| | - Roy L Caldwell
- University of California Berkeley, Department of Integrative Biology, Berkeley, CA 94720-3140, USA
| | - Sonke Johnsen
- Department of Biology, Duke University, Durham, NC 27708-0338, USA
| | - Viktor Gruev
- Electrical and Computer Engineering, University of Illinois, Urbana, IL 61801, USA
| | - T-H Short Chiou
- Department of Life Sciences, National Cheng-Kung University, Tainan City 701, Taiwan
| | - Nicholas W Roberts
- School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Tyndall Avenue, Bristol BS8 1TQ, UK
| | - Martin J How
- School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Tyndall Avenue, Bristol BS8 1TQ, UK
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Dupeyroux J, Viollet S, Serres JR. Polarized skylight-based heading measurements: a bio-inspired approach. J R Soc Interface 2019; 16:20180878. [PMID: 30958149 PMCID: PMC6364636 DOI: 10.1098/rsif.2018.0878] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/22/2018] [Accepted: 12/20/2018] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Many insects such as desert ants, crickets, locusts, dung beetles, bees and monarch butterflies have been found to extract their navigation cues from the regular pattern of the linearly polarized skylight. These species are equipped with ommatidia in the dorsal rim area of their compound eyes, which are sensitive to the angle of polarization of the skylight. In the polarization-based robotic vision, most of the sensors used so far comprise high-definition CCD or CMOS cameras topped with linear polarizers. Here, we present a 2-pixel polarization-sensitive visual sensor, which was strongly inspired by the dorsal rim area of desert ants' compound eyes, designed to determine the direction of polarization of the skylight. The spectral sensitivity of this minimalistic sensor, which requires no lenses, is in the ultraviolet range. Five different methods of computing the direction of polarization were implemented and tested here. Our own methods, the extended and AntBot method, outperformed the other three, giving a mean angular error of only 0.62° ± 0.40° (median: 0.24°) and 0.69° ± 0.52° (median: 0.39°), respectively (mean ± standard deviation). The results obtained in outdoor field studies show that our celestial compass gives excellent results at a very low computational cost, which makes it highly suitable for autonomous outdoor navigation purposes.
Collapse
|
17
|
Quantification of ash sedimentation dynamics through depolarisation imaging with AshCam. Sci Rep 2018; 8:15680. [PMID: 30356119 PMCID: PMC6200762 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-018-34110-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/26/2018] [Accepted: 10/08/2018] [Indexed: 11/25/2022] Open
Abstract
Even modest ash-rich volcanic eruptions can severely impact a range of human activities, especially air travel. The dispersal of ash in these eruptions depends critically on aggregation and sedimentation processes – however these are difficult to quantify in volcanic plumes. Here, we image ash dynamics from mild explosive activity at Santiaguito Volcano, Guatemala, by measuring the depolarisation of scattered sunlight by non-spherical ash particles, allowing the dynamics of diffuse ash plumes to be investigated with high temporal resolution (>1 Hz). We measure the ash settling velocity downwind from the main plume, and compare it directly with ground sampled ash particles, finding good agreement with a sedimentation model based on particle size. Our new, cost-effective technique leverages existing technology, opening a new frontier of integrated ash visualisation and ground collection studies which could test models of ash coagulation and sedimentation, leading to improved ash dispersion forecasts. This will provide risk managers with improved data quality on ash location, reducing the economic and societal impacts of future ash-rich eruptions.
Collapse
|
18
|
Guan L, Li S, Zhai L, Liu S, Liu H, Lin W, Cui Y, Chu J, Xie H. Study on skylight polarization patterns over the ocean for polarized light navigation application. APPLIED OPTICS 2018; 57:6243-6251. [PMID: 30118011 DOI: 10.1364/ao.57.006243] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/10/2018] [Accepted: 06/25/2018] [Indexed: 06/08/2023]
Abstract
Polarized skylight navigation has excellent navigation performance with no error accumulation over time and low susceptibility to interference. The skylight polarization distribution contains rich directional information, such as the solar meridian, the neutral point, and the polarization angle, which plays a key role in the polarization navigation. But up to now the polarizations of both sunlit and moonlit skies have been investigated mainly over the land. In this work, the polarization distribution patterns of the skylight over the East China Sea and the Yellow Sea were studied. The polarization patterns were captured continuously during daytime and nighttime by using a full-sky imaging polarimetry system and then compared with the simulation results using the libRadtran radiative transfer software package. The result shows that the skylight polarization distribution over the sea has almost the same pattern as that on the land. The accuracy of the angle of polarization and the degree of polarization dropped significantly under the cloudy sky. It was found that when the ship sailed on the sea, the direction of the real meridian was close to the solar azimuth during the daytime and close to the lunar azimuth during the nighttime. It was also found that the nautical twilight polarization distribution was affected by both the solar polarization and the lunar polarization, but the solar polarization was dominant. The experiments show that the skylight polarization distribution pattern over the sea can still be applied in the field of polarization navigation. Thus, it is feasible for ships and unmanned aerial vehicles to use the polarized skylight to navigate and orient on the sea.
