1
|
Zefferman MR, Baumgarten MD, Trumble BC, Mathew S. Little evidence that posttraumatic stress is associated with diurnal hormone dysregulation in Turkana pastoralists. Evol Med Public Health 2025; 13:77-91. [PMID: 40196852 PMCID: PMC11973635 DOI: 10.1093/emph/eoaf004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/26/2024] [Revised: 01/10/2025] [Indexed: 04/09/2025] Open
Abstract
Research in industrialized populations suggests that posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) may be associated with decreased cortisol or testosterone sensitivity, resulting in a blunted diurnal rhythm. However, the evolutionary implications of this association are unclear. Studies have primarily been conducted in Western industrialized populations, so we do not know whether hormonal blunting is a reliable physiological response to PTSD or stems from factors unique to industrialized settings. Furthermore, existing studies combine PTSD from diverse types of traumas, and comparison groups with and without PTSD differ along multiple dimensions, making it hard to know if PTSD or other life factors drive the blunted cortisol response. We conducted a study among n = 60 male Turkana pastoralists, aged between about 18-65 years in Kenya, exposed to high levels of lethal inter-ethnic cattle raiding. 28% of men in this area have PTSD symptom severity that would qualify them for a provisional PTSD diagnosis. Saliva samples were collected at three points to compare the cortisol and testosterone profiles of Turkana warriors with and without PTSD. Contrary to existing work, our preregistered analysis found little evidence for a difference in the hormonal profiles of warriors with high versus low PTSD symptom severity. Our results imply that the relationship between PTSD and hormonal diurnal variation may vary across populations and ecologies or that the association documented in Western populations stems from other correlated life factors. Studies in a wider range of populations and ecological contexts are needed to understand the evolutionary underpinnings of hormonal responses to trauma.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Matthew R Zefferman
- Department of Defense Analysis, Naval Postgraduate School, Monterey, CA, 93943, USA
- Institute of Human Origins, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ, 85281, USA
| | - Michael D Baumgarten
- Institute of Human Origins, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ, 85281, USA
- School of Human Evolution and Social Change, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ, 85281, USA
| | - Benjamin C Trumble
- Institute of Human Origins, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ, 85281, USA
- School of Human Evolution and Social Change, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ, 85281, USA
- Center for Evolution and Medicine, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ, 85281, USA
| | - Sarah Mathew
- Institute of Human Origins, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ, 85281, USA
- School of Human Evolution and Social Change, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ, 85281, USA
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Watanabe DK, Kitayama S, Williams DP, Thayer JF. Emotion suppression differentially moderates the link between stress and cardiovascular disease risk in Japanese and Americans. Int J Clin Health Psychol 2025; 25:100555. [PMID: 40110442 PMCID: PMC11919598 DOI: 10.1016/j.ijchp.2025.100555] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/11/2024] [Accepted: 02/17/2025] [Indexed: 03/22/2025] Open
Abstract
Background Cardiovascular disease (CVD) remains a key cause of mortality worldwide. Prior work has found that the association between stress and cardiovascular outcomes is moderated by emotion regulation (ER) and expressive suppression (i.e., emotion inhibition), which is linked with adverse outcomes (i.e., inflammation) in Western (Americans) but not Eastern (Japanese) populations. Existing cultural differences in biological stress responses and suppression use suggest that these factors may have different implications for CV outcomes. Objective We address this gap in the literature by examining if ER differentially moderates the relationship between stress and CVD risk between Japanese and American adults. Method Participants were from the Midlife in Japan and Midlife in the United States studies and had complete biomarker and psychological data (Japanese: N = 315, M age = 59.22, 149 females; Americans: N = 524, M age = 51.98, 291 females). Stress was indexed using the perceived stress scale. Trait suppression and reappraisal were indexed using the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire. CVD risk was indexed using a composite score of body mass index, C-reactive protein, interleukin-6, systolic blood pressure, and high-density lipoprotein cholesterol ratio. Results Adjusting for age, sex, education, tobacco, alcohol, and prescription medication use, linear regressions revealed robust cultural differences among those with high suppression (r = -0.10 [-0.19, -0.01]). Higher stress