1
|
Kong G, Frisken I, Liaw GJ, Keys R, Alais D. Efficient measurement of dynamic working memory. Mem Cognit 2025:10.3758/s13421-025-01724-x. [PMID: 40279027 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-025-01724-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 04/11/2025] [Indexed: 04/26/2025]
Abstract
Working memory is the ability to maintain a limited amount of information after it has been removed from perception. It is a key cognitive ability, thought to play a role in other cognitive functions, including perception, attention and action. Given its importance, its accurate and efficient measurement is a major goal in working memory research. Here we introduce a novel working memory tracking paradigm, inspired by continuous psychophysics and multiple object tracking. Participants viewed a sequence of stimuli moving along variable paths and were asked to reproduce the path by tracing it on a touchscreen. This reproduction was then compared to the original stimulus to determine error and thus memory performance. Across three experiments, we found that this new method is efficient, reliable and powerful, with only ten trials per condition required for stable performance estimates. We have also shown that the method is only minimally affected by perceptual or attentional confounds. Most importantly, since performance was measured across the trial, this method also allows for the investigation of how working memory changes across time. By averaging equivalent time points across trials, we identified influences from both primacy and recency effects, and quantified performance around particularly important points along the motion path. The working memory tracking paradigm is therefore especially useful when experimental time is limited, experimental conditions are extensive or when the time-course is a key interest. The method also opens up the study of working memory with dynamic stimuli.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Garry Kong
- Department of Integrated Arts and Sciences, Hiroshima University, Hiroshima, Japan.
- Waseda Institute for Advanced Study, Waseda University, Tokyo, Japan.
| | - Isabelle Frisken
- School of Psychology, University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia
| | - Gwenisha J Liaw
- School of Psychology, University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia
| | - Robert Keys
- School of Psychology, University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia
| | - David Alais
- School of Psychology, University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Wang Y, Yang K, Zhou S, Zhang H, Ma T, Shi X, Ma W. Bilingual Proficiency Effects on Word Recall and Recognition. Behav Sci (Basel) 2025; 15:437. [PMID: 40282059 PMCID: PMC12023998 DOI: 10.3390/bs15040437] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/23/2025] [Revised: 03/13/2025] [Accepted: 03/26/2025] [Indexed: 04/29/2025] Open
Abstract
This study investigates the effects of bilingual proficiency on word recognition and recall across different memory tasks, with a focus on Chinese-English bilinguals. Participants learned lists of words in either their L1 (Chinese) or L2 (English) language while performing a semantic judgment task. Their memory for the learned words was subsequently assessed using three distinct tasks: a word recognition task (Experiment 1), a picture endorsement task (Experiment 2), and a free recall task (Experiment 3). The results revealed a significant L2 advantage in word recognition, as evidenced by higher hit rates, lower false alarm rates, and greater discrimination scores for L2 words. Furthermore, altering the retrieval cues from words to pictures led to a significant decrease in memory performance, but this did not diminish the L2 advantage. However, removing retrieval cues entirely eliminated the L2 advantage: participants demonstrated similar levels of correct recall for both L1 and L2 words, but showed a higher frequency of false recall for L1 words. To account for these dissociations between recall and recognition tasks, a level-based bilingual cognitive efficiency framework was proposed, incorporating factors such as pre-experimental exposure, cognitive resource allocation, the strength of lexical associations, and the demands of retrieval cues.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yaqi Wang
- School of Foreign Languages and Literature, Shandong University, Jinan 250100, China; (Y.W.); (K.Y.); (S.Z.); (H.Z.); (T.M.)
- Center for Clinical Neurolinguistics, Shandong University, Jinan 250100, China
| | - Kai Yang
- School of Foreign Languages and Literature, Shandong University, Jinan 250100, China; (Y.W.); (K.Y.); (S.Z.); (H.Z.); (T.M.)
| | - Simin Zhou
- School of Foreign Languages and Literature, Shandong University, Jinan 250100, China; (Y.W.); (K.Y.); (S.Z.); (H.Z.); (T.M.)
- Center for Clinical Neurolinguistics, Shandong University, Jinan 250100, China
| | - Hao Zhang
- School of Foreign Languages and Literature, Shandong University, Jinan 250100, China; (Y.W.); (K.Y.); (S.Z.); (H.Z.); (T.M.)
- Center for Clinical Neurolinguistics, Shandong University, Jinan 250100, China
| | - Tinghui Ma
- School of Foreign Languages and Literature, Shandong University, Jinan 250100, China; (Y.W.); (K.Y.); (S.Z.); (H.Z.); (T.M.)
- Center for Clinical Neurolinguistics, Shandong University, Jinan 250100, China
| | - Xiujuan Shi
- School of Foreign Languages and Literature, Shandong University, Jinan 250100, China; (Y.W.); (K.Y.); (S.Z.); (H.Z.); (T.M.)
- Center for Clinical Neurolinguistics, Shandong University, Jinan 250100, China
| | - Wen Ma
- School of Foreign Languages and Literature, Shandong University, Jinan 250100, China; (Y.W.); (K.Y.); (S.Z.); (H.Z.); (T.M.)
- Center for Clinical Neurolinguistics, Shandong University, Jinan 250100, China
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Allegretti E, D'Innocenzo G, Coco MI. The Visual Integration of Semantic and Spatial Information of Objects in Naturalistic Scenes (VISIONS) database: attentional, conceptual, and perceptual norms. Behav Res Methods 2025; 57:42. [PMID: 39753746 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-024-02535-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 09/23/2024] [Indexed: 01/11/2025]
Abstract
The complex interplay between low- and high-level mechanisms governing our visual system can only be fully understood within ecologically valid naturalistic contexts. For this reason, in recent years, substantial efforts have been devoted to equipping the scientific community with datasets of realistic images normed on semantic or spatial features. Here, we introduce VISIONS, an extensive database of 1136 naturalistic scenes normed on a wide range of perceptual and conceptual norms by 185 English speakers across three levels of granularity: isolated object, whole scene, and object-in-scene. Each naturalistic scene contains a critical object systematically manipulated and normed regarding its semantic consistency (e.g., a toothbrush vs. a flashlight in a bathroom) and spatial position (i.e., left, right). Normative data are also available for low- (i.e., clarity, visual complexity) and high-level (i.e., name agreement, confidence, familiarity, prototypicality, manipulability) features of the critical object and its embedding scene context. Eye-tracking data during a free-viewing task further confirms the experimental validity of our manipulations while theoretically demonstrating that object semantics is acquired in extra-foveal vision and used to guide early overt attention. To our knowledge, VISIONS is the first database exhaustively covering norms about integrating objects in scenes and providing several perceptual and conceptual norms of the two as independently taken. We expect VISIONS to become an invaluable image dataset to examine and answer timely questions above and beyond vision science, where a diversity of perceptual, attentive, mnemonic, or linguistic processes could be explored as they develop, age, or become neuropathological.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elena Allegretti
- Department of Psychology, Sapienza, University of Rome, Rome, Italy.
| | | | - Moreno I Coco
- Department of Psychology, Sapienza, University of Rome, Rome, Italy.
- I.R.C.C.S. Fondazione Santa Lucia, Rome, Italy.
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Gros H, Thibaut JP, Sander E. Uncovering the interplay between drawings, mental representations, and arithmetic problem-solving strategies in children and adults. Mem Cognit 2025; 53:76-95. [PMID: 38347259 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01523-w] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 01/14/2024] [Indexed: 01/30/2025]
Abstract
There is an ongoing debate in the scientific community regarding the nature and role of the mental representations involved in solving arithmetic word problems. In this study, we took a closer look at the interplay between mental representations, drawing production, and strategy choice. We used dual-strategy isomorphic word problems sharing the same mathematical structure, but differing in the entities they mentioned in their problem statement. Due to the non-mathematical knowledge attached to these entities, some problems were believed to lead to a specific (cardinal) encoding compatible with one solving strategy, whereas other problems were thought to foster a different (ordinal) encoding compatible with the other solving strategy. We asked 59 children and 52 adults to solve 12 of those arithmetic word problems and to make a diagram of each problem. We hypothesized that the diagrams of both groups would display prototypical features indicating either a cardinal representation or an ordinal representation, depending on the entities mentioned in the problem statement. Joint analysis of the drawing task and the problem-solving task showed that the cardinal and ordinal features of the diagrams are linked with the hypothesized semantic properties of the problems and, crucially, with the choice of one solving strategy over another. We showed that regardless of their experience, participants' strategy use depends on their problem representation, which is influenced by the non-mathematical information in the problem statement, as revealed in their diagrams. We discuss the relevance of drawing tasks for investigating mental representations and fostering mathematical development in school.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hippolyte Gros
- CY Cergy Paris University, Paragraphe Lab, Gennevilliers, France.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
5
|
Bainbridge WA, Chamberlain R, Wammes J, Fan JE. Drawing as a means to characterize memory and cognition. Mem Cognit 2025; 53:1-5. [PMID: 39192141 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01618-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 08/29/2024]
Abstract
As psychological research embraces more naturalistic questions and large-scale analytic methods, drawing has emerged as an exciting tool for studying cognition. Drawing provides rich information about how we view the world, ranging from largely veridical perceptual representations to abstracted meta-cognitive representations. Drawing also requires the integration of multiple processes (e.g., vision, memory, motor learning), and experience with drawing can have an impact on such processes. As a result, drawing presents several interesting cognitive questions, while also providing a way to gain insight into a multitude of others. This Special Issue features 25 cutting-edge studies utilizing drawing to reveal discoveries transversing fields in psychology. These diverse studies investigate drawing across children, young adults, older adults, and special populations such as individuals with blindness, anterograde amnesia, apraxia, and semantic dementia. These studies detail new discoveries about the mechanisms underlying memory, attention, mathematical reasoning, and other cognitive processes. They employ a range of methods including psychophysical experiments, deep learning, and neuroimaging. Finally, many of these studies cover topics about the impact of drawing as a process on other cognitive processes, including how drawing expertise impacts other processes like visual memory or spatial abilities. Overall, this collection of studies paves the way for an exciting future of drawing as a commonplace tool used by psychologists to understand complex phenomena.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Wilma A Bainbridge
- Department of Psychology, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA.
- Neuroscience Institute, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA.