Collapse
|
19
|
Ilić M, Meglič A, Kreft M, Belušič G. The Fly Sensitizing Pigment Enhances UV Spectral Sensitivity While Preventing Polarization-Induced Artifacts. Front Cell Neurosci 2018; 12:34. [PMID: 29467626 PMCID: PMC5808286 DOI: 10.3389/fncel.2018.00034] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/08/2017] [Accepted: 01/25/2018] [Indexed: 12/31/2022] Open
Abstract
Microvillar photoreceptors are intrinsically capable of detecting the orientation of e-vector of linearly polarized light. They provide most invertebrates with an additional sensory channel to detect important features of their visual environment. However, polarization sensitivity (PS) of photoreceptors may lead to the detection of polarization-induced false colors and intensity contrasts. Most insect photoreceptors are thus adapted to have minimal PS. Flies have twisted rhabdomeres with microvilli rotated along the length of the ommatidia to reduce PS. The additional UV-absorbing sensitizing pigment on their opsin minimizes PS in the ultraviolet. We recorded voltage from Drosophila photoreceptors R1-6 to measure the spectral dependence of PS and found that PS in the UV is invariably negligible but can be substantial above 400 nm. Using modeling, we demonstrate that in R1-6 without the sensitizing pigment, PS in the UV (PS UV ) would exceed PS in the visible part of the spectrum (PS VIS ) by a factor PS UV /PS VIS = 1.2-1.8, as lower absorption of Rh1 rhodopsin reduces self-screening. We use polarimetric imaging of objects relevant to fly polarization vision to show that their degree of polarization outdoors is highest in the short-wavelength part of the spectrum. Thus, under natural illumination, the sensitizing pigment in R1-6 renders even those cells with high PS in the visible part unsuitable for proper polarization vision. We assume that fly ventral polarization vision can be mediated by R7 alone, with R1-6 serving as an unpolarized reference channel.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | | | - Gregor Belušič
- Department of Biology, Biotechnical Faculty, University of Ljubljana, Ljubljana, Slovenia
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Templin RM, How MJ, Roberts NW, Chiou TH, Marshall J. Circularly polarized light detection in stomatopod crustaceans: a comparison of photoreceptors and possible function in six species. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2017; 220:3222-3230. [PMID: 28667244 DOI: 10.1242/jeb.162941] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/16/2017] [Accepted: 06/27/2017] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
A combination of behavioural and electrophysiological experiments have previously shown that two species of stomatopod, Odontodactylus scyllarus and Gonodactylaceus falcatus, can differentiate between left- and right-handed circularly polarized light (CPL), and between CPL and linearly polarized light (LPL). It remains unknown if these visual abilities are common across all stomatopod species, and if so, how circular polarization sensitivity may vary between and within species. A subsection of the midband, a specialized region of stomatopod eyes, contains distally placed photoreceptor cells, termed R8 (retinular cell number 8). These cells are specifically built with unidirectional microvilli and appear to be angled precisely to convert CPL into LPL. They are mostly quarter-wave retarders for human visible light (400-700 nm), as well as being ultraviolet-sensitive linear polarization detectors. The effectiveness of the R8 cells in this role is determined by their geometric and optical properties. In particular, the length and birefringence of the R8 cells are crucial for retardation efficiency. Here, our comparative studies show that most species investigated have the theoretical ability to convert CPL into LPL, such that the handedness of an incoming circular reflection or signal could be discriminated. One species, Haptosquilla trispinosa, shows less than quarter-wave retardance. Whilst some species are known to produce circularly polarized reflections (some Odontodactylus species and G. falcatus, for example), others do not, so a variety of functions for this ability are worth considering.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rachel M Templin
- Queensland Brain Institute, University of Queensland, St Lucia, QLD 4072, Australia
| | - Martin J How
- School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol BS8 1TQ, UK
| | - Nicholas W Roberts
- School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol BS8 1TQ, UK
| | - Tsyr-Huei Chiou
- Department of Life Sciences, National Cheng Kung University, Tainan 701, Taiwan
| | - Justin Marshall
- Queensland Brain Institute, University of Queensland, St Lucia, QLD 4072, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Belušič G, Šporar K, Meglič A. Extreme polarisation sensitivity in the retina of the corn borer moth Ostrinia. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2017; 220:2047-2056. [PMID: 28341662 DOI: 10.1242/jeb.153718] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/22/2016] [Accepted: 03/16/2017] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
The visual system of the European corn borer (Ostrinia nubilalis) was analysed with microscopy and electrophysiological methods (electroretinograms and single-cell recordings). Ostrinia nubilalis has a pair of mainly ultraviolet-sensitive ocelli and a pair of compound eyes, maximally sensitive to green light. The ommatidia contain a tiered, fused rhabdom, consisting of the rhabdomeres of 9-12 photoreceptor cells with sensitivity peak wavelengths at 356, 413, 480 and 530 nm. The photoreceptors in a large dorsal rim area have straight rhabdomeres and high polarisation sensitivity (PS1,2=3.4, 14). Elsewhere, in the main retina, the majority of photoreceptors have non-aligned microvilli and negligible PS, but each ommatidium contains one or two blue-sensitive distal photoreceptors with straight microvilli parallel to the dorsoventral axis, yielding extremely high PS (PS1,2,3=56, 63, 316). Rhabdoms containing distal cells with potentially high PS have evolved at least twice: in moths (Crambidae, Noctuidae, Saturniidae), as well as in dung beetles (Scarabaeidae). The distal photoreceptors with high PS, sensitive to vertically polarised light, represent a monopolatic system, which is unsuitable for the proper analysis of electric field vector (e-vector) orientation. However, the distal photoreceptors might be used in conjunction with polarisation-insensitive photoreceptors to detect objects that reflect polarised light with stereotyped orientation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gregor Belušič
- Department of Biology, Biotechnical Faculty, University of Ljubljana, SI-1000 Ljubljana, Slovenia
| | - Katja Šporar
- Department of Biology, Biotechnical Faculty, University of Ljubljana, SI-1000 Ljubljana, Slovenia.,European Neuroscience Institute, D-37077 Goettingen, Germany
| | - Andrej Meglič
- Department of Biology, Biotechnical Faculty, University of Ljubljana, SI-1000 Ljubljana, Slovenia
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
A computational model of the integration of landmarks and motion in the insect central complex. PLoS One 2017; 12:e0172325. [PMID: 28241061 PMCID: PMC5328262 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0172325] [Citation(s) in RCA: 35] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/26/2016] [Accepted: 02/02/2017] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
The insect central complex (CX) is an enigmatic structure whose computational function has evaded inquiry, but has been implicated in a wide range of behaviours. Recent experimental evidence from the fruit fly (Drosophila melanogaster) and the cockroach (Blaberus discoidalis) has demonstrated the existence of neural activity corresponding to the animal's orientation within a virtual arena (a neural 'compass'), and this provides an insight into one component of the CX structure. There are two key features of the compass activity: an offset between the angle represented by the compass and the true angular position of visual features in the arena, and the remapping of the 270° visual arena onto an entire circle of neurons in the compass. Here we present a computational model which can reproduce this experimental evidence in detail, and predicts the computational mechanisms that underlie the data. We predict that both the offset and remapping of the fly's orientation onto the neural compass can be explained by plasticity in the synaptic weights between segments of the visual field and the neurons representing orientation. Furthermore, we predict that this learning is reliant on the existence of neural pathways that detect rotational motion across the whole visual field and uses this rotation signal to drive the rotation of activity in a neural ring attractor. Our model also reproduces the 'transitioning' between visual landmarks seen when rotationally symmetric landmarks are presented. This model can provide the basis for further investigation into the role of the central complex, which promises to be a key structure for understanding insect behaviour, as well as suggesting approaches towards creating fully autonomous robotic agents.
Collapse
|
23
|
Abstract
Vehicle state estimation is an essential prerequisite for navigation. The present approach seeks to use skylight polarization to facilitate state estimation under autonomous unconstrained flight conditions. Atmospheric scattering polarizes incident sunlight such that solar position is mathematically encoded in the resulting skylight polarization pattern. Indeed, several species of insects are able to sense skylight polarization and are believed to navigate polarimetrically. Sun-finding methodologies for polarized skylight navigation (PSN) have been proposed in the literature but typically rely on calibration updates to account for changing atmospheric conditions and/or are limited to 2D operation. To address this technology gap, a gradient-based PSN solution is developed based upon the Rayleigh sky model. The solution is validated in simulation, and effects of measurement error and changing atmospheric conditions are investigated. Finally, an experimental effort is described wherein polarimetric imagery is collected, ground-truth is established through independent imager-attitude measurement, the gradient-based PSN solution is applied, and results are analyzed.