was linked with higher CVD risk in Americans regardless of the level of reappraisal or suppression (r's > 0.11, p's < 0.07). In contrast, among Japanese with high suppression, higher stress was associated with lower CVD risk (r = -0.09 [-0.23, 0.05]). Higher stress was associated with greater inflammation among Japanese with lower suppression (r = 0.10 [-0.07, 0.28]). Conclusions Consistent with prior work, these findings suggest that adaptive ER moderates the association between stress and CVD risk, and that suppression may not be universally 'maladaptive.' Results emphasize the importance of considering cultural context when assessing the impact of emotion suppression on health, which may help explain differences in CVD outcomes between individuals from Eastern and Western populations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Darcianne K Watanabe
- School of Social Ecology, c/o Department of Psychological Science, University of California, Irvine, 4201 Social and Behavioral Sciences Gateway, Irvine, CA, 92697, USA
| | - Shinobu Kitayama
- Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, 1004 East Hall, 530 Church Street, Ann Arbor, MI, 48109, USA
| | - DeWayne P Williams
- Department of Psychological Science, University of California, Irvine, 4201 Social and Behavioral Sciences Gateway, Irvine, CA, 92697, USA
| | - Julian F Thayer
- Department of Psychological Science, University of California, Irvine, 4201 Social and Behavioral Sciences Gateway, Irvine, CA, 92697, USA
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Novak L, Kiknadze N. Does the good life feel good? The role of positive emotion in competing conceptions of the good life. Front Psychol 2024; 15:1425415. [PMID: 39171224 PMCID: PMC11335640 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1425415] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/29/2024] [Accepted: 07/26/2024] [Indexed: 08/23/2024] Open
Abstract
Flourishing refers to one kind of generalized wellbeing. Contemporary flourishing research often privileges positive emotion in the theorization and measurement of the construct, such that flourishing is frequently conceptualized as involving a predominance of positive over negative emotions. Positive emotions are thus, on some views of flourishing, seen as an essential component of "the good life." This paper explores the nuanced variations in conceptions of the good life, focusing on the interplay between positive emotion and flourishing. Through an analysis of contemporary perspectives on flourishing, we underscore the diversity in conceptualizations of flourishing and the implications of this diversity for flourishing theorists. Our review reveals significant disparities in perspectives regarding the significance of positive emotion in the pursuit of a good life. Furthermore, we delineate the theoretical distinctions between objective-list approaches and functional approaches to flourishing, highlighting their respective advantages and limitations. Theoretical dissensus persists regarding whether positive emotion is a necessary constituent of the good life, thus prompting a critical examination of the justification for its inclusion in flourishing models. Finally, we emphasize the need for greater theoretical clarity in defining wellbeing to inform both research endeavors and societal discourse. We suggest that an adequate appreciation of variation in the development and maintenance of flourishing requires admitting for more complex relationships between the construct and both positive and negative emotionality, while embracing the cultural and individual variety that are unavoidable in accurate models of human life.
Collapse
|
4
|
Park J, Kitayama S, Miyamoto Y. When High Subjective Social Status Becomes a Burden: A Japan-U.S. Comparison of Biological Health Markers. PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN 2024; 50:1098-1112. [PMID: 37002677 PMCID: PMC11143766 DOI: 10.1177/01461672231162747] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/11/2022] [Accepted: 02/19/2023] [Indexed: 11/05/2023]
Abstract
High subjective social status (SSS) is believed to protect health in the current literature. However, high SSS entails social responsibilities that can be stressful in collectivistic cultural contexts. Here, we tested the hypothesis that those socialized in collectivistic societies (e.g., Japan) recognize their high social status as entailing social duties difficult to ignore even when they are excessive. Using cross-cultural survey data (N = 1,289) and a measure of biological health risk (BHR) by biomarkers of inflammation and cardiovascular malfunction, we found that higher SSS predicted lower BHR for American males. In contrast, higher SSS predicted higher BHR for Japanese males, mediated by the perceived difficulty of disengaging from their current goals. In both cultural groups, females showed no association between SSS and BHR. These findings suggest that social status has differing health implications, depending on the relative salience of privileges and burden-producing responsibilities in different cultural contexts.
