1
|
Vallesi A. Credibility revolution: pursuing a balanced and sustainable approach, without dogmas, without magic elixirs. Front Psychol 2025; 16:1581160. [PMID: 40309213 PMCID: PMC12041089 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1581160] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/21/2025] [Accepted: 03/28/2025] [Indexed: 05/02/2025] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Antonino Vallesi
- Department of Neuroscience, Università degli Studi di Padova, Padova, Italy
- Padova Neuroscience Center, Università degli Studi di Padova, Padova, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Kuhbandner C, Mayrhofer R. The hidden complexity of the simple world of basic experimental psychology: the principal and practical limits of gaining psychological knowledge using the experimental method. Front Psychol 2025; 15:1397553. [PMID: 40290648 PMCID: PMC12023913 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1397553] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/08/2024] [Accepted: 11/04/2024] [Indexed: 04/30/2025] Open
Abstract
Basic experimental research in psychology is based on the assumption that law-like behavior can be observed if the complexity of the human psyche is reduced by the creation of experimental settings in which simple psychical phenomena occur which reflect the effect of an isolated psychological mechanism. However, we show that this assumption does not hold for many phenomena studied in basic experimental psychology because even phenomena that are regarded as simple and fully controllable often fluctuate unpredictably as a function of unintentionally chosen details of the experimental setting. The reason is that in a complex system like the human psyche, even minimal, and from the perspective of the investigated research question irrelevant, differences in the experimental setting can build up to large unsystematic effects. Law-like behavior in experiments could only occur if truly low-level mechanisms were studied in a truly isolated way. However, this is often not the case in current experimental research. One problem is that often fuzzy theoretical terms are used which only give the impression that low-level mechanisms are being investigated, although in reality the complexity of the human psyche is unintentionally brought on board. Another problem is that, unlike in the natural sciences, the mechanisms of the human psyche can only be isolated from each other to a limited extent because the human psyche always reacts as a whole system. If such problems could be overcome, meaningful knowledge could be gained through experimental psychological research. However, the knowledge gained is very limited in terms of its explanatory power for human behavior, as it is only helpful for understanding a very specific aspect of behavior, namely the mechanistic functioning of isolated low-level mechanisms. When it comes to understanding motivated behavior in real life, knowledge about the non-mechanistic functioning of the higher levels of the human psyche is necessary, but this knowledge cannot be gained through the experimental method.
Collapse
|
3
|
Chakravorti T, Koneru S, Rajtmajer S. Reproducibility and replicability in research: What 452 professors think in Universities across the USA and India. PLoS One 2025; 20:e0319334. [PMID: 40138274 PMCID: PMC11940819 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0319334] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/02/2024] [Accepted: 01/29/2025] [Indexed: 03/29/2025] Open
Abstract
In the past decade, open science and science of science communities have initiated innovative efforts to address concerns about the reproducibility and replicability of published scientific research. In some respects, these efforts have been successful, yet there are still many pockets of researchers with little to no familiarity with these concerns, subsequent responses, or best practices for engaging in reproducible, replicable, and reliable scholarship. In this study, we surveyed 452 professors from universities across the USA and India to understand perspectives on scientific processes and identify key points for intervention. Our findings reveal both national and disciplinary gaps in attention to reproducibility and transparency in science, aggravated by incentive misalignment and resource constraints. We suggest that solutions addressing scientific integrity should be culturally-centered, where definitions of culture should include both regional and domain-specific elements. This study examines research cultures in India and the USA across a diverse range of social science and engineering disciplines. The universities included in the study were carefully selected to represent various regions of each country and reflect institutions across different ranking levels, ensuring a broad and representative sample. While the findings provide valuable insights into the research environments of India and the USA, their applicability is limited to these two countries and respective disciplines. The survey relies on self-reported data, which can be subject to biases, e.g., social desirability or recall bias. Future research will expand the scope to include additional countries, allowing for a more comprehensive comparison of global research cultures. Additionally, we aim to investigate how regional, institutional, and disciplinary factors influence research practices and collaboration across borders, providing a deeper understanding of international academic environments.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tatiana Chakravorti