Collapse
|
24
|
Abstract
The visual world is rich in linearly polarized light stimuli, which are hidden from the human eye. But many invertebrate species make use of polarized light as a source of valuable visual information. However, exploiting light polarization does not necessarily imply that the electric (e)-vector orientation of polarized light can be perceived as a separate modality of light. In this Review, I address the question of whether invertebrates can detect specific e-vector orientations in a manner similar to that of humans perceiving spectral stimuli as specific hues. To analyze e-vector orientation, the signals of at least three polarization-sensitive sensors (analyzer channels) with different e-vector tuning axes must be compared. The object-based, imaging polarization vision systems of cephalopods and crustaceans, as well as the water-surface detectors of flying backswimmers, use just two analyzer channels. Although this excludes the perception of specific e-vector orientations, a two-channel system does provide a coarse, categoric analysis of polarized light stimuli, comparable to the limited color sense of dichromatic, 'color-blind' humans. The celestial compass of insects employs three or more analyzer channels. However, that compass is multimodal, i.e. e-vector information merges with directional information from other celestial cues, such as the solar azimuth and the spectral gradient in the sky, masking e-vector information. It seems that invertebrate organisms take no interest in the polarization details of visual stimuli, but polarization vision grants more practical benefits, such as improved object detection and visual communication for cephalopods and crustaceans, compass readings to traveling insects, or the alert 'water below!' to water-seeking bugs.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Thomas Labhart
- Institute of Molecular Life Sciences, University of Zurich, Winterthurerstrasse 190, Zürich CH 8057, Switzerland
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Zhao H, Xu W. A Bionic Polarization Navigation Sensor and Its Calibration Method. SENSORS 2016; 16:s16081223. [PMID: 27527171 PMCID: PMC5017388 DOI: 10.3390/s16081223] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/09/2016] [Revised: 07/24/2016] [Accepted: 07/24/2016] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
The polarization patterns of skylight which arise due to the scattering of sunlight in the atmosphere can be used by many insects for deriving compass information. Inspired by insects' polarized light compass, scientists have developed a new kind of navigation method. One of the key techniques in this method is the polarimetric sensor which is used to acquire direction information from skylight. In this paper, a polarization navigation sensor is proposed which imitates the working principles of the polarization vision systems of insects. We introduce the optical design and mathematical model of the sensor. In addition, a calibration method based on variable substitution and non-linear curve fitting is proposed. The results obtained from the outdoor experiments provide support for the feasibility and precision of the sensor. The sensor's signal processing can be well described using our mathematical model. A relatively high degree of accuracy in polarization measurement can be obtained without any error compensation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Huijie Zhao
- School of Instrument Science and Opto-Electronics Engineering, Beihang University, No. 37 Xueyuan RD, Haidian, Beijing 100191, China.
| | - Wujian Xu
- School of Instrument Science and Opto-Electronics Engineering, Beihang University, No. 37 Xueyuan RD, Haidian, Beijing 100191, China.
| |
Collapse
|
26
|
Taylor GJ, Ribi W, Bech M, Bodey AJ, Rau C, Steuwer A, Warrant EJ, Baird E. The Dual Function of Orchid Bee Ocelli as Revealed by X-Ray Microtomography. Curr Biol 2016; 26:1319-24. [PMID: 27112298 DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.03.038] [Citation(s) in RCA: 41] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/14/2015] [Revised: 02/17/2016] [Accepted: 03/14/2016] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
Visually guided flight control in the rainforest is arguably one of the most complex insect behaviors: illumination varies dramatically depending on location [1], and the densely cluttered environment blocks out most of the sky [2]. What visual information do insects sample for flight control in this habitat? To begin answering this question, we determined the visual fields of the ocelli-thought to play a role in attitude stabilization of some flying insects [3-5]-of an orchid bee, Euglossa imperialis. High-resolution 3D models of the ocellar system from X-ray microtomography were used for optical ray tracing simulations. Surprisingly, these showed that each ocellus possesses two distinct visual fields-a focused monocular visual field suitable for detecting features elevated above the horizon and therefore assisting with flight stabilization [3-5] and, unlike other ocelli investigated to date [4, 6, 7], a large trinocular fronto-dorsal visual field shared by all ocelli. Histological analyses show that photoreceptors have similar orientations within each ocellus and are likely to be sensitive to polarized light, as in some other hymenopterans [7, 8]. We also found that the average receptor orientation is offset between the ocelli, each having different axes of polarization sensitivity relative to the head. Unlike the eyes of any other insect described to date, this ocellar system meets the requirements of a true polarization analyzer [9, 10]. The ocelli of E. imperialis could provide sensitive compass information for navigation in the rainforest and, additionally, provide cues for visual discrimination or flight control.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gavin J Taylor
- Department of Biology, Lund University, Lund 223 62, Sweden.
| | - Willi Ribi
- Research School of Biology, Australian National University, Canberra, ACT, 2601, Australia
| | - Martin Bech
- Department of Clinical Sciences, Lund University, Lund 223 62, Sweden
| | | | | | - Axel Steuwer
- Nelson Mandela Metropolitan University, Port Elizabeth 6011, South Africa
| | - Eric J Warrant
- Department of Biology, Lund University, Lund 223 62, Sweden
| | - Emily Baird
- Department of Biology, Lund University, Lund 223 62, Sweden.
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Where paths meet and cross: navigation by path integration in the desert ant and the honeybee. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 2015; 201:533-46. [PMID: 25971358 DOI: 10.1007/s00359-015-1000-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/15/2014] [Revised: 03/09/2015] [Accepted: 03/10/2015] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
Abstract
Animals that travel large distances in search of food need to be equipped with navigation systems that are capable of keeping track of the distance and direction of travel throughout their outbound journey, so that they may return home expeditiously and without losing their way. The challenge of homing is especially acute when the environment is devoid of landmarks. Desert ants and honeybees are able to meet this challenge, despite their minuscule brains and restricted computational capacity. This article reviews some of the processes and mechanisms that underlie the homing abilities of these creatures, which are among the best-understood navigators in the animal kingdom.
Collapse
|
28
|
Photoreceptor projections and receptive fields in the dorsal rim area and main retina of the locust eye. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 2015; 201:427-40. [PMID: 25715758 DOI: 10.1007/s00359-015-0990-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/01/2014] [Revised: 02/10/2015] [Accepted: 02/12/2015] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
Abstract
In many insect species, photoreceptors of a small dorsal rim area of the eye are specialized for sensitivity to the oscillation plane of polarized skylight and, thus, serve a role in sky compass orientation. To further understand peripheral mechanisms of polarized-light processing in the optic lobe, we have studied the projections of photoreceptors and their receptive fields in the main eye and dorsal rim area of the desert locust, a model system for polarization vision analysis. In both eye regions, one photoreceptor per ommatidium, R7, has a long visual fiber projecting through the lamina to the medulla. Axonal fibers from R7 receptors of the dorsal rim area have short side branches throughout the depth of the dorsal lamina and maintain retinotopic projections to the dorsal medulla following the first optic chiasma. Receptive fields of dorsal rim photoreceptors are considerably larger (average acceptance angle 33°) than those of the main eye (average acceptance angle 2.04°) and, taken together, cover almost the entire sky. The data challenge previous reports of two long visual fibers per ommatidium in the main eye of the locust and provide data for future analysis of peripheral networks underlying polarization opponency in the locust brain.