Collapse
|
5
|
Kitayama S, Salvador CE. Cultural Psychology: Beyond East and West. Annu Rev Psychol 2024; 75:495-526. [PMID: 37585666 DOI: 10.1146/annurev-psych-021723-063333] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 08/18/2023]
Abstract
Research in cultural psychology over the last three decades has revealed the profound influence of culture on cognitive, emotional, and motivational processes shaping individuals into active agents. This article aims to show cultural psychology's promise in three key steps. First, we review four notable cultural dimensions believed to underlie cultural variations: independent versus interdependent self, individualism versus collectivism, tightness versus looseness of social norms, and relational mobility. Second, we examine how ecology and geography shape human activities and give rise to organized systems of cultural practices and meanings, called eco-cultural complexes. In turn, the eco-cultural complex of each zone is instrumental in shaping a wide range of psychological processes, revealing a psychological diversity that extends beyond the scope of the current East-West literature. Finally, we examine some of the non-Western cultural zones present today, including Arab, East Asian, Latin American, and South Asian zones, and discuss how they may have contributed, to varying degrees, to the formation of the contemporary Western cultural zone.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Shinobu Kitayama
- Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan, USA;
| | - Cristina E Salvador
- Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, North Carolina, USA;
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Hoemann K, Gendron M, Crittenden AN, Mangola SM, Endeko ES, Dussault È, Barrett LF, Mesquita B. What We Can Learn About Emotion by Talking With the Hadza. PERSPECTIVES ON PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE 2024; 19:173-200. [PMID: 37428509 PMCID: PMC10776822 DOI: 10.1177/17456916231178555] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 07/11/2023]
Abstract
Emotions are often thought of as internal mental states centering on individuals' subjective feelings and evaluations. This understanding is consistent with studies of emotion narratives, or the descriptions people give for experienced events that they regard as emotions. Yet these studies, and contemporary psychology more generally, often rely on observations of educated Europeans and European Americans, constraining psychological theory and methods. In this article, we present observations from an inductive, qualitative analysis of interviews conducted with the Hadza, a community of small-scale hunter-gatherers in Tanzania, and juxtapose them with a set of interviews conducted with Americans from North Carolina. Although North Carolina event descriptions largely conformed to the assumptions of eurocentric psychological theory, Hadza descriptions foregrounded action and bodily sensations, the physical environment, immediate needs, and the experiences of social others. These observations suggest that subjective feelings and internal mental states may not be the organizing principle of emotion the world around. Qualitative analysis of emotion narratives from outside of a U.S. (and western) cultural context has the potential to uncover additional diversity in meaning-making, offering a descriptive foundation on which to build a more robust and inclusive science of emotion.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | | | | | | | | | - Lisa Feldman Barrett
- Department of Psychology, Northeastern University
- Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Massachusetts
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital, Massachusetts
| | | |
Collapse
|
7
|
Yin H, Han Z, Li Y. Traditional bullying, cyberbullying, and quality of life among adolescents in 35 countries: Do cultural values matter? Soc Sci Med 2024; 340:116499. [PMID: 38103495 DOI: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2023.116499] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/27/2023] [Revised: 11/21/2023] [Accepted: 12/05/2023] [Indexed: 12/19/2023]
Abstract
RATIONALE Substantial evidence indicates that involvement in school bullying has been linked to lower quality of life (QoL). Yet there is little information elucidating the link between bullying involvement and QoL in different cultures. OBJECTIVE This study investigated the associations between different types of bullying involvement and QoL among adolescents. The moderating roles of cultural values in the relationship between bullying involvement and QoL were examined. METHODS This study included representative samples from 35 countries (N = 184,017) using data from the 2017/2018 Health Behavior in School-Aged Children (HBSC) survey and the new 2023 World Cultural Map Scores from the World Values Survey Wave 7 (2017/2021). Multilevel mixed-effects analysis was employed to examine individual-level and country-level effects simultaneously. RESULTS All types of bullying involvement were associated with lower QoL, with being a victim having the lowest QoL. Adolescents in societies that prioritize higher self-expression values exhibited a reduced propensity to engage in school bullying. In countries with higher self-expression values, there was a more pronounced negative association between bullying involvement and QoL. Traditional/Secular-rational values had no significant and consistent moderating effect on the association between bullying involvement and QoL. Those results were quite similar for both traditional bullying and cyberbullying. CONCLUSION These findings imply that cultural values are closely intertwined with adolescent bullying and quality of life. Therefore, prevention and intervention programs should pay more attention to addressing bullying and promoting the QoL of adolescents according to various cultural values.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hui Yin
- School of Political Science and Public Administration, Shandong University, China.