- Institute for Mind and Biology, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA.
| | | | - Jeffrey Wammes
- Department of Psychology, Queen's University, Kingston, ON, Canada
- Centre for Neuroscience Studies, Queen's University, Kingston, ON, Canada
| | - Judith E Fan
- Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Hall EH, Geng JJ. Object-based attention during scene perception elicits boundary contraction in memory. Mem Cognit 2025; 53:6-18. [PMID: 38530622 PMCID: PMC11779785 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01540-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 02/17/2024] [Indexed: 03/28/2024]
Abstract
Boundary contraction and extension are two types of scene transformations that occur in memory. In extension, viewers extrapolate information beyond the edges of the image, whereas in contraction, viewers forget information near the edges. Recent work suggests that image composition influences the direction and magnitude of boundary transformation. We hypothesize that selective attention at encoding is an important driver of boundary transformation effects, selective attention to specific objects at encoding leading to boundary contraction. In this study, one group of participants (N = 36) memorized 15 scenes while searching for targets, while a separate group (N = 36) just memorized the scenes. Both groups then drew the scenes from memory with as much object and spatial detail as they could remember. We asked online workers to provide ratings of boundary transformations in the drawings, as well as how many objects they contained and the precision of remembered object size and location. We found that search condition drawings showed significantly greater boundary contraction than drawings of the same scenes in the memorize condition. Search drawings were significantly more likely to contain target objects, and the likelihood to recall other objects in the scene decreased as a function of their distance from the target. These findings suggest that selective attention to a specific object due to a search task at encoding will lead to significant boundary contraction.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elizabeth H Hall
- Department of Psychology, University of California Davis, Davis, CA, 95616, USA.
- Center for Mind and Brain, University of California Davis, Davis, CA, 95618, USA.
| | - Joy J Geng
- Department of Psychology, University of California Davis, Davis, CA, 95616, USA
- Center for Mind and Brain, University of California Davis, Davis, CA, 95618, USA
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Děchtěrenko F, Bainbridge WA, Lukavský J. Visual free recall and recognition in art students and laypeople. Mem Cognit 2025; 53:363-378. [PMID: 39078592 PMCID: PMC11789472 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01607-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 06/19/2024] [Indexed: 07/31/2024]
Abstract
Artists and laypeople differ in their ability to create drawings. Previous research has shown that artists have improved memory performance during drawing; however, it is unclear whether they have better visual memory after the drawing is finished. In this paper, we focused on the question of differences in visual memory between art students and the general population in two studies. In Study 1, both groups studied a set of images and later drew them in a surprise visual recall test. In Study 2, the drawings from Study 1 were evaluated by a different set of raters based on their drawing quality and similarity to the original image to link drawing evaluations with memory performance for both groups. We found that both groups showed comparable visual recognition memory performance; however, the artist group showed increased recall memory performance. Moreover, they produced drawings that were both better quality and more similar to the original image. Individually, participants whose drawings were rated as better showed higher recognition accuracy. Results from Study 2 also have practical implications for the usage of drawing as a tool for measuring free recall - the majority of the drawings were recognizable, and raters showed a high level of consistency during their evaluation of the drawings. Taken together, we found that artists have better visual recall memory than laypeople.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Filip Děchtěrenko
- Institute of Psychology, Czech Academy of Sciences, Pod Vodárenskou věží 4, Prague, 18200, Czech Republic.
| | - Wilma A Bainbridge
- Department of Psychology, University of Chicago, 5848 S University Ave, Beecher Hall 303, Chicago, IL, 60637, USA
- Neuroscience Institute, University of Chicago, 5812 S Ellis Ave, Chicago, IL, 60637, USA
| | - Jiří Lukavský
- Institute of Psychology, Czech Academy of Sciences, Pod Vodárenskou věží 4, Prague, 18200, Czech Republic
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Zhou SS, Rowchan K, Mckeown B, Smallwood J, Wammes JD. Drawing behaviour influences ongoing thought patterns and subsequent memory. Conscious Cogn 2025; 127:103791. [PMID: 39671842 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2024.103791] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/07/2024] [Revised: 11/08/2024] [Accepted: 11/24/2024] [Indexed: 12/15/2024]
Abstract
For millennia, humans have created drawings as a means of externalizing visual representations, and later, to aid communication and learning. Despite its cultural value, we understand little about the cognitive states elicited by drawing, and their downstream benefits. In two preregistered experiments, we explored these states; Undergraduate participants (Ns = 69, 60) encoded words by drawing or writing, periodically describing their thoughts using multi-dimensional experience sampling, a tool for characterizing the features of ongoing thought. Subsequent memory was tested via free recall. Contrasted with writing, drawing improved memory, and evoked thoughts that were more visual and elaborative. Recall was also dictated by the emergence of these thought patterns, with the former most important when drawing. Our findings establish that drawing elicits unique thought patterns that promote successful memory, providing an explanation for drawing's influential role in our everyday lives.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Keanna Rowchan
- Psychology Department, Queen's University, Kingston, ON, Canada.
| | - Brontë Mckeown
- Psychology Department, Queen's University, Kingston, ON, Canada.
| | | | - Jeffrey D Wammes
- Psychology Department, Queen's University, Kingston, ON, Canada; Centre for Neuroscience Studies, Queen's University, Kingston, ON, Canada.
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Cárdenas-Miller N, O'Donnell RE, Tam J, Wyble B. Surprise! Draw the scene: Visual recall reveals poor incidental working memory following visual search in natural scenes. Mem Cognit 2025; 53:19-32. [PMID: 37770695 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-023-01465-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 09/01/2023] [Indexed: 09/30/2023]
Abstract
Searching within natural scenes can induce incidental encoding of information about the scene and the target, particularly when the scene is complex or repeated. However, recent evidence from attribute amnesia (AA) suggests that in some situations, searchers can find a target without building a robust incidental memory of its task relevant features. Through drawing-based visual recall and an AA search task, we investigated whether search in natural scenes necessitates memory encoding. Participants repeatedly searched for and located an easily detected item in novel scenes for numerous trials before being unexpectedly prompted to draw either the entire scene (Experiment 1) or their search target (Experiment 2) directly after viewing the search image. Naïve raters assessed the similarity of the drawings to the original information. We found that surprise-trial drawings of the scene and search target were both poorly recognizable, but the same drawers produced highly recognizable drawings on the next trial when they had an expectation to draw the image. Experiment 3 further showed that the poor surprise trial memory could not merely be attributed to interference from the surprising event. Our findings suggest that even for searches done in natural scenes, it is possible to locate a target without creating a robust memory of either it or the scene it was in, even if attended to just a few seconds prior. This disconnection between attention and memory might reflect a fundamental property of cognitive computations designed to optimize task performance and minimize resource use.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Ryan E O'Donnell
- Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, USA
- Drexel University, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - Joyce Tam
- Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, USA
| | - Brad Wyble
- Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Schmidt F, Tiedemann H, Fleming RW, Morgenstern Y. Inferring shape transformations in a drawing task. Mem Cognit 2025; 53:189-199. [PMID: 37668880 PMCID: PMC11779755 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-023-01452-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 08/09/2023] [Indexed: 09/06/2023]
Abstract
Many objects and materials in our environment are subject to transformations that alter their shape. For example, branches bend in the wind, ice melts, and paper crumples. Still, we recognize objects and materials across these changes, suggesting we can distinguish an object's original features from those caused by the transformations ("shape scission"). Yet, if we truly understand transformations, we should not only be able to identify their signatures but also actively apply the transformations to new objects (i.e., through imagination or mental simulation). Here, we investigated this ability using a drawing task. On a tablet computer, participants viewed a sample contour and its transformed version, and were asked to apply the same transformation to a test contour by drawing what the transformed test shape should look like. Thus, they had to (i) infer the transformation from the shape differences, (ii) envisage its application to the test shape, and (iii) draw the result. Our findings show that drawings were more similar to the ground truth transformed test shape than to the original test shape-demonstrating the inference and reproduction of transformations from observation. However, this was only observed for relatively simple shapes. The ability was also modulated by transformation type and magnitude but not by the similarity between sample and test shapes. Together, our findings suggest that we can distinguish between representations of original object shapes and their transformations, and can use visual imagery to mentally apply nonrigid transformations to observed objects, showing how we not only perceive but also 'understand' shape.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Filipp Schmidt
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Justus Liebig University Giessen, Otto-Behaghel-Str. 10F, 35394, Giessen, Germany.
- Center for Mind, Brain and Behavior (CMBB), University of Marburg and Justus Liebig University, Giessen, Germany.
| | - Henning Tiedemann
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Justus Liebig University Giessen, Otto-Behaghel-Str. 10F, 35394, Giessen, Germany
| | - Roland W Fleming
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Justus Liebig University Giessen, Otto-Behaghel-Str. 10F, 35394, Giessen, Germany
- Center for Mind, Brain and Behavior (CMBB), University of Marburg and Justus Liebig University, Giessen, Germany
| | - Yaniv Morgenstern
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Justus Liebig University Giessen, Otto-Behaghel-Str. 10F, 35394, Giessen, Germany
- University of Leuven (KU Leuven), Leuven, Belgium
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Cen D, Teichert E, Hodgetts CJ, Gruber MJ. Curiosity shapes spatial exploration and cognitive map formation in humans. COMMUNICATIONS PSYCHOLOGY 2024; 2:129. [PMID: 39738810 DOI: 10.1038/s44271-024-00174-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/28/2024] [Accepted: 12/05/2024] [Indexed: 01/02/2025]
Abstract
Cognitive maps are thought to arise, at least in part, from our intrinsic curiosity to explore unknown places. However, it remains untested how curiosity shapes aspects of spatial exploration in humans. Combining a virtual reality task with indices of exploration complexity, we found that pre-exploration curiosity states predicted how much individuals spatially explored environments, whereas markers of visual exploration determined post-exploration feelings of interest. Moreover, individual differences in curiosity traits, particularly Stress Tolerance, modulated the relationship between curiosity and spatial exploration, suggesting the capacity to cope with uncertainty enhances the curiosity-exploration link. Furthermore, both curiosity and spatial exploration predicted how precisely participants could recall spatial-relational details of the environment, as measured by a sketch map task. These results provide new evidence for a link between curiosity and exploratory behaviour, and how curiosity might shape cognitive map formation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Danlu Cen
- Cardiff University Brain Research Imaging Centre (CUBRIC), School of Psychology, Cardiff University, Wales, UK.