Collapse
|
29
|
Volkov V. Discovering electrophysiology in photobiology: A brief overview of several photobiological processes with an emphasis on electrophysiology. Commun Integr Biol 2014; 7:e28423. [PMID: 25328636 PMCID: PMC4183612 DOI: 10.4161/cib.28423] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/29/2013] [Revised: 03/01/2014] [Accepted: 03/03/2014] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
The mini-review gives special attention to holistic approach and mechanisms of processes. The physical and chemical frames and background for visual perception and signaling are discussed. Perception of photons by retinal rod cells is described in more detail starting from photon absorption and culminating in ion currents. Dark noise and temperature-dependence of photocurrents in photoreceptor cells are analyzed. Perception of polarized light, its effects and informational importance are discussed based on underlying mechanisms and specialized morphological structures of biological organisms. Role of statistics of photons in photoreception is questioned. The review also pinpoints new and developing directions and raises questions for future research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Vadim Volkov
- Faculty of Life Sciences and Computing; London Metropolitan University; London, UK
| |
Collapse
|
30
|
|
31
|
Evangelista C, Kraft P, Dacke M, Labhart T, Srinivasan MV. Honeybee navigation: critically examining the role of the polarization compass. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2014; 369:20130037. [PMID: 24395964 DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0037] [Citation(s) in RCA: 36] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Although it is widely accepted that honeybees use the polarized-light pattern of the sky as a compass for navigation, there is little direct evidence that this information is actually sensed during flight. Here, we ask whether flying bees can obtain compass cues derived purely from polarized light, and communicate this information to their nest-mates through the 'waggle dance'. Bees, from an observation hive with vertically oriented honeycombs, were trained to fly to a food source at the end of a tunnel, which provided overhead illumination that was polarized either parallel to the axis of the tunnel, or perpendicular to it. When the illumination was transversely polarized, bees danced in a predominantly vertical direction with waggles occurring equally frequently in the upward or the downward direction. They were thus using the polarized-light information to signal the two possible directions in which they could have flown in natural outdoor flight: either directly towards the sun, or directly away from it. When the illumination was axially polarized, the bees danced in a predominantly horizontal direction with waggles directed either to the left or the right, indicating that they could have flown in an azimuthal direction that was 90° to the right or to the left of the sun, respectively. When the first half of the tunnel provided axial illumination and the second half transverse illumination, bees danced along all of the four principal diagonal directions, which represent four equally likely locations of the food source based on the polarized-light information that they had acquired during their journey. We conclude that flying bees are capable of obtaining and signalling compass information that is derived purely from polarized light. Furthermore, they deal with the directional ambiguity that is inherent in polarized light by signalling all of the possible locations of the food source in their dances, thus maximizing the chances of recruitment to it.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- C Evangelista
- Queensland Brain Institute, The University of Queensland, , St. Lucia, Queensland, Australia
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
32
|
How MJ, Christy J, Roberts NW, Marshall NJ. Null point of discrimination in crustacean polarisation vision. J Exp Biol 2014; 217:2462-7. [DOI: 10.1242/jeb.103457] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
Abstract
The polarisation of light is used by many species of cephalopods and crustaceans to discriminate objects or to communicate. Most visual systems with this ability, such as that of the fiddler crab, include receptors with photopigments that are oriented horizontally and vertically relative to the outside world. Photoreceptors in such an orthogonal array are maximally sensitive to polarised light with the same fixed e-vector orientation. Using opponent neural connections, this two-channel system may produce a single value of polarisation contrast and, consequently, it may suffer from null points of discrimination. Stomatopod crustaceans use a different system for polarisation vision, comprising at least four types of polarisation-sensitive photoreceptor arranged at 0°, 45°, 90° and 135° relative to each other, in conjunction with extensive rotational eye movements. This anatomical arrangement should not suffer from equivalent null points of discrimination. To test whether these two systems were vulnerable to null points, we presented the fiddler crab Uca heteropleura and the stomatopod Haptosquilla trispinosa with polarised looming stimuli on a modified LCD monitor. The fiddler crab was less sensitive to differences in the degree of polarised light when the e-vector was at -45°, than when the e-vector was horizontal. In comparison, stomatopods showed no difference in sensitivity between the two stimulus types. The results suggest that fiddler crabs suffer from a null point of sensitivity, while stomatopods do not.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - John Christy
- Smithsonian Tropical Research Institute, Republic of Panama
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
33
|
Fuss T, Bleckmann H, Schluessel V. The shark Chiloscyllium griseum can orient using turn responses before and after partial telencephalon ablation. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 2013; 200:19-35. [PMID: 24114617 DOI: 10.1007/s00359-013-0858-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/21/2013] [Revised: 09/18/2013] [Accepted: 09/19/2013] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
Abstract
This study assessed spatial memory and orientation strategies in Chiloscyllium griseum. In the presence of visual landmarks, six sharks were trained in a fixed turn response. Group 1 started from two possible compartments approaching two goal locations, while group 2 started from and approached only one location, respectively. The learning criterion was reached within 9 ± 5.29 (group 1) and 8.3 ± 3.51 sessions (group 2). Transfer tests revealed that sharks had applied a direction strategy, possibly in combination with some form of place learning. Without visual cues, sharks relied solely on the former. To identify the underlying neural substrate(s), telencephalic were lesioned and performance compared before and after surgery. Ablation of the dorsal and medial pallia only had an effect on one shark (group 1), indicating that the acquisition and retention of previously gained knowledge were unaffected in the remaining four individuals. Nonetheless, the shark re-learned the task. In summary, C. griseum can utilize fixed turn responses to navigate to a goal; there is also some evidence for the use of external visual landmarks while orienting. Probably, strategies can be used alone or in combination. Neither the dorsal nor medial pallium seems to be responsible for the acquisition and processing of egocentric information.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Theodora Fuss
- Abteilung für vergleichende Sinnes- und Neurobiologie, Institut für Zoologie, Rheinische Friedrich-Wilhelms-Universität Bonn, Meckenheimer Allee 169, 53115, Bonn, Germany,
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
34
|
Cartron L, Dickel L, Shashar N, Darmaillacq AS. Maturation of polarization and luminance contrast sensitivities in cuttlefish (Sepia officinalis). ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2013; 216:2039-45. [PMID: 23430993 DOI: 10.1242/jeb.080390] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
Polarization sensitivity is a characteristic of the visual system of cephalopods. It has been well documented in adult cuttlefish, which use polarization sensitivity in a large range of tasks such as communication, orientation and predation. Because cuttlefish do not benefit from parental care, their visual system (including the ability to detect motion) must be efficient from hatching to enable them to detect prey or predators. We studied the maturation and functionality of polarization sensitivity in newly hatched cuttlefish. In a first experiment, we examined the response of juvenile cuttlefish from hatching to the age of 1 month towards a moving, vertically oriented grating (contrasting and polarized stripes) using an optomotor response apparatus. Cuttlefish showed differences in maturation of polarization versus luminance contrast motion detection. In a second experiment, we examined the involvement of polarization information in prey preference and detection in cuttlefish of the same age. Cuttlefish preferentially chose not to attack transparent prey whose polarization contrast had been removed with a depolarizing filter. Performances of prey detection based on luminance contrast improved with age. Polarization contrast can help cuttlefish detect transparent prey. Our results suggest that polarization is not a simple modulation of luminance information, but rather that it is processed as a distinct channel of visual information. Both luminance and polarization sensitivity are functional, though not fully matured, in newly hatched cuttlefish and seem to help in prey detection.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lelia Cartron
- Université de Caen Basse-Normandie, Groupe Mémoire et Plasticité Comportementale (EA 4259), F-14032 Caen, France
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
35
|
Karman SB, Diah SZM, Gebeshuber IC. Bio-inspired polarized skylight-based navigation sensors: a review. SENSORS (BASEL, SWITZERLAND) 2012; 12:14232-61. [PMID: 23202158 PMCID: PMC3522911 DOI: 10.3390/s121114232] [Citation(s) in RCA: 31] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/05/2012] [Revised: 10/15/2012] [Accepted: 10/15/2012] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Animal senses cover a broad range of signal types and signal bandwidths and have inspired various sensors and bioinstrumentation devices for biological and medical applications. Insects, such as desert ants and honeybees, for example, utilize polarized skylight pattern-based information in their navigation activities. They reliably return to their nests and hives from places many kilometers away. The insect navigation system involves the dorsal rim area in their compound eyes and the corresponding polarization sensitive neurons in the brain. The dorsal rim area is equipped with photoreceptors, which have orthogonally arranged small hair-like structures termed microvilli. These are the specialized sensors for the detection of polarized skylight patterns (e-vector orientation). Various research groups have been working on the development of novel navigation systems inspired by polarized skylight-based navigation in animals. Their major contributions are critically reviewed. One focus of current research activities is on imitating the integration path mechanism in desert ants. The potential for simple, high performance miniaturized bioinstrumentation that can assist people in navigation will be explored.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Salmah B. Karman
- Institute of Microengineering and Nanoelectronics, Universiti Kebangsaan Malaysia, 43600 UKM Bangi, Selangor, Malaysia; E-Mails: (S.Z.M.D.); (I.C.G.)