| | - Ziqiang Han
- School of Political Science and Public Administration, Shandong University, China.
| | - Yuhuan Li
- School of Government, Central University of Finance and Economics, China.
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Kitayama S, Rossmaier A. Cultural evolution needed to complete the Grossmann theory. Behav Brain Sci 2023; 46:e67. [PMID: 37154356 DOI: 10.1017/s0140525x22001790] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/10/2023]
Abstract
Grossmann used evolutionary analysis to argue for the adaptive nature of fearfulness. This analysis, however, falls short of addressing why negative affectivity is maladaptive in contemporary Western societies. Here, we fill the gap by documenting the implied cultural variation and considering cultural (rather than biological) evolution over the last 10,000 years to explain the observed cultural variation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Shinobu Kitayama
- Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI 48109, USA ; https://lsa.umich.edu/psych/people/faculty/kitayama.html ; https://lsa.umich.edu/psych/people/graduate-students/arossman.html
| | - Amelie Rossmaier
- Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI 48109, USA ; https://lsa.umich.edu/psych/people/faculty/kitayama.html ; https://lsa.umich.edu/psych/people/graduate-students/arossman.html
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Liew K, Uchida Y, Domae H, Koh AHQ. Energetic music is used for anger downregulation: A cross‐cultural differentiation of intensity from rhythmic arousal. JOURNAL OF APPLIED SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 2022. [DOI: 10.1111/jasp.12951] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/05/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Kongmeng Liew
- Graduate School of Human and Environmental Studies Kyoto University Kyoto Japan
- Graduate School of Science and Technology Nara Institute of Science and Technology Ikoma Japan
| | - Yukiko Uchida
- Institute for the Future of Human Society Kyoto University Kyoto Japan
| | - Hiina Domae
- Graduate School of Human and Environmental Studies Kyoto University Kyoto Japan
| | - Alethea H. Q. Koh
- Institute for the Future of Human Society Kyoto University Kyoto Japan
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Nelson MH. Resentment Is Like Drinking Poison? The Heterogeneous Health Effects of Affective Polarization. JOURNAL OF HEALTH AND SOCIAL BEHAVIOR 2022; 63:508-524. [PMID: 35148647 PMCID: PMC9716484 DOI: 10.1177/00221465221075311] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/14/2023]
Abstract
Affective polarization-the tendency for individuals to exhibit animosity toward those on the opposite side of the partisan divide-has increased in the United States in recent years. This article presents evidence that this trend may have consequences for Americans' health. Structural equation model analyses of nationally representative survey data from Pew Research Center's American Trends Panel (n = 4,685) showed heterogeneous relationships between affectively polarized attitudes and self-rated health. On one hand, such attitudes were directly negatively associated with health such that the polarized political environment was proposed to operate as a sociopolitical stressor. Simultaneously, affective polarization was positively associated with political participation, which in turn was positively associated with health, although the direct negative effect was substantially larger than the indirect positive one. These results suggest that today's increasingly hostile and pervasive form of partisanship may undermine Americans' health even as it induces greater political engagement.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Micah H. Nelson
- University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, NC, USA
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Uchida Y, Nakayama M, Bowen KS. Interdependence of Emotion: Conceptualization, Evidence, and Social Implications From Cultural Psychology. CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE 2022. [DOI: 10.1177/09637214221109584] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
People feel a wide range of emotions. In many psychological traditions, emotions are defined as primarily emerging from within the individual, even if influenced by external factors (e.g., approval from other people). This definition is consistent with an independent self-construal. However, in some contexts, emotions are understood to have more interdependent characteristics that can be shared with other people and that arise from social contexts and collective, shared situations. We define the lay theory of interdependence of emotion as the perception that emotional experience or its causes and consequences are shared with other people. Interdependence of emotion can be conceptualized along a spectrum, rather than as categorical. Additionally, the degree to which people understand emotions as interdependent likely varies by cultural context. In this article, we review studies that have investigated this lay theory of emotions across cultures, focusing on function. We suggest that people from non-WEIRD cultures (i.e., cultures that are not Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich, and Democratic) are more likely than others to experience emotions as interdependent. Next, we highlight examples of this interdependence, focusing on two specific emotions: happiness and awe, which may have both independent and interdependent elements. The mechanisms and functions of the lay theory of interdependence of emotions are discussed using the example of a current collective threat, COVID-19.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yukiko Uchida