| | - Eva Teichert
- Cardiff University Brain Research Imaging Centre (CUBRIC), School of Psychology, Cardiff University, Wales, UK
| | - Carl J Hodgetts
- Cardiff University Brain Research Imaging Centre (CUBRIC), School of Psychology, Cardiff University, Wales, UK
- Department of Psychology, Royal Holloway, University of London, London, UK
| | - Matthias J Gruber
- Cardiff University Brain Research Imaging Centre (CUBRIC), School of Psychology, Cardiff University, Wales, UK
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Megla E, Rosenthal SR, Bainbridge WA. Drawings reveal changes in object memory, but not spatial memory, across time. Cognition 2024; 254:105988. [PMID: 39476659 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105988] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/27/2024] [Revised: 08/09/2024] [Accepted: 10/15/2024] [Indexed: 11/06/2024]
Abstract
Time has an immense influence on our memory. Truncated encoding leads to memory for only the 'gist' of an image, and long delays before recall result in generalized memories with few details. Here, we used crowdsourced scoring of hundreds of drawings made from memory after variable encoding (Experiment 1) and retentions of that memory (Experiment 2) to quantify what features of memory content change across time. We found that whereas some features of memory are highly dependent on time, such as the proportion of objects recalled from a scene and false recall for objects not in the original image, spatial memory was highly accurate and relatively independent of time. We also found that we could predict which objects were recalled across time based on the location, meaning, and saliency of the objects. The differential impact of time on object and spatial memory supports a separation of these memory systems.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Emma Megla
- Department of Psychology, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA.
| | | | - Wilma A Bainbridge
- Department of Psychology, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA; Neuroscience Institute, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Hirschhorn R, Mudrik L. More than words: can free reports adequately measure the richness of perception? Neurosci Conscious 2024; 2024:niae035. [PMID: 39445136 PMCID: PMC11498181 DOI: 10.1093/nc/niae035] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/09/2023] [Revised: 08/28/2024] [Accepted: 09/02/2024] [Indexed: 10/25/2024] Open
Abstract
The question of the richness (or sparseness) of conscious experience has evoked ongoing debate and discussion. Claims for both richness and sparseness are supported by empirical data, yet they are often indirect, and alternative explanations have been put forward. Recently, it has been suggested that current experimental methods limit participants' responses, thereby preventing researchers from assessing the actual richness of perception. Instead, free verbal reports were presented as a possible way to overcome this limitation. As part of this approach, a novel paradigm of freely reported words was developed using a new metric, intersubjective agreement (IA), with experimental results interpreted as capturing aspects of conscious perception. Here, we challenge the validity of freely reported words as a tool for studying the richness of conscious experience. We base our claims on two studies (each composed of three experiments), where we manipulated the richness of percepts and tested whether IA changed accordingly. Five additional control experiments were conducted to validate the experimental logic and examine alternative explanations. Our results suggest otherwise, presenting four challenges to the free verbal report paradigm: first, impoverished stimuli did not evoke lower IA scores. Second, the IA score was correlated with word frequency in English. Third, the original positive relationship between IA scores and rated confidence was not found in any of the six experiments. Fourth, a high rate of nonexisting words was found, some of which described items that matched the gist of the scene but did not appear in the image. We conclude that a metric based on freely reported words might be better explained by vocabulary conventions and gist-based reports than by capturing the richness of perception.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rony Hirschhorn
- Sagol School of Neuroscience, Tel Aviv University, P.O. Box 39040, Tel Aviv 6997801, Israel
| | - Liad Mudrik
- Sagol School of Neuroscience, Tel Aviv University, P.O. Box 39040, Tel Aviv 6997801, Israel
- School of Psychological Sciences, Tel Aviv University, P.O. Box 39040, Tel Aviv 6997801, Israel
- Canadian Institute for Advanced Research (CIFAR), Brain, Mind, and Consciousness Program, 661 University Ave., Suite 505, Toronto, ON M5G 1M1, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Beitner J, Helbing J, David EJ, Võ MLH. Using a flashlight-contingent window paradigm to investigate visual search and object memory in virtual reality and on computer screens. Sci Rep 2024; 14:8596. [PMID: 38615047 PMCID: PMC11379806 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-024-58941-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/13/2023] [Accepted: 04/04/2024] [Indexed: 04/15/2024] Open
Abstract
A popular technique to modulate visual input during search is to use gaze-contingent windows. However, these are often rather discomforting, providing the impression of visual impairment. To counteract this, we asked participants in this study to search through illuminated as well as dark three-dimensional scenes using a more naturalistic flashlight with which they could illuminate the rooms. In a surprise incidental memory task, we tested the identities and locations of objects encountered during search. Importantly, we tested this study design in both immersive virtual reality (VR; Experiment 1) and on a desktop-computer screen (Experiment 2). As hypothesized, searching with a flashlight increased search difficulty and memory usage during search. We found a memory benefit for identities of distractors in the flashlight condition in VR but not in the computer screen experiment. Surprisingly, location memory was comparable across search conditions despite the enormous difference in visual input. Subtle differences across experiments only appeared in VR after accounting for previous recognition performance, hinting at a benefit of flashlight search in VR. Our findings highlight that removing visual information does not necessarily impair location memory, and that screen experiments using virtual environments can elicit the same major effects as VR setups.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Julia Beitner
- Department of Psychology, Goethe University Frankfurt, Frankfurt am Main, Germany.
| | - Jason Helbing
- Department of Psychology, Goethe University Frankfurt, Frankfurt am Main, Germany
| | - Erwan Joël David
- Department of Psychology, Goethe University Frankfurt, Frankfurt am Main, Germany
- LIUM, Le Mans Université, Le Mans, France
| | - Melissa Lê-Hoa Võ
- Department of Psychology, Goethe University Frankfurt, Frankfurt am Main, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Wang G, Foxwell MJ, Cichy RM, Pitcher D, Kaiser D. Individual differences in internal models explain idiosyncrasies in scene perception. Cognition 2024; 245:105723. [PMID: 38262271 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105723] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/14/2023] [Revised: 01/12/2024] [Accepted: 01/14/2024] [Indexed: 01/25/2024]
Abstract
According to predictive processing theories, vision is facilitated by predictions derived from our internal models of what the world should look like. However, the contents of these models and how they vary across people remains unclear. Here, we use drawing as a behavioral readout of the contents of the internal models in individual participants. Participants were first asked to draw typical versions of scene categories, as descriptors of their internal models. These drawings were converted into standardized 3d renders, which we used as stimuli in subsequent scene categorization experiments. Across two experiments, participants' scene categorization was more accurate for renders tailored to their own drawings compared to renders based on others' drawings or copies of scene photographs, suggesting that scene perception is determined by a match with idiosyncratic internal models. Using a deep neural network to computationally evaluate similarities between scene renders, we further demonstrate that graded similarity to the render based on participants' own typical drawings (and thus to their internal model) predicts categorization performance across a range of candidate scenes. Together, our results showcase the potential of a new method for understanding individual differences - starting from participants' personal expectations about the structure of real-world scenes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gongting Wang
- Department of Education and Psychology, Freie Universität Berlin, Germany; Department of Mathematics and Computer Science, Physics, Geography, Justus-Liebig-Universität Gießen, Germany
| | | | - Radoslaw M Cichy
- Department of Education and Psychology, Freie Universität Berlin, Germany
| | | | - Daniel Kaiser
- Department of Mathematics and Computer Science, Physics, Geography, Justus-Liebig-Universität Gießen, Germany; Center for Mind, Brain and Behavior (CMBB), Philipps-Universität Marburg and Justus-Liebig-Universität Gießen, Germany.
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Lahner B, Mohsenzadeh Y, Mullin C, Oliva A. Visual perception of highly memorable images is mediated by a distributed network of ventral visual regions that enable a late memorability response. PLoS Biol 2024; 22:e3002564. [PMID: 38557761 PMCID: PMC10984539 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pbio.3002564] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/13/2023] [Accepted: 02/26/2024] [Indexed: 04/04/2024] Open
Abstract
Behavioral and neuroscience studies in humans and primates have shown that memorability is an intrinsic property of an image that predicts its strength of encoding into and retrieval from memory. While previous work has independently probed when or where this memorability effect may occur in the human brain, a description of its spatiotemporal dynamics is missing. Here, we used representational similarity analysis (RSA) to combine functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) with source-estimated magnetoencephalography (MEG) to simultaneously measure when and where the human cortex is sensitive to differences in image memorability. Results reveal that visual perception of High Memorable images, compared to Low Memorable images, recruits a set of regions of interest (ROIs) distributed throughout the ventral visual cortex: a late memorability response (from around 300 ms) in early visual cortex (EVC), inferior temporal cortex, lateral occipital cortex, fusiform gyrus, and banks of the superior temporal sulcus. Image memorability magnitude results are represented after high-level feature processing in visual regions and reflected in classical memory regions in the medial temporal lobe (MTL). Our results present, to our knowledge, the first unified spatiotemporal account of visual memorability effect across the human cortex, further supporting the levels-of-processing theory of perception and memory.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Benjamin Lahner
- Computer Science and Artificial Intelligence Laboratory, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, Massachusetts, United States of America
| | - Yalda Mohsenzadeh
- The Brain and Mind Institute, The University of Western Ontario, London, Canada
- Department of Computer Science, The University of Western Ontario, London, Canada
- Vector Institute for Artificial Intelligence, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
| | - Caitlin Mullin
- Vision: Science to Application (VISTA), York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
| | - Aude Oliva
- Computer Science and Artificial Intelligence Laboratory, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, Massachusetts, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Westebbe L, Liang Y, Blaser E. The Accuracy and Precision of Memory for Natural Scenes: A Walk in the Park. Open Mind (Camb) 2024; 8:131-147. [PMID: 38435706 PMCID: PMC10898787 DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00122] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/02/2023] [Accepted: 01/17/2024] [Indexed: 03/05/2024] Open
Abstract
It is challenging to quantify the accuracy and precision of scene memory because it is unclear what 'space' scenes occupy (how can we quantify error when misremembering a natural scene?). To address this, we exploited the ecologically valid, metric space in which scenes occur and are represented: routes. In a delayed estimation task, participants briefly saw a target scene drawn from a video of an outdoor 'route loop', then used a continuous report wheel of the route to pinpoint the scene. Accuracy was high and unbiased, indicating there was no net boundary extension/contraction. Interestingly, precision was higher for routes that were more self-similar (as characterized by the half-life, in meters, of a route's Multiscale Structural Similarity index), consistent with previous work finding a 'similarity advantage' where memory precision is regulated according to task demands. Overall, scenes were remembered to within a few meters of their actual location.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Leo Westebbe
- Department of Psychology, University of Massachusetts Boston, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Yibiao Liang
- Department of Psychology, University of Massachusetts Boston, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Erik Blaser
- Department of Psychology, University of Massachusetts Boston, Boston, MA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Long B, Fan JE, Huey H, Chai Z, Frank MC. Parallel developmental changes in children's production and recognition of line drawings of visual concepts. Nat Commun 2024; 15:1191. [PMID: 38331850 PMCID: PMC10853520 DOI: 10.1038/s41467-023-44529-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/23/2021] [Accepted: 12/18/2023] [Indexed: 02/10/2024] Open
Abstract
Childhood is marked by the rapid accumulation of knowledge and the prolific production of drawings. We conducted a systematic study of how children create and recognize line drawings of visual concepts. We recruited 2-10-year-olds to draw 48 categories via a kiosk at a children's museum, resulting in >37K drawings. We analyze changes in the category-diagnostic information in these drawings using vision algorithms and annotations of object parts. We find developmental gains in children's inclusion of category-diagnostic information that are not reducible to variation in visuomotor control or effort. Moreover, even unrecognizable drawings contain information about the animacy and size of the category children tried to draw. Using guessing games at the same kiosk, we find that children improve across childhood at recognizing each other's line drawings. This work leverages vision algorithms to characterize developmental changes in children's drawings and suggests that these changes reflect refinements in children's internal representations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Bria Long
- Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Building 420, 450 Jane Stanford Way, Stanford, 94305, CA, USA.