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Faculty of Engineering, University of Malaya, 50603 Kuala Lumpur, Malaysia
| | - S. Zaleha M. Diah
- Institute of Microengineering and Nanoelectronics, Universiti Kebangsaan Malaysia, 43600 UKM Bangi, Selangor, Malaysia; E-Mails: (S.Z.M.D.); (I.C.G.)
| | - Ille C. Gebeshuber
- Institute of Microengineering and Nanoelectronics, Universiti Kebangsaan Malaysia, 43600 UKM Bangi, Selangor, Malaysia; E-Mails: (S.Z.M.D.); (I.C.G.)
- Institute of Applied Physics, Vienna University of Technology, Wiedner Hauptstrasse 8-10/134, 1040 Vienna, Austria
| |
Collapse
|
36
|
Abstract
Foraging- and feeding-related behaviors across eumetazoans share similar molecular mechanisms, suggesting the early evolution of an optimal foraging behavior called area-restricted search (ARS), involving mechanisms of dopamine and glutamate in the modulation of behavioral focus. Similar mechanisms in the vertebrate basal ganglia control motor behavior and cognition and reveal an evolutionary progression toward increasing internal connections between prefrontal cortex and striatum in moving from amphibian to primate. The basal ganglia in higher vertebrates show the ability to transfer dopaminergic activity from unconditioned stimuli to conditioned stimuli. The evolutionary role of dopamine in the modulation of goal-directed behavior and cognition is further supported by pathologies of human goal-directed cognition, which have motor and cognitive dysfunction and organize themselves, with respect to dopaminergic activity, along the gradient described by ARS, from perseverative to unfocused. The evidence strongly supports the evolution of goal-directed cognition out of mechanisms initially in control of spatial foraging but, through increasing cortical connections, eventually used to forage for information.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Thomas T Hills
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Indiana University
| |
Collapse
|
37
|
Henze MJ, Dannenhauer K, Kohler M, Labhart T, Gesemann M. Opsin evolution and expression in arthropod compound eyes and ocelli: insights from the cricket Gryllus bimaculatus. BMC Evol Biol 2012; 12:163. [PMID: 22935102 PMCID: PMC3502269 DOI: 10.1186/1471-2148-12-163] [Citation(s) in RCA: 53] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/17/2012] [Accepted: 08/24/2012] [Indexed: 11/28/2022] Open
Abstract
Background Opsins are key proteins in animal photoreception. Together with a light-sensitive group, the chromophore, they form visual pigments which initiate the visual transduction cascade when photoactivated. The spectral absorption properties of visual pigments are mainly determined by their opsins, and thus opsins are crucial for understanding the adaptations of animal eyes. Studies on the phylogeny and expression pattern of opsins have received considerable attention, but our knowledge about insect visual opsins is still limited. Up to now, researchers have focused on holometabolous insects, while general conclusions require sampling from a broader range of taxa. We have therefore investigated visual opsins in the ocelli and compound eyes of the two-spotted cricket Gryllus bimaculatus, a hemimetabolous insect. Results Phylogenetic analyses place all identified cricket sequences within the three main visual opsin clades of insects. We assign three of these opsins to visual pigments found in the compound eyes with peak absorbances in the green (515 nm), blue (445 nm) and UV (332 nm) spectral range. Their expression pattern divides the retina into distinct regions: (1) the polarization-sensitive dorsal rim area with blue- and UV-opsin, (2) a newly-discovered ventral band of ommatidia with blue- and green-opsin and (3) the remainder of the compound eye with UV- and green-opsin. In addition, we provide evidence for two ocellar photopigments with peak absorbances in the green (511 nm) and UV (350 nm) spectral range, and with opsins that differ from those expressed in the compound eyes. Conclusions Our data show that cricket eyes are spectrally more specialized than has previously been assumed, suggesting that similar adaptations in other insect species might have been overlooked. The arrangement of spectral receptor types within some ommatidia of the cricket compound eyes differs from the generally accepted pattern found in holometabolous insect taxa and awaits a functional explanation. From the opsin phylogeny, we conclude that gene duplications, which permitted differential opsin expression in insect ocelli and compound eyes, occurred independently in several insect lineages and are recent compared to the origin of the eyes themselves.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Miriam J Henze
- Department of Biology, Lund University, Sölvegatan 35, 22362 Lund, Sweden.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
38
|
Posnien N, Hopfen C, Hilbrant M, Ramos-Womack M, Murat S, Schönauer A, Herbert SL, Nunes MDS, Arif S, Breuker CJ, Schlötterer C, Mitteroecker P, McGregor AP. Evolution of eye morphology and rhodopsin expression in the Drosophila melanogaster species subgroup. PLoS One 2012; 7:e37346. [PMID: 22662147 PMCID: PMC3360684 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0037346] [Citation(s) in RCA: 39] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/24/2012] [Accepted: 04/18/2012] [Indexed: 01/31/2023] Open
Abstract
A striking diversity of compound eye size and shape has evolved among insects. The number of ommatidia and their size are major determinants of the visual sensitivity and acuity of the compound eye. Each ommatidium is composed of eight photoreceptor cells that facilitate the discrimination of different colours via the expression of various light sensitive Rhodopsin proteins. It follows that variation in eye size, shape, and opsin composition is likely to directly influence vision. We analyzed variation in these three traits in D. melanogaster, D. simulans and D. mauritiana. We show that D. mauritiana generally has larger eyes than its sibling species, which is due to a combination of larger ommatidia and more ommatidia. In addition, intra- and inter-specific differences in eye size among D. simulans and D. melanogaster strains are mainly caused by variation in ommatidia number. By applying a geometric morphometrics approach to assess whether the formation of larger eyes influences other parts of the head capsule, we found that an increase in eye size is associated with a reduction in the adjacent face cuticle. Our shape analysis also demonstrates that D. mauritiana eyes are specifically enlarged in the dorsal region. Intriguingly, this dorsal enlargement is associated with enhanced expression of rhodopsin 3 in D. mauritiana. In summary, our data suggests that the morphology and functional properties of the compound eyes vary considerably within and among these closely related Drosophila species and may be part of coordinated morphological changes affecting the head capsule.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nico Posnien
- Department of Biological and Medical Sciences, Oxford Brookes University, Oxford, United Kingdom
- Institut für Populationsgenetik, Veterinärmedizinische Universität Wien, Vienna, Austria
| | - Corinna Hopfen
- Institut für Populationsgenetik, Veterinärmedizinische Universität Wien, Vienna, Austria
| | - Maarten Hilbrant
- Department of Biological and Medical Sciences, Oxford Brookes University, Oxford, United Kingdom
| | - Margarita Ramos-Womack
- Howard Hughes Medical Institute, Princeton University, Princeton, New Jersey, United States of America
- Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, Princeton University, Princeton, New Jersey, United States of America
| | - Sophie Murat
- Department of Biological and Medical Sciences, Oxford Brookes University, Oxford, United Kingdom
- Institut für Populationsgenetik, Veterinärmedizinische Universität Wien, Vienna, Austria
| | - Anna Schönauer
- Institut für Populationsgenetik, Veterinärmedizinische Universität Wien, Vienna, Austria
| | - Samantha L. Herbert