- Institute for the Future of Human Society, Kyoto University
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
12
|
Tung NYC, Yap Y, Bei B, Luecken LJ, Wiley JF. A 14-day ecological momentary assessment study on whether resilience and early family risk moderate daily stress and affect on cortisol diurnal slope. Sci Rep 2022; 12:1240. [PMID: 35075226 PMCID: PMC8786880 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-022-05277-w] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/03/2021] [Accepted: 12/21/2021] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
Abstract
This study examined whether resilience capacity moderates the association of daily perceived stress and affect with cortisol diurnal slope among relocated emerging adults. Relocated undergraduates (N = 98; aged 18-25 years) were recruited from three groups: Resilient, Vulnerable, and Control. The Resilient group required Risky Family Questionnaire (RFQ) scores ≥ 29 and Brief Resilience Scale (BRS) scores ≥ 3.6. The Vulnerable group required RFQ scores ≥ 29 and BRS scores ≤ 3. The comparison Control group required RFQ scores ≤ 21 and T-scores < 60 on PROMIS anxiety and depression symptoms. Mixed-effects models were used to test the unique associations of perceived stress, negative affect, and positive affect x group interactions (predictors) on diurnal cortisol slope (outcome) across 14 consecutive days. The Resilient group did not moderate the associations between daily stress or affect on cortisol diurnal slope. Instead, both the Resilient and Vulnerable groups with early family risk, showed a steeper diurnal slope unique to higher stress and a flatter slope unique to higher negative affect. Results suggest that riskier early family life was significantly associated with altered cortisol diurnal slope outcomes to stress (i.e., demand) and negative affect (i.e., distress). These associations were not attenuated by current resilience capacity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Natasha Yan Chi Tung
- School of Psychological Sciences and Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, Monash University, 18 Innovation Walk, Melbourne, VIC, 3800, Australia
| | - Yang Yap
- School of Psychological Sciences and Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, Monash University, 18 Innovation Walk, Melbourne, VIC, 3800, Australia
| | - Bei Bei
- School of Psychological Sciences and Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, Monash University, 18 Innovation Walk, Melbourne, VIC, 3800, Australia
| | - Linda J Luecken
- Department of Psychology, Arizona State University, Tempe, USA
| | - Joshua F Wiley
- School of Psychological Sciences and Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, Monash University, 18 Innovation Walk, Melbourne, VIC, 3800, Australia.
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Leung AK, Koh B, Phang R, Lee STH, Huang T. Linking Creativity to Psychological Well‐being: Integrative Insights from the Instrumental Emotion Regulation Theory. JOURNAL OF CREATIVE BEHAVIOR 2021. [DOI: 10.1002/jocb.524] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022]
|
14
|
Miyamoto Y, Ryff CD. Culture and Health: Recent Developments and Future Directions
1. JAPANESE PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2021; 64:90-108. [PMID: 35509718 PMCID: PMC9060271 DOI: 10.1111/jpr.12378] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/05/2023]
Abstract
Evidence of cultural differences in relationships and emotions has accumulated over the past few decades. As findings on cultural differences in psychological processes have accumulated, there has been growing interest in investigating whether they have implications for other phenomena such as health. Using scientific advances from the MIDUS and MIDJA studies, both publicly available, we examine links between culture and health. We first briefly review the accumulated evidence on cultural influences on health correlates of psychosocial factors. We then feature two recent developments - a more micro-level perspective on biological factors that may be involved in the culture and health linkage, and a more macro-level view of socioeconomic inequality, which also matters for health. Both perspectives inform the pathways through which health effects occur. Finally, we conclude our review by highlighting the changing historical contexts surrounding these cross-cultural investigations. Specifically, we draw attention to widening of economic inequality across cultures and the world-wide COVID-19 pandemic. These happenings bring notable implications for future research on health across cultural contexts.