| | - Judith E Fan
- Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Building 420, 450 Jane Stanford Way, Stanford, 94305, CA, USA
- Department of Psychology, University of California, San Diego, 9500 Gilman Dr., La Jolla, 92093, CA, USA
| | - Holly Huey
- Department of Psychology, University of California, San Diego, 9500 Gilman Dr., La Jolla, 92093, CA, USA
| | - Zixian Chai
- Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Building 420, 450 Jane Stanford Way, Stanford, 94305, CA, USA
| | - Michael C Frank
- Department of Psychology, University of California, San Diego, 9500 Gilman Dr., La Jolla, 92093, CA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Wise T, Emery K, Radulescu A. Naturalistic reinforcement learning. Trends Cogn Sci 2024; 28:144-158. [PMID: 37777463 PMCID: PMC10878983 DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2023.08.016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/16/2023] [Revised: 08/23/2023] [Accepted: 08/24/2023] [Indexed: 10/02/2023]
Abstract
Humans possess a remarkable ability to make decisions within real-world environments that are expansive, complex, and multidimensional. Human cognitive computational neuroscience has sought to exploit reinforcement learning (RL) as a framework within which to explain human decision-making, often focusing on constrained, artificial experimental tasks. In this article, we review recent efforts that use naturalistic approaches to determine how humans make decisions in complex environments that better approximate the real world, providing a clearer picture of how humans navigate the challenges posed by real-world decisions. These studies purposely embed elements of naturalistic complexity within experimental paradigms, rather than focusing on simplification, generating insights into the processes that likely underpin humans' ability to navigate complex, multidimensional real-world environments so successfully.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Toby Wise
- Department of Neuroimaging, King's College London, London, UK.
| | - Kara Emery
- Center for Data Science, New York University, New York, NY, USA
| | - Angela Radulescu
- Center for Computational Psychiatry, Icahn School of Medicine at Mt. Sinai, New York, NY, USA
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Megla E, Rosenthal SR, Bainbridge WA. Drawings reveal changes in object memory, but not spatial memory, across time. BIORXIV : THE PREPRINT SERVER FOR BIOLOGY 2024:2024.01.26.577281. [PMID: 38352427 PMCID: PMC10862701 DOI: 10.1101/2024.01.26.577281] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/21/2024]
Abstract
Time has an immense influence on our memory. Truncated encoding leads to memory for only the 'gist' of an image, and long delays before recall result in generalized memories with few details. Here, we used crowdsourced scoring of hundreds of drawings made from memory after variable encoding (Experiment 1) and retentions of that memory (Experiment 2) to quantify what features of memory content change across time. We found that whereas some features of memory are highly dependent on time, such as the proportion of objects recalled from a scene and false recall for objects not in the original image, spatial memory was highly accurate and relatively independent of time. We also found that we could predict which objects were recalled across time based on the location, meaning, and saliency of the objects. The differential impact of time on object and spatial memory supports a separation of these memory systems.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Emma Megla
- Department of Psychology, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL USA
| | | | - Wilma A. Bainbridge
- Department of Psychology, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL USA
- Neuroscience Institute, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL USA
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Peacock CE, Hall EH, Henderson JM. Objects are selected for attention based upon meaning during passive scene viewing. Psychon Bull Rev 2023; 30:1874-1886. [PMID: 37095319 PMCID: PMC11164276 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-023-02286-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 03/26/2023] [Indexed: 04/26/2023]
Abstract
While object meaning has been demonstrated to guide attention during active scene viewing and object salience guides attention during passive viewing, it is unknown whether object meaning predicts attention in passive viewing tasks and whether attention during passive viewing is more strongly related to meaning or salience. To answer this question, we used a mixed modeling approach where we computed the average meaning and physical salience of objects in scenes while statistically controlling for the roles of object size and eccentricity. Using eye-movement data from aesthetic judgment and memorization tasks, we then tested whether fixations are more likely to land on high-meaning objects than low-meaning objects while controlling for object salience, size, and eccentricity. The results demonstrated that fixations are more likely to be directed to high meaning objects than low meaning objects regardless of these other factors. Further analyses revealed that fixation durations were positively associated with object meaning irrespective of the other object properties. Overall, these findings provide the first evidence that objects are, in part, selected by meaning for attentional selection during passive scene viewing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Candace E Peacock
- Center for Mind and Brain, University of California, 267 Cousteau Place, Davis, CA, 95618, USA.
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Davis, CA, USA.
| | - Elizabeth H Hall
- Center for Mind and Brain, University of California, 267 Cousteau Place, Davis, CA, 95618, USA
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Davis, CA, USA
| | - John M Henderson
- Center for Mind and Brain, University of California, 267 Cousteau Place, Davis, CA, 95618, USA
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Davis, CA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Fan JE, Bainbridge WA, Chamberlain R, Wammes JD. Drawing as a versatile cognitive tool. NATURE REVIEWS PSYCHOLOGY 2023; 2:556-568. [PMID: 39239312 PMCID: PMC11377027 DOI: 10.1038/s44159-023-00212-w] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 06/21/2023] [Indexed: 09/07/2024]
Abstract
Drawing is a cognitive tool that makes the invisible contents of mental life visible. Humans use this tool to produce a remarkable variety of pictures, from realistic portraits to schematic diagrams. Despite this variety and the prevalence of drawn images, the psychological mechanisms that enable drawings to be so versatile have yet to be fully explored. In this Review, we synthesize contemporary work in multiple areas of psychology, computer science and neuroscience that examines the cognitive processes involved in drawing production and comprehension. This body of findings suggests that the balance of contributions from perception, memory and social inference during drawing production varies depending on the situation, resulting in some drawings that are more realistic and other drawings that are more abstract. We also consider the use of drawings as a research tool for investigating various aspects of cognition, as well as the role that drawing has in facilitating learning and communication. Taken together, information about how drawings are used in different contexts illuminates the central role of visually grounded abstractions in human thought and behaviour.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Judith E Fan
- Department of Psychology, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA
- Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA
| | | | | | - Jeffrey D Wammes
- Department of Psychology, Centre for Neuroscience Studies, Queen's University, Kingston, Ontario, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Rubinstein DY, Weidemann CT, Sperling MR, Kahana MJ. Direct brain recordings suggest a causal subsequent-memory effect. Cereb Cortex 2023; 33:6891-6901. [PMID: 36702495 PMCID: PMC10233277 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhad008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/14/2022] [Revised: 01/04/2023] [Accepted: 01/05/2023] [Indexed: 01/28/2023] Open
Abstract
Endogenous variation in brain state and stimulus-specific evoked activity can both contribute to successful encoding. Previous studies, however, have not clearly distinguished among these components. We address this question by analysing intracranial EEG recorded from epilepsy patients as they studied and subsequently recalled lists of words. We first trained classifiers to predict recall of either single items or entire lists and found that both classifiers exhibited similar performance. We found that list-level classifier output-a biomarker of successful encoding-tracked item presentation and recall events, despite having no information about the trial structure. Across widespread brain regions, decreased low- and increased high-frequency activity (HFA) marked successful encoding of both items and lists. We found regional differences in the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex, where in the hippocampus HFA correlated more strongly with item recall, whereas, in the prefrontal cortex, HFA correlated more strongly with list performance. Despite subtle differences in item- and list-level features, the similarity in overall classification performance, spectral signatures of successful recall and fluctuations of spectral activity across the encoding period argue for a shared endogenous process that causally impacts the brain's ability to learn new information.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Daniel Y Rubinstein
- Department of Neurology, Thomas Jefferson University, Philadelphia, PA 19107, USA
| | - Christoph T Weidemann
- Department of Psychology, Swansea University, Swansea SA2 8PP, UK
- Department of Bioengineering, Columbia University, New York, NY 10027, USA
| | - Michael R Sperling
- Department of Neurology, Thomas Jefferson University, Philadelphia, PA 19107, USA
| | - Michael J Kahana
- Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA 19104, USA
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Huey H, Lu X, Walker CM, Fan JE. Visual explanations prioritize functional properties at the expense of visual fidelity. Cognition 2023; 236:105414. [PMID: 36870147 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2023.105414] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/30/2022] [Revised: 01/20/2023] [Accepted: 02/14/2023] [Indexed: 03/06/2023]
Abstract
Visual explanations play an integral role in communicating mechanistic knowledge about how things work. What do people think distinguishes such pictures from those that are intended to convey how things look? To explore this question, we used a drawing paradigm to elicit both visual explanations and depictions of novel machine-like objects, then conducted a detailed analysis of the semantic information conveyed in each drawing. We found that visual explanations placed greater emphasis on parts of the machines that move or interact to produce an effect, while visual depictions emphasized parts that were visually salient, even if they were static. Moreover, we found that these differences in visual emphasis impacted what information naive viewers could extract from these drawings: explanations made it easier to infer which action was needed to operate the machine, but more difficult to identify which machine it represented. Taken together, our findings suggest that people spontaneously prioritize functional information when producing visual explanations but that this strategy may be double-edged, facilitating inferences about physical mechanism at the expense of preserving visual fidelity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Holly Huey
- Department of Psychology, University of California San Diego, United States of America
| | - Xuanchen Lu
- Department of Psychology, University of California San Diego, United States of America
| | - Caren M Walker
- Department of Psychology, University of California San Diego, United States of America
| | - Judith E Fan
- Department of Psychology, University of California San Diego, United States of America.