- Champalimaud Neuroscience Programme, Champalimaud Centre for the Unknown, Lisbon, Portugal
| | - Maria D. S. Nunes
- Department of Biological and Medical Sciences, Oxford Brookes University, Oxford, United Kingdom
- Institut für Populationsgenetik, Veterinärmedizinische Universität Wien, Vienna, Austria
| | - Saad Arif
- Department of Biological and Medical Sciences, Oxford Brookes University, Oxford, United Kingdom
- Institut für Populationsgenetik, Veterinärmedizinische Universität Wien, Vienna, Austria
| | - Casper J. Breuker
- Department of Biological and Medical Sciences, Oxford Brookes University, Oxford, United Kingdom
| | - Christian Schlötterer
- Institut für Populationsgenetik, Veterinärmedizinische Universität Wien, Vienna, Austria
| | - Philipp Mitteroecker
- Department of Theoretical Biology, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
- * E-mail: (PM); or (APM)
| | - Alistair P. McGregor
- Department of Biological and Medical Sciences, Oxford Brookes University, Oxford, United Kingdom
- Institut für Populationsgenetik, Veterinärmedizinische Universität Wien, Vienna, Austria
- * E-mail: (PM); or (APM)
| |
Collapse
|
39
|
Visual control of navigation in insects and its relevance for robotics. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2012; 21:535-43. [PMID: 21689925 DOI: 10.1016/j.conb.2011.05.020] [Citation(s) in RCA: 64] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/07/2011] [Revised: 05/02/2011] [Accepted: 05/24/2011] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
Flying insects display remarkable agility, despite their diminutive eyes and brains. This review describes our growing understanding of how these creatures use visual information to stabilize flight, avoid collisions with objects, regulate flight speed, detect and intercept other flying insects such as mates or prey, navigate to a distant food source, and orchestrate flawless landings. It also outlines the ways in which these insights are now being used to develop novel, biologically inspired strategies for the guidance of autonomous, airborne vehicles.
Collapse
|
40
|
Wernet MF, Velez MM, Clark DA, Baumann-Klausener F, Brown JR, Klovstad M, Labhart T, Clandinin TR. Genetic dissection reveals two separate retinal substrates for polarization vision in Drosophila. Curr Biol 2012; 22:12-20. [PMID: 22177904 PMCID: PMC3258365 DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.11.028] [Citation(s) in RCA: 79] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/26/2011] [Revised: 11/14/2011] [Accepted: 11/15/2011] [Indexed: 10/14/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Linearly polarized light originates from atmospheric scattering or surface reflections and is perceived by insects, spiders, cephalopods, crustaceans, and some vertebrates. Thus, the neural basis underlying how this fundamental quality of light is detected is of broad interest. Morphologically unique, polarization-sensitive ommatidia exist in the dorsal periphery of many insect retinas, forming the dorsal rim area (DRA). However, much less is known about the retinal substrates of behavioral responses to polarized reflections. SUMMARY Drosophila exhibits polarotactic behavior, spontaneously aligning with the e-vector of linearly polarized light, when stimuli are presented either dorsally or ventrally. By combining behavioral experiments with genetic dissection and ultrastructural analyses, we show that distinct photoreceptors mediate the two behaviors: inner photoreceptors R7+R8 of DRA ommatidia are necessary and sufficient for dorsal polarotaxis, whereas ventral responses are mediated by combinations of outer and inner photoreceptors, both of which manifest previously unknown features that render them polarization sensitive. CONCLUSIONS Drosophila uses separate retinal pathways for the detection of linearly polarized light emanating from the sky or from shiny surfaces. This work establishes a behavioral paradigm that will enable genetic dissection of the circuits underlying polarization vision.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Mariel M. Velez
- Department of Neurobiology, Stanford University, Stanford CA94305
| | - Damon A. Clark
- Department of Neurobiology, Stanford University, Stanford CA94305
| | | | - Julian R. Brown
- HHMI and Department of Neurobiology, Stanford, California 94305, USA
| | - Martha Klovstad
- Department of Neurobiology, Stanford University, Stanford CA94305
| | - Thomas Labhart
- Institute of Molecular Life Sciences, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
| | | |
Collapse
|
41
|
el Jundi B, Pfeiffer K, Homberg U. A distinct layer of the medulla integrates sky compass signals in the brain of an insect. PLoS One 2011; 6:e27855. [PMID: 22114712 PMCID: PMC3218074 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0027855] [Citation(s) in RCA: 45] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/08/2011] [Accepted: 10/26/2011] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Mass migration of desert locusts is a common phenomenon in North Africa and the Middle East but how these insects navigate is still poorly understood. Laboratory studies suggest that locusts are able to exploit the sky polarization pattern as a navigational cue. Like other insects locusts detect polarized light through a specialized dorsal rim area (DRA) of the eye. Polarization signals are transmitted through the optic lobe to the anterior optic tubercle (AOTu) and, finally, to the central complex in the brain. Whereas neurons of the AOTu integrate sky polarization and chromatic cues in a daytime dependent manner, the central complex holds a topographic representation of azimuthal directions suggesting a role as an internal sky compass. To understand further the integration of sky compass cues we studied polarization-sensitive (POL) neurons in the medulla that may be intercalated between DRA photoreceptors and AOTu neurons. Five types of POL-neuron were characterized and four of these in multiple recordings. All neurons had wide arborizations in medulla layer 4 and most, additionally, in the dorsal rim area of the medulla and in the accessory medulla, the presumed circadian clock. The neurons showed type-specific orientational tuning to zenithal polarized light and azimuth tuning to unpolarized green and UV light spots. In contrast to neurons of the AOTu, we found no evidence for color opponency and daytime dependent adjustment of sky compass signals. Therefore, medulla layer 4 is a distinct stage in the integration of sky compass signals that precedes the time-compensated integration of celestial cues in the AOTu.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Basil el Jundi
- Department of Biology, Animal Physiology, Philipps-University, Marburg, Germany
| | - Keram Pfeiffer
- Department of Physiology and Biophysics, Dalhousie University, Halifax, Canada
| | - Uwe Homberg
- Department of Biology, Animal Physiology, Philipps-University, Marburg, Germany
- * E-mail:
| |
Collapse
|
42
|
Mota T, Yamagata N, Giurfa M, Gronenberg W, Sandoz JC. Neural organization and visual processing in the anterior optic tubercle of the honeybee brain. J Neurosci 2011; 31:11443-56. [PMID: 21832175 PMCID: PMC6623125 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.0995-11.2011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 49] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/23/2011] [Revised: 06/01/2011] [Accepted: 06/03/2011] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