Collapse
|
15
|
Joseph NT, Chow EC, Peterson LM, Kamarck TW, Clinton M, DeBruin M. What Can We Learn From More Than 140,000 Moments of Ecological Momentary Assessment-Assessed Negative Emotion and Ambulatory Blood Pressure? A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Psychosom Med 2021; 83:746-755. [PMID: 34267091 DOI: 10.1097/psy.0000000000000966] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE Two decades of research has examined within-person associations between negative emotion states and ambulatory blood pressure (ABP) using ecological momentary assessment (EMA), but no meta-analysis has been conducted. We conducted this systematic review and meta-analysis to quantify the magnitude of this association and identify moderators, review strengths and weaknesses in conceptual and measurement approaches, and provide recommendations. METHODS We searched databases (PsycINFO, PubMed), identified 15 studies, and obtained data from 13 studies (n = 2511; 142,307 observations). RESULTS Random-effects meta-analyses demonstrated small effect r values between momentary negative emotions and systolic ABP (r = 0.06) and diastolic ABP (r = 0.05; p values < .001). Meta-regressions found that effects were larger among studies focused on anxiety, multidimensional negative emotions, predominantly female samples, or less observations of each participant (p values from .003 to .049). A qualitative review found that few studies examined moderators contributing to the substantial interindividual differences in this association. CONCLUSIONS The small association between momentary negative emotion and ABP extends laboratory findings on the association between the experiential and physiological aspects of emotion to the daily, natural emotional experiences of individuals. This literature could be strengthened by determining interindividual and intraindividual moderators of this association (e.g., trait negative emotion and state positive emotion), examining differential associations of different negative emotions with ABP, and standardizing EMA protocols. Although the effect is small, to the extent that repeated emotion-related cardiovascular reactivity may contribute to cardiovascular disease risk, identifying daily life triggers of emotion is important.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nataria T Joseph
- From the Department of Psychology (Joseph, Chow), Pepperdine University, Malibu, California; Department of Psychology (Peterson), Bryn Mawr College, Bryn Mawr; Departments of Psychology and Psychiatry (Kamarck), University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania; McGovern Medical School, University of Texas Health Science Center at Houston, Houston, Texas (Clinton); and Department of Psychology, Fuller Theological Seminary, Pasadena, California (DeBruin)
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
16
|
Kitayama S, Park J. Is Conscientiousness Always Associated With Better Health? A U.S.-Japan Cross-Cultural Examination of Biological Health Risk. PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN 2020; 47:486-498. [PMID: 32552349 DOI: 10.1177/0146167220929824] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
In Western societies, conscientiousness is associated with better health. Here, we tested whether this pattern would extend to East Asian, collectivistic societies. In these societies, social obligation motivated by conscientiousness could be excessive and thus health-impairing. We tested this prediction using cross-cultural surveys of Americans (N = 1,054) and Japanese (N = 382). Biomarkers of inflammation (interleukin-6 and C-reactive protein) and cardiovascular malfunction (systolic blood pressure and total-to-HDL cholesterol ratio) were adopted to define biological health risk (BHR). Among Americans, conscientiousness was associated with lower BHR. Moreover, this relationship was mediated by healthy lifestyle. In contrast, among Japanese, the relationship between conscientiousness and BHR was not significant. Further analysis revealed, however, that conscientiousness was associated with a greater commitment to social obligation, which in turn predicted higher BHR. These findings suggest that conscientiousness may or may not be salubrious, depending on health implications of normatively sanctioned behaviors in varying cultures.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Jiyoung Park
- The University of Texas at Dallas, Richardson, USA
| |
Collapse
|