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Prasad D, Bainbridge WA. The Visual Mandela Effect as Evidence for Shared and Specific False Memories Across People. Psychol Sci 2022; 33:1971-1988. [PMID: 36219739 DOI: 10.1177/09567976221108944] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/14/2022] Open
Abstract
The Mandela effect is an Internet phenomenon describing shared and consistent false memories for specific icons in popular culture. The visual Mandela effect is a Mandela effect specific to visual icons (e.g., the Monopoly Man is falsely remembered as having a monocle) and has not yet been empirically quantified or tested. In Experiment 1 (N = 100 adults), we demonstrated that certain images from popular iconography elicit consistent, specific false memories. In Experiment 2 (N = 60 adults), using eye-tracking-like methods, we found no attentional or visual differences that drive this phenomenon. There is no clear difference in the natural visual experience of these images (Experiment 3), and these errors also occur spontaneously during recall (Experiment 4; N = 50 adults). These results demonstrate that there are certain images for which people consistently make the same false-memory error, despite the majority of visual experience being the canonical image.
Collapse
|
26
|
Cooper RA, Ritchey M. Patterns of episodic content and specificity predicting subjective memory vividness. Mem Cognit 2022; 50:1629-1643. [PMID: 35246786 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-022-01291-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 02/10/2022] [Indexed: 12/30/2022]
Abstract
The ability to remember and internally represent events is often accompanied by a subjective sense of "vividness". Vividness measures are frequently used to evaluate the experience of remembering and imagining events, yet little research has considered the objective attributes of event memories that underlie this subjective judgment, and individual differences in this mapping. Here, we tested how the content and specificity of event memories support subjectively vivid recollection. Over three experiments, participants encoded events containing a theme word and three distinct elements - a person, a place, and an object. In a memory test, memory for event elements was assessed at two levels of specificity - semantic gist (names) and perceptual details (lure discrimination). We found a strong correspondence between memory vividness and memory for gist information that did not vary by which elements were contained in memory. There was a smaller, additive benefit of remembering specific perceptual details on vividness, which, in one study, was driven by memory for place details. Moreover, we found individual differences in the relationship between memory vividness and objective memory attributes primarily along the specificity dimension, such that one cluster of participants used perceptual detail to inform memory vividness whereas another cluster was more driven by gist information. Therefore, while gist memory appears to drive vividness on average, there were idiosyncrasies in this pattern across participants. When assessing subjective ratings of memory and imagination, research should consider how these ratings map onto objective memory attributes in the context of their study design and population.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rose A Cooper
- Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, USA.
- Roux Institute, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, USA.
| | - Maureen Ritchey
- Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Boston College, Boston, MA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Wyer NA, Hollins TJ, Pahl S. Remembering Social Events: A Construal Level Approach. PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN 2022; 48:1238-1254. [PMID: 34404279 PMCID: PMC9245159 DOI: 10.1177/01461672211038188] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/06/2020] [Accepted: 07/19/2021] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Social events are rich in information, yet research into how people remember such events has typically been limited to considering one aspect (e.g., faces, behaviors) at a time. Based on an internal meta-analysis of a program work encompassing 15 laboratory, field, and on-line experiments involving 1,230 participants, we found that construal level influences both the ability to recognize people involved in the event (d = 0.30) and the way the social aspects of the event are described (average d = 0.48). In contrast, memory for background objects/scenes that were present during the event was unaffected by construal level. We discuss these findings in terms of their implications for both event memory (and situations where different aspects of the same event are remembered) and for construal level (and the question of how and when construal is likely to affect memory).
Collapse
|
28
|
Tiedemann H, Morgenstern Y, Schmidt F, Fleming RW. One-shot generalization in humans revealed through a drawing task. eLife 2022; 11:75485. [PMID: 35536739 PMCID: PMC9090327 DOI: 10.7554/elife.75485] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/11/2021] [Accepted: 05/01/2022] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Humans have the amazing ability to learn new visual concepts from just a single exemplar. How we achieve this remains mysterious. State-of-the-art theories suggest observers rely on internal 'generative models', which not only describe observed objects, but can also synthesize novel variations. However, compelling evidence for generative models in human one-shot learning remains sparse. In most studies, participants merely compare candidate objects created by the experimenters, rather than generating their own ideas. Here, we overcame this key limitation by presenting participants with 2D 'Exemplar' shapes and asking them to draw their own 'Variations' belonging to the same class. The drawings reveal that participants inferred-and synthesized-genuine novel categories that were far more varied than mere copies. Yet, there was striking agreement between participants about which shape features were most distinctive, and these tended to be preserved in the drawn Variations. Indeed, swapping distinctive parts caused objects to swap apparent category. Our findings suggest that internal generative models are key to how humans generalize from single exemplars. When observers see a novel object for the first time, they identify its most distinctive features and infer a generative model of its shape, allowing them to mentally synthesize plausible variants.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Henning Tiedemann
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Justus Liebig University Giessen, Giessen, Germany
| | - Yaniv Morgenstern
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Justus Liebig University Giessen, Giessen, Germany.,Laboratory of Experimental Psychology, University of Leuven (KU Leuven), Leuven, Belgium
| | - Filipp Schmidt
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Justus Liebig University Giessen, Giessen, Germany.,Center for Mind, Brain and Behavior (CMBB), University of Marburg and Justus Liebig University Giessen, Giessen, Germany
| | - Roland W Fleming
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Justus Liebig University Giessen, Giessen, Germany.,Center for Mind, Brain and Behavior (CMBB), University of Marburg and Justus Liebig University Giessen, Giessen, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
29
|
Děchtěrenko F, Lukavský J. False memories when viewing overlapping scenes. PeerJ 2022; 10:e13187. [PMID: 35411252 PMCID: PMC8994494 DOI: 10.7717/peerj.13187] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/08/2021] [Accepted: 03/08/2022] [Indexed: 01/12/2023] Open
Abstract
Humans can memorize and later recognize many objects and complex scenes. In this study, we prepared large photographs and presented participants with only partial views to test the fidelity of their memories. The unpresented parts of the photographs were used as a source of distractors with similar semantic and perceptual information. Additionally, we presented overlapping views to determine whether the second presentation provided a memory advantage for later recognition tests. Experiment 1 (N = 28) showed that while people were good at recognizing presented content and identifying new foils, they showed a remarkable level of uncertainty about foils selected from the unseen parts of presented photographs (false alarm, 59%). The recognition accuracy was higher for the parts that were shown twice, irrespective of whether the same identical photograph was viewed twice or whether two photographs with overlapping content were observed. In Experiment 2 (N = 28), the memorability of the large image was estimated by a pre-trained deep neural network. Neither the recognition accuracy for an image part nor the tendency for false alarms correlated with the memorability. Finally, in Experiment 3 (N = 21), we repeated the experiment while measuring eye movements. Fixations were biased toward the center of the original large photograph in the first presentation, and this bias was repeated during the second presentation in both identical and overlapping views. Altogether, our experiments show that people recognize parts of remembered photographs, but they find it difficult to reject foils from unseen parts, suggesting that their memory representation is not sufficiently detailed to rule them out as distractors.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Filip Děchtěrenko
- Institute of Psychology, Czech Academy of Sciences, Prague, Czech Republic
| | - Jiří Lukavský
- Institute of Psychology, Czech Academy of Sciences, Prague, Czech Republic
| |
Collapse
|
30
|
Bainbridge WA. A tutorial on capturing mental representations through drawing and crowd-sourced scoring. Behav Res Methods 2022; 54:663-675. [PMID: 34341961 PMCID: PMC9046317 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-021-01672-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 07/13/2021] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
When we draw, we are depicting a rich mental representation reflecting a memory, percept, schema, imagination, or feeling. In spite of the abundance of data created by drawings, drawings are rarely used as an output measure in the field of psychology, due to concerns about their large variance and their difficulty of quantification. However, recent work leveraging pen-tracking, computer vision, and online crowd-sourcing has revealed new ways to capture and objectively quantify drawings, to answer a wide range of questions across fields of psychology. Here, I present a tutorial on modern methods for drawing experiments, ranging from how to quantify pen-and-paper type studies, up to how to administer a fully closed-loop online experiment. I go through the concrete steps of designing a drawing experiment, recording drawings, and objectively quantifying them through online crowd-sourcing and computer vision methods. Included with this tutorial are code examples at different levels of complexity and tutorials designed to teach basic lessons about web architecture and be useful regardless of skill level. I also discuss key methodological points of consideration, and provide a series of potential jumping points for drawing studies across fields in psychology. I hope this tutorial will arm more researchers with the skills to capture these naturalistic snapshots of a mental image.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Wilma A Bainbridge
- Department of Psychology, University of Chicago, 5848 S University Ave, Beecher Hall 303, Chicago, IL, 60637, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
31
|
Hayes TR, Henderson JM. Meaning maps detect the removal of local semantic scene content but deep saliency models do not. Atten Percept Psychophys 2022; 84:647-654. [PMID: 35138579 PMCID: PMC11128357 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-021-02395-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 10/12/2021] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Meaning mapping uses human raters to estimate different semantic features in scenes, and has been a useful tool in demonstrating the important role semantics play in guiding attention. However, recent work has argued that meaning maps do not capture semantic content, but like deep learning models of scene attention, represent only semantically-neutral image features. In the present study, we directly tested this hypothesis using a diffeomorphic image transformation that is designed to remove the meaning of an image region while preserving its image features. Specifically, we tested whether meaning maps and three state-of-the-art deep learning models were sensitive to the loss of semantic content in this critical diffeomorphed scene region. The results were clear: meaning maps generated by human raters showed a large decrease in the diffeomorphed scene regions, while all three deep saliency models showed a moderate increase in the diffeomorphed scene regions. These results demonstrate that meaning maps reflect local semantic content in scenes while deep saliency models do something else. We conclude the meaning mapping approach is an effective tool for estimating semantic content in scenes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Taylor R Hayes
- Center for Mind and Brain, University of California, Davis, CA, USA.
| | - John M Henderson
- Center for Mind and Brain, University of California, Davis, CA, USA
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Davis, CA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
32
|
Hedayati S, O'Donnell RE, Wyble B. A model of working memory for latent representations. Nat Hum Behav 2022; 6:709-719. [PMID: 35115675 DOI: 10.1038/s41562-021-01264-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/03/2021] [Accepted: 11/29/2021] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
Abstract
We propose a mechanistic explanation of how working memories are built and reconstructed from the latent representations of visual knowledge. The proposed model features a variational autoencoder with an architecture that corresponds broadly to the human visual system and an activation-based binding pool of neurons that links latent space activities to tokenized representations. The simulation results revealed that new pictures of familiar types of items can be encoded and retrieved efficiently from higher levels of the visual hierarchy, whereas truly novel patterns are better stored using only early layers. Moreover, a given stimulus in working memory can have multiple codes, which allows representation of visual detail in addition to categorical information. Finally, we validated our model's assumptions by testing a series of predictions against behavioural results obtained from working memory tasks. The model provides a demonstration of how visual knowledge yields compact visual representation for efficient memory encoding.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Shekoofeh Hedayati
- Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, USA.