The honeybee Apis mellifera represents a valuable model for studying the neural segregation and integration of visual information. Vision in honeybees has been extensively studied at the behavioral level and, to a lesser degree, at the physiological level using intracellular electrophysiological recordings of single neurons. However, our knowledge of visual processing in honeybees is still limited by the lack of functional studies of visual processing at the circuit level. Here we contribute to filling this gap by providing a neuroanatomical and neurophysiological characterization at the circuit level of a practically unstudied visual area of the bee brain, the anterior optic tubercle (AOTu). First, we analyzed the internal organization and neuronal connections of the AOTu. Second, we established a novel protocol for performing optophysiological recordings of visual circuit activity in the honeybee brain and studied the responses of AOTu interneurons during stimulation of distinct eye regions. Our neuroanatomical data show an intricate compartmentalization and connectivity of the AOTu, revealing a dorsoventral segregation of the visual input to the AOTu. Light stimuli presented in different parts of the visual field (dorsal, lateral, or ventral) induce distinct patterns of activation in AOTu output interneurons, retaining to some extent the dorsoventral input segregation revealed by our neuroanatomical data. In particular, activity patterns evoked by dorsal and ventral eye stimulation are clearly segregated into distinct AOTu subunits. Our results therefore suggest an involvement of the AOTu in the processing of dorsoventrally segregated visual information in the honeybee brain.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Theo Mota
- Centre de Recherches sur la Cognition Animale, Université de Toulouse, Université Paul Sabatier and
- Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), F-31062 Toulouse, France
| | - Nobuhiro Yamagata
- Centre de Recherches sur la Cognition Animale, Université de Toulouse, Université Paul Sabatier and
- Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), F-31062 Toulouse, France
| | - Martin Giurfa
- Centre de Recherches sur la Cognition Animale, Université de Toulouse, Université Paul Sabatier and
- Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), F-31062 Toulouse, France
| | - Wulfila Gronenberg
- Department of Neuroscience, University of Arizona, Tucson, Arizona 85721, and
| | - Jean-Christophe Sandoz
- Centre de Recherches sur la Cognition Animale, Université de Toulouse, Université Paul Sabatier and
- Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), F-31062 Toulouse, France
- Laboratoire Evolution, Génomes et Spéciation, CNRS, 91198 Gif-sur-Yvette, France
| |
Collapse
|
43
|
Schechner YY. Inversion by P4: polarization-picture post-processing. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2011; 366:638-48. [PMID: 21282167 DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0205] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Polarization may be sensed by imaging modules. This is done in various engineering systems as well as in biological systems, specifically by insects and some marine species. However, polarization per pixel is usually not the direct variable of interest. Rather, polarization-related data serve as a cue for recovering task-specific scene information. How should polarization-picture post-processing (P(4)) be done for the best scene understanding? Answering this question is not only helpful for advanced engineering (computer vision), but also to prompt hypotheses as to the processing occurring within biological systems. In various important cases, the answer is found by a principled expression of scene recovery as an inverse problem. Such an expression relies directly on a physics-based model of effects in the scene. The model includes analysis that depends on the different polarization components, thus facilitating the use of these components during the inversion, in a proper, even if non-trivial, manner. We describe several examples for this approach. These include automatic removal of path radiance in haze or underwater, overcoming partial semireflections and visual reverberations; three-dimensional recovery and distance-adaptive denoising. The resulting inversion algorithms rely on signal-processing methods, such as independent component analysis, deconvolution and optimization.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yoav Y Schechner
- Department of Electrical Engineering, Technion, Israel Institute of Technology, Haifa 32000, Israel.
| |
Collapse
|
44
|
Homberg U, Heinze S, Pfeiffer K, Kinoshita M, el Jundi B. Central neural coding of sky polarization in insects. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2011; 366:680-7. [PMID: 21282171 DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0199] [Citation(s) in RCA: 166] [Impact Index Per Article: 11.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Many animals rely on a sun compass for spatial orientation and long-range navigation. In addition to the Sun, insects also exploit the polarization pattern and chromatic gradient of the sky for estimating navigational directions. Analysis of polarization-vision pathways in locusts and crickets has shed first light on brain areas involved in sky compass orientation. Detection of sky polarization relies on specialized photoreceptor cells in a small dorsal rim area of the compound eye. Brain areas involved in polarization processing include parts of the lamina, medulla and lobula of the optic lobe and, in the central brain, the anterior optic tubercle, the lateral accessory lobe and the central complex. In the optic lobe, polarization sensitivity and contrast are enhanced through convergence and opponency. In the anterior optic tubercle, polarized-light signals are integrated with information on the chromatic contrast of the sky. Tubercle neurons combine responses to the UV/green contrast and e-vector orientation of the sky and compensate for diurnal changes of the celestial polarization pattern associated with changes in solar elevation. In the central complex, a topographic representation of e-vector tunings underlies the columnar organization and suggests that this brain area serves as an internal compass coding for spatial directions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Uwe Homberg
- Department of Biology, Animal Physiology, University of Marburg, 35032 Marburg, Germany.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
45
|
Kraft P, Evangelista C, Dacke M, Labhart T, Srinivasan MV. Honeybee navigation: following routes using polarized-light cues. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2011; 366:703-8. [PMID: 21282174 DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0203] [Citation(s) in RCA: 37] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
While it is generally accepted that honeybees (Apis mellifera) are capable of using the pattern of polarized light in the sky to navigate to a food source, there is little or no direct behavioural evidence that they actually do so. We have examined whether bees can be trained to find their way through a maze composed of four interconnected tunnels, by using directional information provided by polarized light illumination from the ceilings of the tunnels. The results show that bees can learn this task, thus demonstrating directly, and for the first time, that bees are indeed capable of using the polarized-light information in the sky as a compass to steer their way to a food source.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- P Kraft
- Queensland Brain Institute, The University of Queensland, Saint Lucia, Queensland 4072, Australia
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
46
|
Shashar N, Johnsen S, Lerner A, Sabbah S, Chiao CC, Mäthger LM, Hanlon RT. Underwater linear polarization: physical limitations to biological functions. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2011; 366:649-54. [PMID: 21282168 DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0190] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Polarization sensitivity is documented in a range of marine animals. The variety of tasks for which animals can use this sensitivity, and the range over which they do so, are confined by the visual systems of these animals and by the propagation of the polarization information in the aquatic environment. We examine the environmental physical constraints in an attempt to reveal the depth, range and other limitations to the use of polarization sensitivity by marine animals. In clear oceanic waters, navigation that is based on the polarization pattern of the sky appears to be limited to shallow waters, while solar-based navigation is possible down to 200-400 m. When combined with intensity difference, polarization sensitivity allows an increase in target detection range by 70-80% with an upper limit of 15 m for large-eyed animals. This distance will be significantly smaller for small animals, such as plankton, and in turbid waters. Polarization-contrast detection, which is relevant to object detection and communication, is strongly affected by water conditions and in clear waters its range limit may reach 15 m as well. We show that polarization sensitivity may also serve for target distance estimation, when examining point source bioluminescent objects in the photic mesopelagic depth range.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nadav Shashar