| | - Ryan E O'Donnell
- Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, USA
| | - Brad Wyble
- Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
33
|
Son G, Walther DB, Mack ML. Scene wheels: Measuring perception and memory of real-world scenes with a continuous stimulus space. Behav Res Methods 2022; 54:444-456. [PMID: 34244986 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-021-01630-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 05/19/2021] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Precisely characterizing mental representations of visual experiences requires careful control of experimental stimuli. Recent work leveraging such stimulus control has led to important insights; however, these findings are constrained to simple visual properties like color and line orientation. There remains a critical methodological barrier to characterizing perceptual and mnemonic representations of realistic visual experiences. Here, we introduce a novel method to systematically control visual properties of natural scene stimuli. Using generative adversarial networks (GANs), a state-of-the-art deep learning technique for creating highly realistic synthetic images, we generated scene wheels in which continuously changing visual properties smoothly transition between meaningful realistic scenes. To validate the efficacy of scene wheels, we conducted two behavioral experiments that assess perceptual and mnemonic representations attained from the scene wheels. In the perceptual validation experiment, we tested whether the continuous transition of scene images along the wheel is reflected in human perceptual similarity judgment. The perceived similarity of the scene images correspondingly decreased as distances between the images increase on the wheel. In the memory experiment, participants reconstructed to-be-remembered scenes from the scene wheels. Reconstruction errors for these scenes resemble error distributions observed in prior studies using simple stimulus properties. Importantly, perceptual similarity judgment and memory precision varied systematically with scene wheel radius. These findings suggest our novel approach offers a window into the mental representations of naturalistic visual experiences.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gaeun Son
- Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Sidney Smith Hall, 100 St George St, Toronto, ON, Canada.
| | - Dirk B Walther
- Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Sidney Smith Hall, 100 St George St, Toronto, ON, Canada
| | - Michael L Mack
- Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Sidney Smith Hall, 100 St George St, Toronto, ON, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
34
|
Hall EH, Bainbridge WA, Baker CI. Highly similar and competing visual scenes lead to diminished object but not spatial detail in memory drawings. Memory 2021; 30:279-292. [PMID: 34913412 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2021.2010761] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
Abstract
Drawings of scenes made from memory can be highly detailed and spatially accurate, with little information not found in the observed stimuli. While prior work has focused on studying memory for distinct scenes, less is known about the specific detail recalled when episodes are highly similar and competing. Here, participants (N = 30) were asked to study and recall eight complex scene images using a drawing task. Importantly, four of these images were exemplars of different scene categories, while the other four images were from the same scene category. The resulting 213 drawings were judged by 1764 online scorers for a comprehensive set of measures, including scene and object diagnosticity, spatial information, and fixation and pen movement behaviour. We observed that competition in memory resulted in diminished object detail, with drawings and objects that were less diagnostic of their original image. However, repeated exemplars of a category did not result in differences in spatial memory accuracy, and there were no differences in fixations during study or pen movements during recall. These results reveal that while drawings for distinct categories of scenes can be highly detailed and accurate, drawings for scenes from repeated categories, creating competition in memory, show reduced object detail.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elizabeth H Hall
- Department of Psychology, University of California Davis, Davis, CA, USA.,Center for Mind and Brain, University of California Davis, Davis, CA, USA.,Laboratory of Brain and Cognition, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, MD, USA
| | | | - Chris I Baker
- Laboratory of Brain and Cognition, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, MD, USA
| |
Collapse
|
35
|
Abstract
We often take people’s ability to understand and produce line drawings for granted. But where should we draw lines, and why? We address psychological principles that underlie efficient representations of complex information in line drawings. First, 58 participants with varying degree of artistic experience produced multiple drawings of a small set of scenes by tracing contours on a digital tablet. Second, 37 independent observers ranked the drawings by how representative they are of the original photograph. Matching contours between drawings of the same scene revealed that the most consistently drawn contours tend to be drawn earlier. We generated half-images with the most- versus least-consistently drawn contours and asked 25 observers categorize the quickly presented scenes. Observers performed significantly better for the most compared to the least consistent half-images. The most consistently drawn contours were more likely to depict occlusion boundaries, whereas the least consistently drawn contours frequently depicted surface normals.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Heping Sheng
- School of Medicine, Boston University, Boston, MA, United States of America
| | - John Wilder
- Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Canada
| | - Dirk B. Walther
- Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Canada
- * E-mail:
| |
Collapse
|
36
|
Henderson JM, Hayes TR, Peacock CE, Rehrig G. Meaning maps capture the density of local semantic features in scenes: A reply to Pedziwiatr, Kümmerer, Wallis, Bethge & Teufel (2021). Cognition 2021; 214:104742. [PMID: 33892912 PMCID: PMC11166323 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104742] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/12/2021] [Revised: 04/13/2021] [Accepted: 04/15/2021] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
Pedziwiatr, Kümmerer, Wallis, Bethge, & Teufel (2021) contend that Meaning Maps do not represent the spatial distribution of semantic features in scenes. We argue that Pesziwiatr et al. provide neither logical nor empirical support for that claim, and we conclude that Meaning Maps do what they were designed to do: represent the spatial distribution of meaning in scenes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- John M Henderson
- Center for Mind and Brain, University of California, Davis, USA; Department of Psychology, University of California, Davis, USA.
| | - Taylor R Hayes
- Center for Mind and Brain, University of California, Davis, USA
| | - Candace E Peacock
- Center for Mind and Brain, University of California, Davis, USA; Department of Psychology, University of California, Davis, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
37
|
Grande X, Berron D, Maass A, Bainbridge WA, Düzel E. Content-specific vulnerability of recent episodic memories in Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychologia 2021; 160:107976. [PMID: 34314781 PMCID: PMC8434425 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2021.107976] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/09/2021] [Revised: 07/21/2021] [Accepted: 07/22/2021] [Indexed: 11/21/2022]
Abstract
Endel Tulving's episodic memory framework emphasizes the multifaceted re-experiencing of personal events. Indeed, decades of research focused on the experiential nature of episodic memories, usually treating recent episodic memory as a coherent experiential quality. However, recent insights into the functional architecture of the medial temporal lobe show that different types of mnemonic information are segregated into distinct neural pathways in brain circuits empirically associated with episodic memory. Moreover, recent memories do not fade as a whole under conditions of progressive neurodegeneration in these brain circuits, notably in Alzheimer's disease. Instead, certain memory content seem particularly vulnerable from the moment of their encoding while other content can remain memorable consistently across individuals and contexts. We propose that these observations are related to the content-specific functional architecture of the medial temporal lobe and consequently to a content-specific impairment of memory at different stages of the neurodegeneration. To develop Endel Tulving's inspirational legacy further and to advance our understanding of how memory function is affected by neurodegenerative conditions such as Alzheimer's disease, we postulate that it is compelling to focus on the representational content of recent episodic memories. The functional anatomy of episodic memory segregates different memory content. Alzheimer's disease may cause content-specific loss of recent memories Content-specific memorability across individuals changes with Alzheimer's disease. Content-specific assessment could provide new insights into episodic memory in health and disease
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Xenia Grande
- German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases, Magdeburg, Germany; Institute of Cognitive Neurology and Dementia Research, Otto von Guericke University Magdeburg, Germany.
| | - David Berron
- German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases, Magdeburg, Germany; Clinical Memory Research Unit, Department of Clinical Sciences Malmö, Lund University, Lund, Sweden
| | - Anne Maass
- German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases, Magdeburg, Germany
| | | | - Emrah Düzel
- German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases, Magdeburg, Germany; Institute of Cognitive Neurology and Dementia Research, Otto von Guericke University Magdeburg, Germany; Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, United Kingdom.
| |
Collapse
|
38
|
Visual memorability in the absence of semantic content. Cognition 2021; 212:104714. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104714] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/02/2018] [Revised: 02/15/2021] [Accepted: 03/28/2021] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
|
39
|
Bainbridge WA, Pounder Z, Eardley AF, Baker CI. Quantifying aphantasia through drawing: Those without visual imagery show deficits in object but not spatial memory. Cortex 2021; 135:159-172. [PMID: 33383478 PMCID: PMC7856239 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2020.11.014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 71] [Impact Index Per Article: 17.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/14/2020] [Revised: 07/31/2020] [Accepted: 11/10/2020] [Indexed: 01/19/2023]
Abstract
Congenital aphantasia is a recently characterized variation of experience defined by the inability to form voluntary visual imagery, in individuals who are otherwise high performing. Because of this specific deficit to visual imagery, individuals with aphantasia serve as an ideal group for probing the nature of representations in visual memory, particularly the interplay of object, spatial, and symbolic information. Here, we conducted a large-scale online study of aphantasia and revealed a dissociation in object and spatial content in their memory representations. Sixty-one individuals with aphantasia and matched controls with typical imagery studied real-world scene images, and were asked to draw them from memory, and then later copy them during a matched perceptual condition. Drawings were objectively quantified by 2,795 online scorers for object and spatial details. Aphantasic participants recalled significantly fewer objects than controls, with less color in their drawings, and an increased reliance on verbal scaffolding. However, aphantasic participants showed high spatial accuracy equivalent to controls, and made significantly fewer memory errors. These differences between groups only manifested during recall, with no differences between groups during the matched perceptual condition. This object-specific memory impairment in individuals with aphantasia provides evidence for separate systems in memory that support object versus spatial information. The study also provides an important experimental validation for the existence of aphantasia as a variation in human imagery experience.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Wilma A Bainbridge
- Department of Psychology, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA; Laboratory of Brain and Cognition, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, MD, USA.
| | - Zoë Pounder
- Department of Psychology, University of Westminster, London, UK.
| | - Alison F Eardley
- Department of Psychology, University of Westminster, London, UK.
| | - Chris I Baker
- Laboratory of Brain and Cognition, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, MD, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
40
|
Abstract
Intraub posits the existence of two separate processes in scene memory: one in which we automatically extrapolate the visual information in a scene beyond its boundaries (scene construction), and one in which we normalize our memories to either a schema or an average (normalization). She claims that scene construction will lead to transformations exclusively in the direction of boundary extension (BE), while normalization will produce bidirectional transformations of both BE and the opposite effect of boundary contraction (BC). Thus, because we observed both BE and BC in our study [1], our paradigm must be tapping into additional cognitive processes than just scene construction. However, our paper [1] questions this premise - if BE and BC are equally common using large, representative stimulus sets in the same tasks used previously [2,3], then perhaps prior studies primarily found unidirectional BE effects due to limited stimulus sampling, and there was no privileged link between boundary extension and scene construction in memory to begin with.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Wilma A Bainbridge
- Department of Psychology, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL 60637, USA.
| | - Chris I Baker
- Laboratory of Brain and Cognition, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, MD 20814, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
41
|
Bainbridge WA, Hall EH, Baker CI. Distinct Representational Structure and Localization for Visual Encoding and Recall during Visual Imagery. Cereb Cortex 2020; 31:1898-1913. [PMID: 33285563 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhaa329] [Citation(s) in RCA: 36] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/09/2020] [Revised: 10/12/2020] [Accepted: 10/12/2020] [Indexed: 01/03/2023] Open
Abstract
During memory recall and visual imagery, reinstatement is thought to occur as an echoing of the neural patterns during encoding. However, the precise information in these recall traces is relatively unknown, with previous work primarily investigating either broad distinctions or specific images, rarely bridging these levels of information. Using ultra-high-field (7T) functional magnetic resonance imaging with an item-based visual recall task, we conducted an in-depth comparison of encoding and recall along a spectrum of granularity, from coarse (scenes, objects) to mid (e.g., natural, manmade scenes) to fine (e.g., living room, cupcake) levels. In the scanner, participants viewed a trial-unique item, and after a distractor task, visually imagined the initial item. During encoding, we observed decodable information at all levels of granularity in category-selective visual cortex. In contrast, information during recall was primarily at the coarse level with fine-level information in some areas; there was no evidence of mid-level information. A closer look revealed segregation between voxels showing the strongest effects during encoding and those during recall, and peaks of encoding-recall similarity extended anterior to category-selective cortex. Collectively, these results suggest visual recall is not merely a reactivation of encoding patterns, displaying a different representational structure and localization from encoding, despite some overlap.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Wilma A Bainbridge
- Department of Psychology, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL 60637, USA.,Laboratory of Brain and Cognition, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, MD 20814, USA
| | - Elizabeth H Hall
- Laboratory of Brain and Cognition, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, MD 20814, USA.,Department of Psychology, University of California Davis, Davis, CA 95616, USA.,Center for Mind and Brain, University of California Davis, Davis, CA 95618, USA
| | - Chris I Baker
- Laboratory of Brain and Cognition, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, MD 20814, USA
| |
Collapse
|
42
|
Diamond NB, Armson MJ, Levine B. The Truth Is Out There: Accuracy in Recall of Verifiable Real-World Events. Psychol Sci 2020; 31:1544-1556. [DOI: 10.1177/0956797620954812] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022] Open
Abstract
How accurate is memory? Although people implicitly assume that their memories faithfully represent past events, the prevailing view in research is that memories are error prone and constructive. Yet little is known about the frequency of errors, particularly in memories for naturalistic experiences. Here, younger and older adults underwent complex real-world experiences that were nonetheless controlled and verifiable, freely recalling these experiences after days to years. As expected, memory quantity and the richness of episodic detail declined with increasing age and retention interval. Details that participants did recall, however, were highly accurate (93%–95%) across age and time. This level of accuracy far exceeded comparatively low estimations among memory scientists and other academics in a survey. These findings suggest that details freely recalled from one-time real-world experiences can retain high correspondence to the ground truth despite significant forgetting, with higher accuracy than expected given the emphasis on fallibility in the field of memory research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nicholas B. Diamond
- Rotman Research Institute, Baycrest Health Sciences, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
- Department of Psychology, University of Toronto
- Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania
| | - Michael J. Armson
- Rotman Research Institute, Baycrest Health Sciences, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
- Department of Psychology, University of Toronto
| | - Brian Levine
- Rotman Research Institute, Baycrest Health Sciences, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
- Department of Psychology, University of Toronto
- Department of Medicine (Neurology), University of Toronto
| |
Collapse
|
43
|
Ramey MM, Henderson JM, Yonelinas AP. The spatial distribution of attention predicts familiarity strength during encoding and retrieval. J Exp Psychol Gen 2020; 149:2046-2062. [PMID: 32250136 PMCID: PMC7541439 DOI: 10.1037/xge0000758] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
The memories we form are determined by what we attend to, and conversely, what we attend to is influenced by our memory for past experiences. Although we know that shifts of attention via eye movements are related to memory during encoding and retrieval, the role of specific memory processes in this relationship is unclear. There is evidence that attention may be especially important for some forms of memory (i.e., conscious recollection), and less so for others (i.e., familiarity-based recognition and unconscious influences of memory), but results are conflicting with respect to both the memory processes and eye movement patterns involved. To address this, we used a confidence-based method of isolating eye movement indices of spatial attention that are related to different memory processes (i.e., recollection, familiarity strength, and unconscious memory) during encoding and retrieval of real-world scenes. We also developed a new method of measuring the dispersion of eye movements, which proved to be more sensitive to memory processing than previously used measures. Specifically, in 2 studies, we found that familiarity strength-that is, changes in subjective reports of memory confidence-increased with (a) more dispersed patterns of viewing during encoding, (b) less dispersed viewing during retrieval, and (c) greater overlap in regions viewed between encoding and retrieval (i.e., resampling). Recollection was also related to these eye movements in a similar manner, though the associations with recollection were less consistent across experiments. Furthermore, we found no evidence for effects related to unconscious influences of memory. These findings indicate that attentional processes during viewing may not preferentially relate to recollection, and that the spatial distribution of eye movements is directly related to familiarity-based memory during encoding and retrieval. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michelle M. Ramey
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Davis, CA, USA
- Center for Neuroscience, University of California, Davis, CA, USA
- Center for Mind and Brain, University of California, Davis, CA, USA
| | - John M. Henderson
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Davis, CA, USA
- Center for Mind and Brain, University of California, Davis, CA, USA
| | - Andrew P. Yonelinas
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Davis, CA, USA
- Center for Neuroscience, University of California, Davis, CA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
44
|
Henderson JM, Goold JE, Choi W, Hayes TR. Neural Correlates of Fixated Low- and High-level Scene Properties during Active Scene Viewing. J Cogn Neurosci 2020; 32:2013-2023. [PMID: 32573384 PMCID: PMC11164273 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_01599] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
Abstract
During real-world scene perception, viewers actively direct their attention through a scene in a controlled sequence of eye fixations. During each fixation, local scene properties are attended, analyzed, and interpreted. What is the relationship between fixated scene properties and neural activity in the visual cortex? Participants inspected photographs of real-world scenes in an MRI scanner while their eye movements were recorded. Fixation-related fMRI was used to measure activation as a function of lower- and higher-level scene properties at fixation, operationalized as edge density and meaning maps, respectively. We found that edge density at fixation was most associated with activation in early visual areas, whereas semantic content at fixation was most associated with activation along the ventral visual stream including core object and scene-selective areas (lateral occipital complex, parahippocampal place area, occipital place area, and retrosplenial cortex). The observed activation from semantic content was not accounted for by differences in edge density. The results are consistent with active vision models in which fixation gates detailed visual analysis for fixated scene regions, and this gating influences both lower and higher levels of scene analysis.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Wonil Choi
- Gwangju Institute of Science and Technology
| | | |
Collapse
|
45
|
Rust NC, Mehrpour V. Understanding Image Memorability. Trends Cogn Sci 2020; 24:557-568. [DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2020.04.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/04/2020] [Revised: 04/10/2020] [Accepted: 04/11/2020] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
|
46
|
Xie W, Bainbridge WA, Inati SK, Baker CI, Zaghloul KA. Memorability of words in arbitrary verbal associations modulates memory retrieval in the anterior temporal lobe. Nat Hum Behav 2020; 4:937-948. [PMID: 32601459 PMCID: PMC7501186 DOI: 10.1038/s41562-020-0901-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/25/2019] [Accepted: 05/27/2020] [Indexed: 01/08/2023]
Abstract
Despite large individual differences in memory performance, people remember certain stimuli with overwhelming consistency. This phenomenon is referred to as the memorability of an individual item. However, it remains unknown whether memorability also affects our ability to retrieve associations between items. Here, using a paired-associates verbal memory task, we combine behavioural data, computational modelling and direct recordings from the human brain to examine how memorability influences associative memory retrieval. We find that certain words are correctly retrieved across participants irrespective of the cues used to initiate memory retrieval. These words, which share greater semantic similarity with other words, are more readily available during retrieval and lead to more intrusions when retrieval fails. Successful retrieval of these memorable items, relative to less memorable ones, results in faster reinstatement of neural activity in the anterior temporal lobe. Collectively, our data reveal how the brain prioritizes certain information to facilitate memory retrieval.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Weizhen Xie
- Surgical Neurology Branch, NINDS, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD, USA.
| | - Wilma A Bainbridge
- Department of Psychology, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA. .,Laboratory of Brain and Cognition, NIMH, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD, USA.
| | - Sara K Inati
- Office of the Clinical Director, NINDS, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD, USA
| | - Chris I Baker
- Laboratory of Brain and Cognition, NIMH, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD, USA
| | - Kareem A Zaghloul
- Surgical Neurology Branch, NINDS, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
47
|
Helbing J, Draschkow D, Võ MLH. Search superiority: Goal-directed attentional allocation creates more reliable incidental identity and location memory than explicit encoding in naturalistic virtual environments. Cognition 2020; 196:104147. [PMID: 32004760 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2019.104147] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/09/2019] [Revised: 11/19/2019] [Accepted: 11/20/2019] [Indexed: 01/23/2023]
Abstract
We use representations and expectations formed during life-long learning to support attentional allocation and perception. In comparison to traditional laboratory investigations, real-world memory formation is usually achieved without explicit instruction and on-the-fly as a by-product of natural interactions with our environment. Understanding this process and the quality of naturally formed representations is critical to understanding how memory is used to guide attention and perception. Utilizing immersive, navigable, and realistic virtual environments, we investigated incidentally generated memory representations by comparing them to memories for items which were explicitly memorized. Participants either searched for objects embedded in realistic indoor environments or explicitly memorized them for follow-up identity and location memory tests. We show for the first time that memory for the identity of naturalistic objects and their location in 3D space is higher after incidental encoding compared to explicit memorization, even though the subsequent memory tests came as a surprise to participants. Relating gaze behavior to memory performance revealed that encoding time was more predictive of subsequent memory when participants explicitly memorized an item, compared to incidentally encoding it. Our results suggest that the active nature of guiding attentional allocation during proactive behavior allows for behaviorally optimal formation and utilization of representations. This highlights the importance of investigating cognition under ecologically valid conditions and shows that understanding the most natural processes for encoding and maintaining information is critical for understanding adaptive behavior.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jason Helbing
- Scene Grammar Lab, Department of Psychology, Goethe University Frankfurt, Frankfurt am Main, Germany
| | - Dejan Draschkow
- Scene Grammar Lab, Department of Psychology, Goethe University Frankfurt, Frankfurt am Main, Germany; Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Oxford, England, United Kingdom of Great Britain and Northern Ireland.