- Department of Life Sciences, Eilat Campus, Ben Gurion University, Hatmarim St, Eilat 88000, Israel.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
47
|
Sakura M, Okada R, Aonuma H. Evidence for instantaneous e-vector detection in the honeybee using an associative learning paradigm. Proc Biol Sci 2011; 279:535-42. [PMID: 21733901 DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2011.0929] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/25/2023] Open
Abstract
Many insects use the polarization pattern of the sky for obtaining compass information during orientation or navigation. E-vector information is collected by a specialized area in the dorsal-most part of the compound eye, the dorsal rim area (DRA). We tested honeybees' capability of learning certain e-vector orientations by using a classical conditioning paradigm with the proboscis extension reflex. When one e-vector orientation (CS+) was associated with sugar water, while another orientation (CS-) was not rewarded, the honeybees could discriminate CS+ from CS-. Bees whose DRA was inactivated by painting did not learn CS+. When ultraviolet (UV) polarized light (350 nm) was used for CS, the bees discriminated CS+ from CS-, but no discrimination was observed in blue (442 nm) or green light (546 nm). Our data indicate that honeybees can learn and discriminate between different e-vector orientations, sensed by the UV receptors of the DRA, suggesting that bees can determine their flight direction from polarized UV skylight during foraging. Fixing the bees' heads during the experiments did not prevent learning, indicating that they use an 'instantaneous' algorithm of e-vector detection; that is, the bees do not need to actively scan the sky with their DRAs ('sequential' method) to determine e-vector orientation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Midori Sakura
- Laboratory of Neurocybernetics, Research Institute for Electronic Science, Hokkaido University, Kita 12, Nishi 7, Sapporo, Hokkaido 060-0812, Japan.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
48
|
Srinivasan MV. Honeybees as a model for the study of visually guided flight, navigation, and biologically inspired robotics. Physiol Rev 2011; 91:413-60. [PMID: 21527730 DOI: 10.1152/physrev.00005.2010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 107] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Research over the past century has revealed the impressive capacities of the honeybee, Apis mellifera, in relation to visual perception, flight guidance, navigation, and learning and memory. These observations, coupled with the relative ease with which these creatures can be trained, and the relative simplicity of their nervous systems, have made honeybees an attractive model in which to pursue general principles of sensorimotor function in a variety of contexts, many of which pertain not just to honeybees, but several other animal species, including humans. This review begins by describing the principles of visual guidance that underlie perception of the world in three dimensions, obstacle avoidance, control of flight speed, and orchestrating smooth landings. We then consider how navigation over long distances is accomplished, with particular reference to how bees use information from the celestial compass to determine their flight bearing, and information from the movement of the environment in their eyes to gauge how far they have flown. Finally, we illustrate how some of the principles gleaned from these studies are now being used to design novel, biologically inspired algorithms for the guidance of unmanned aerial vehicles.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mandyam V Srinivasan
- Queensland Brain Institute and School of Information Technology and Electrical Engineering, University of Queensland, and ARC Center of Excellence in Vision Science, St. Lucia, Australia.
| |
Collapse
|
49
|
Polarization-sensitive descending neurons in the locust: connecting the brain to thoracic ganglia. J Neurosci 2011; 31:2238-47. [PMID: 21307260 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.3624-10.2011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/19/2023] Open
Abstract
Many animal species, in particular insects, exploit the E-vector pattern of the blue sky for sun compass navigation. Like other insects, locusts detect dorsal polarized light via photoreceptors in a specialized dorsal rim area of the compound eye. Polarized light information is transmitted through several processing stages to the central complex, a brain area involved in the control of goal-directed orientation behavior. To investigate how polarized light information is transmitted to thoracic motor circuits, we studied the responses of locust descending neurons to polarized light. Three sets of polarization-sensitive descending neurons were characterized through intracellular recordings from axonal fibers in the neck connectives combined with single-cell dye injections. Two descending neurons from the brain, one with ipsilaterally and the second with contralaterally descending axon, are likely to bridge the gap between polarization-sensitive neurons in the brain and thoracic motor centers. In both neurons, E-vector tuning changed linearly with daytime, suggesting that they signal time-compensated spatial directions, an important prerequisite for navigation using celestial signals. The third type connects the suboesophageal ganglion with the prothoracic ganglion. It showed no evidence for time compensation in E-vector tuning and might play a role in flight stabilization and control of head movements.
Collapse
|
50
|
El Jundi B, Homberg U. Evidence for the possible existence of a second polarization-vision pathway in the locust brain. JOURNAL OF INSECT PHYSIOLOGY 2010; 56:971-979. [PMID: 20488187 DOI: 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2010.05.011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/25/2010] [Revised: 05/11/2010] [Accepted: 05/11/2010] [Indexed: 05/29/2023]
Abstract
For spatial orientation and navigation, many insects derive compass information from the polarization pattern of the blue sky. The desert locust Schistocerca gregaria detects polarized light with a specialized dorsal rim area of its compound eye. In the locust brain, polarized-light signals are passed through the anterior optic tract and tubercle to the central complex which most likely serves as an internal sky compass. Here, we suggest that neurons of a second visual pathway, via the accessory medulla and posterior optic tubercle, also provide polarization information to the central complex. Intracellular recordings show that two types of neuron in this posterior pathway are sensitive to polarized light. One cell type connects the dorsal rim area of the medulla with the medulla and accessory medulla, and a second type connects the bilaterally paired posterior optic tubercles. Given the evidence for a role of the accessory medulla as the master clock controlling circadian changes in behavioral activity in flies and cockroaches, our data open the possibility that time-compensated polarized-light signals may reach the central complex via this pathway for time-compensated sky-compass navigation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Basil El Jundi
- Fachbereich Biologie, Tierphysiologie, Philipps-Universität Marburg, D-35032 Marburg, Germany
| | | |
Collapse
|