| | - Melissa L-H Võ
- Scene Grammar Lab, Department of Psychology, Goethe University Frankfurt, Frankfurt am Main, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
48
|
Bainbridge WA, Baker CI. Boundaries Extend and Contract in Scene Memory Depending on Image Properties. Curr Biol 2020; 30:537-543.e3. [PMID: 31983637 DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2019.12.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 32] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/12/2019] [Revised: 11/05/2019] [Accepted: 12/02/2019] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
Boundary extension, a memory distortion in which observers consistently recall a scene with visual information beyond its boundaries, is widely accepted across the psychological sciences as a phenomenon revealing fundamental insight into memory representations [1-3], robust across paradigms [1, 4] and age groups [5-7]. This phenomenon has been taken to suggest that the mental representation of a scene consists of an intermingling of sensory information and a schema that extrapolates the views of a presented scene [8], and it has been used to provide evidence for the role of the neocortex [9] and hippocampus [10, 11] in the schematization of scenes during memory. However, the study of boundary extension has typically focused on object-oriented images that are not representative of our visuospatial world. Here, using a broad set of 1,000 images tested on 2,000 participants in a rapid recognition task, we discover "boundary contraction" as an equally robust phenomenon. Further, image composition largely drives whether extension or contraction is observed-although object-oriented images cause more boundary extension, scene-oriented images cause more boundary contraction. Finally, these effects also occur during drawing tasks, including a task with minimal memory load-when participants copy an image during viewing. Collectively, these results show that boundary extension is not a universal phenomenon and put into question the assumption that scene memory automatically combines visual information with additional context derived from internal schema. Instead, our memory for a scene may be largely driven by its visual composition, with a tendency to extend or contract the boundaries equally likely.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Wilma A Bainbridge
- Laboratory of Brain and Cognition, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, MD 20814, USA.
| | - Chris I Baker
- Laboratory of Brain and Cognition, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, MD 20814, USA
| |
Collapse
|
49
|
Relating Visual Production and Recognition of Objects in Human Visual Cortex. J Neurosci 2019; 40:1710-1721. [PMID: 31871278 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.1843-19.2019] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/30/2019] [Revised: 11/23/2019] [Accepted: 11/26/2019] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
Drawing is a powerful tool that can be used to convey rich perceptual information about objects in the world. What are the neural mechanisms that enable us to produce a recognizable drawing of an object, and how does this visual production experience influence how this object is represented in the brain? Here we evaluate the hypothesis that producing and recognizing an object recruit a shared neural representation, such that repeatedly drawing the object can enhance its perceptual discriminability in the brain. We scanned human participants (N = 31; 11 male) using fMRI across three phases of a training study: during training, participants repeatedly drew two objects in an alternating sequence on an MR-compatible tablet; before and after training, they viewed these and two other control objects, allowing us to measure the neural representation of each object in visual cortex. We found that: (1) stimulus-evoked representations of objects in visual cortex are recruited during visually cued production of drawings of these objects, even throughout the period when the object cue is no longer present; (2) the object currently being drawn is prioritized in visual cortex during drawing production, while other repeatedly drawn objects are suppressed; and (3) patterns of connectivity between regions in occipital and parietal cortex supported enhanced decoding of the currently drawn object across the training phase, suggesting a potential neural substrate for learning how to transform perceptual representations into representational actions. Together, our study provides novel insight into the functional relationship between visual production and recognition in the brain.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Humans can produce simple line drawings that capture rich information about their perceptual experiences. However, the mechanisms that support this behavior are not well understood. Here we investigate how regions in visual cortex participate in the recognition of an object and the production of a drawing of it. We find that these regions carry diagnostic information about an object in a similar format both during recognition and production, and that practice drawing an object enhances transmission of information about it to downstream regions. Together, our study provides novel insight into the functional relationship between visual production and recognition in the brain.
Collapse
|
50
|
Bainbridge WA, Berron D, Schütze H, Cardenas-Blanco A, Metzger C, Dobisch L, Bittner D, Glanz W, Spottke A, Rudolph J, Brosseron F, Buerger K, Janowitz D, Fliessbach K, Heneka M, Laske C, Buchmann M, Peters O, Diesing D, Li S, Priller J, Spruth EJ, Altenstein S, Schneider A, Kofler B, Teipel S, Kilimann I, Wiltfang J, Bartels C, Wolfsgruber S, Wagner M, Jessen F, Baker CI, Düzel E. Memorability of photographs in subjective cognitive decline and mild cognitive impairment: Implications for cognitive assessment. ALZHEIMER'S & DEMENTIA (AMSTERDAM, NETHERLANDS) 2019; 11:610-618. [PMID: 31517023 PMCID: PMC6732671 DOI: 10.1016/j.dadm.2019.07.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/19/2022]
Abstract
INTRODUCTION Impaired long-term memory is a defining feature of mild cognitive impairment (MCI). We tested whether this impairment is item specific, limited to some memoranda, whereas some remain consistently memorable. METHODS We conducted item-based analyses of long-term visual recognition memory. Three hundred ninety-four participants (healthy controls, subjective cognitive decline [SCD], and MCI) in the multicentric DZNE-Longitudinal Cognitive Impairment and Dementia Study (DELCODE) were tested with images from a pool of 835 photographs. RESULTS We observed consistent memorability for images in healthy controls, SCD, and MCI, predictable by a neural network trained on another healthy sample. Looking at memorability differences between groups, we identified images that could successfully categorize group membership with higher success and a substantial image reduction than the original image set. DISCUSSION Individuals with SCD and MCI show consistent memorability for specific items, while other items show significant diagnosticity. Certain stimulus features could optimize diagnostic assessment, while others could support memory.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Wilma A. Bainbridge
- Laboratory of Brain and Cognition, National Institute of Mental Health, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD, USA
| | - David Berron
- Institute of Cognitive Neurology and Dementia Research, Otto-von-Guericke University Magdeburg, Magdeburg, Germany
- German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Magdeburg, Germany
- Clinical Memory Research Unit, Department of Clinical Sciences Malmö, Lund University, Lund, Sweden
| | - Hartmut Schütze
- Institute of Cognitive Neurology and Dementia Research, Otto-von-Guericke University Magdeburg, Magdeburg, Germany
- German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Magdeburg, Germany
| | - Arturo Cardenas-Blanco
- Institute of Cognitive Neurology and Dementia Research, Otto-von-Guericke University Magdeburg, Magdeburg, Germany
- German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Magdeburg, Germany
| | - Coraline Metzger
- Institute of Cognitive Neurology and Dementia Research, Otto-von-Guericke University Magdeburg, Magdeburg, Germany
- German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Magdeburg, Germany
- Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University Hospital Magdeburg, Medical Faculty, Magdeburg, Germany
| | - Laura Dobisch
- German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Magdeburg, Germany
| | - Daniel Bittner
- German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Magdeburg, Germany
- Clinic for Neurology, University Hospital Magdeburg, Medical Faculty, Magdeburg, Germany
| | - Wenzel Glanz
- German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Magdeburg, Germany
| | - Annika Spottke
- German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Bonn, Germany
- Department of Neurology, University of Bonn, Bonn, Germany
| | - Janna Rudolph
- German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Bonn, Germany
| | - Frederic Brosseron
- German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Bonn, Germany
- Department of Neurodegeneration and Geriatric Psychiatry, University Hospital Bonn, Bonn, Germany
| | - Katharina Buerger
- German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Munich, Germany
- Institute for Stroke and Dementia Research, University Hospital, LMU Munich, Munich, Germany
| | - Daniel Janowitz
- Institute for Stroke and Dementia Research, University Hospital, LMU Munich, Munich, Germany
| | - Klaus Fliessbach
- Clinic for Neurology, University Hospital Magdeburg, Medical Faculty, Magdeburg, Germany
| | - Michael Heneka
- German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Bonn, Germany
- Department of Neurodegeneration and Geriatric Psychiatry, University Hospital Bonn, Bonn, Germany
| | - Christoph Laske
- German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Tübingen, Germany
- Section for Dementia Research, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Hertie Institute for Clinical Brain Research and University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
| | - Martina Buchmann
- German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Tübingen, Germany
- Section for Dementia Research, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Hertie Institute for Clinical Brain Research and University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
| | - Oliver Peters
- German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Berlin, Germany
- Charité – Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Freie Universität Berlin, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin Institute of Health, Institute of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Berlin, Germany
| | - Dominik Diesing
- Charité – Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Freie Universität Berlin, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin Institute of Health, Institute of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Berlin, Germany
| | - Siyao Li
- Charité – Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Freie Universität Berlin, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin Institute of Health, Institute of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Berlin, Germany
| | - Josef Priller
- German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Berlin, Germany
- Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Charité, Berlin, Germany
| | - Eike Jakob Spruth
- Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Charité, Berlin, Germany
| | - Slawek Altenstein
- German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Berlin, Germany
| | - Anja Schneider
- German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Bonn, Germany
- Department of Neurodegeneration and Geriatric Psychiatry, University Hospital Bonn, Bonn, Germany
| | - Barbara Kofler
- Department of Neurodegeneration and Geriatric Psychiatry, University Hospital Bonn, Bonn, Germany
| | - Stefan Teipel
- German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Rostock, Germany
- Department of Psychosomatic Medicine, Rostock University Medical Center, Rostock, Germany
| | - Ingo Kilimann
- German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Rostock, Germany
- Department of Psychosomatic Medicine, Rostock University Medical Center, Rostock, Germany
| | - Jens Wiltfang
- German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Goettingen, Germany
- Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University Medical Center Goettingen, University of Goettingen, Goettingen, Germany
| | - Claudia Bartels
- German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Goettingen, Germany
- Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University Medical Center Goettingen, University of Goettingen, Goettingen, Germany
| | | | - Michael Wagner
- German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Bonn, Germany
- Department of Neurodegeneration and Geriatric Psychiatry, University Hospital Bonn, Bonn, Germany
| | - Frank Jessen
- German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Bonn, Germany
- Department of Psychiatry, University of Cologne, Medical Faculty, Cologne, Germany
| | - Chris I. Baker
- Laboratory of Brain and Cognition, National Institute of Mental Health, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD, USA
| | - Emrah Düzel
- Institute of Cognitive Neurology and Dementia Research, Otto-von-Guericke University Magdeburg, Magdeburg, Germany
- German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Magdeburg, Germany
- Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London, UK
| |
Collapse
|