- Information Science and Technology, Pennsylvania State University, State College, Pennsylvania, United States of America
| | - Sai Koneru
- Information Science and Technology, Pennsylvania State University, State College, Pennsylvania, United States of America
| | - Sarah Rajtmajer
- Information Science and Technology, Pennsylvania State University, State College, Pennsylvania, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Skubera M, Korbmacher M, Evans TR, Azevedo F, Pennington CR. International initiatives to enhance awareness and uptake of open research in psychology: a systematic mapping review. ROYAL SOCIETY OPEN SCIENCE 2025; 12:241726. [PMID: 40109933 PMCID: PMC11919529 DOI: 10.1098/rsos.241726] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/04/2024] [Revised: 02/07/2025] [Accepted: 02/07/2025] [Indexed: 03/22/2025]
Abstract
Concerns about the replicability, reproducibility and transparency of research have ushered in a set of practices and behaviours under the umbrella of 'open research'. To this end, many new initiatives have been developed that represent procedural (i.e. behaviours and sets of commonly used practices in the research process), structural (new norms, rules, infrastructure and incentives), and community-based change (working groups, networks). The objectives of this research were to identify and outline international initiatives that enhance awareness and uptake of open research practices in the discipline of psychology. A systematic mapping review was conducted in three stages: (i) a Web search to identify open research initiatives in psychology; (ii) a literature search to identify related articles; and (iii) a hand search of grey literature. Eligible initiatives were then coded into an overarching theme of procedural, structural or community-based change. A total of 187 initiatives were identified; 30 were procedural (e.g. toolkits, resources, software), 70 structural (e.g. policies, strategies, frameworks) and 87 community-based (e.g. working groups, networks). This review highlights that open research is progressing at pace through various initiatives that share a common goal to reform research culture. We hope that this review promotes their further adoption and facilitates coordinated efforts between individuals, organizations, institutions, publishers and funders.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Magda Skubera
- School of Psychology, Aston University, Birmingham, UK
| | - Max Korbmacher
- Mohn Medical Imaging and Visualisation Centre, Bergen, Norway
- Department of Neurology, Haukeland University Hospital, Bergen, Norway
| | - Thomas Rhys Evans
- School of Human Sciences and Institute for Lifecourse Developments, University of Greenwich, London, UK
| | - Flavio Azevedo
- Department of Interdisciplinary Social Sciences, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| | | |
Collapse
|
5
|
Reilly J, Shain C, Borghesani V, Kuhnke P, Vigliocco G, Peelle JE, Mahon BZ, Buxbaum LJ, Majid A, Brysbaert M, Borghi AM, De Deyne S, Dove G, Papeo L, Pexman PM, Poeppel D, Lupyan G, Boggio P, Hickok G, Gwilliams L, Fernandino L, Mirman D, Chrysikou EG, Sandberg CW, Crutch SJ, Pylkkänen L, Yee E, Jackson RL, Rodd JM, Bedny M, Connell L, Kiefer M, Kemmerer D, de Zubicaray G, Jefferies E, Lynott D, Siew CSQ, Desai RH, McRae K, Diaz MT, Bolognesi M, Fedorenko E, Kiran S, Montefinese M, Binder JR, Yap MJ, Hartwigsen G, Cantlon J, Bi Y, Hoffman P, Garcea FE, Vinson D. What we mean when we say semantic: Toward a multidisciplinary semantic glossary. Psychon Bull Rev 2025; 32:243-280. [PMID: 39231896 PMCID: PMC11836185 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-024-02556-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 07/19/2024] [Indexed: 09/06/2024]
Abstract
Tulving characterized semantic memory as a vast repository of meaning that underlies language and many other cognitive processes. This perspective on lexical and conceptual knowledge galvanized a new era of research undertaken by numerous fields, each with their own idiosyncratic methods and terminology. For example, "concept" has different meanings in philosophy, linguistics, and psychology. As such, many fundamental constructs used to delineate semantic theories remain underspecified and/or opaque. Weak construct specificity is among the leading causes of the replication crisis now facing psychology and related fields. Term ambiguity hinders cross-disciplinary communication, falsifiability, and incremental theory-building. Numerous cognitive subdisciplines (e.g., vision, affective neuroscience) have recently addressed these limitations via the development of consensus-based guidelines and definitions. The project to follow represents our effort to produce a multidisciplinary semantic glossary consisting of succinct definitions, background, principled dissenting views, ratings of agreement, and subjective confidence for 17 target constructs (e.g., abstractness, abstraction, concreteness, concept, embodied cognition, event semantics, lexical-semantic, modality, representation, semantic control, semantic feature, simulation, semantic distance, semantic dimension). We discuss potential benefits and pitfalls (e.g., implicit bias, prescriptiveness) of these efforts to specify a common nomenclature that other researchers might index in specifying their own theoretical perspectives (e.g., They said X, but I mean Y).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Cory Shain
- Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, USA
| | | | - Philipp Kuhnke
- Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany
- Leipzig University, Leipzig, Germany
| | | | | | | | - Laurel J Buxbaum
- Thomas Jefferson University, Moss Rehabilitation Research Institute, Elkins Park, PA, USA
| | | | | | | | | | - Guy Dove
- University of Louisville, Louisville, KY, USA
| | - Liuba Papeo
- Centre National de La Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), University Claude-Bernard Lyon, Lyon, France
| | | | | | | | - Paulo Boggio
- Universidade Presbiteriana Mackenzie, São Paulo, Brazil
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | - Eiling Yee
- University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT, USA
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | - Ken McRae
- Western University, London, ON, Canada
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | - Melvin J Yap
- Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany
- National University of Singapore, Singapore, Singapore
| | - Gesa Hartwigsen
- Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany
- Leipzig University, Leipzig, Germany
| | | | - Yanchao Bi
- University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK
- Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
6
|
Ghai S, Thériault R, Forscher P, Shoda Y, Syed M, Puthillam A, Peng HC, Basnight-Brown D, Majid A, Azevedo F, Singh L. A manifesto for a globally diverse, equitable, and inclusive open science. COMMUNICATIONS PSYCHOLOGY 2025; 3:16. [PMID: 39880926 PMCID: PMC11779813 DOI: 10.1038/s44271-024-00179-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/15/2024] [Accepted: 12/13/2024] [Indexed: 01/31/2025]
Abstract
The field of psychology has rapidly transformed its open science practices in recent years. Yet there has been limited progress in integrating principles of diversity, equity and inclusion. In this Perspective, we raise the spectre of Questionable Generalisability Practices and the issue of MASKing (Making Assumptions based on Skewed Knowledge), calling for more responsible practices in generalising study findings and co-authorship to promote global equity in knowledge production. To drive change, researchers must target all four key components of the research process: design, reporting, generalisation, and evaluation. Additionally, macro-level geopolitical factors must be considered to move towards a robust behavioural science that is truly inclusive, representing the voices and experiences of the majority world (i.e., low-and-middle-income countries).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sakshi Ghai
- Department of Psychological and Behavioural Science, London School of Economics and Political Science, London, UK.
| | - Rémi Thériault
- Department of Psychology, Université du Québec à Montréal, Montréal, Canada
| | | | - Yuichi Shoda
- Department of Psychology, University of Washington, Seattle, USA
| | - Moin Syed
- Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Twin Cities, USA
| | - Arathy Puthillam
- Rady School of Management, University of California, San Diego, USA
| | - Hu Chuan Peng
- School of Psychology, Nanjing Normal University, Nanjing, China
| | - Dana Basnight-Brown
- Department of Psychology, United States International University-Africa, Nariobi, Kenya
| | - Asifa Majid
- Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
| | - Flavio Azevedo
- Interdisciplinary Social Science, Utrecht University, Utrecht, Netherlands
| | - Leher Singh
- Department of Psychology, University of Maryland, Baltimore County, Baltimore, USA
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Abalde SF, Rigby A, Keller PE, Novembre G. A framework for joint music making: Behavioral findings, neural processes, and computational models. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2024; 167:105816. [PMID: 39032841 DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2024.105816] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/20/2023] [Revised: 07/15/2024] [Accepted: 07/16/2024] [Indexed: 07/23/2024]
Abstract
Across different epochs and societies, humans occasionally gather to jointly make music. This universal form of collective behavior is as fascinating as it is fragmentedly understood. As the interest in joint music making (JMM) rapidly grows, we review the state-of-the-art of this emerging science, blending behavioral, neural, and computational contributions. We present a conceptual framework synthesizing research on JMM within four components. The framework is centered upon interpersonal coordination, a crucial requirement for JMM. The other components imply the influence of individuals' (past) experience, (current) social factors, and (future) goals on real-time coordination. Our aim is to promote the development of JMM research by organizing existing work, inspiring new questions, and fostering accessibility for researchers belonging to other research communities.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sara F Abalde
- Neuroscience of Perception and Action Lab, Italian Institute of Technology, Rome, Italy; The Open University Affiliated Research Centre at the Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia, Italy.
| | - Alison Rigby
- Neurosciences Graduate Program, University of California, San Diego, USA
| | - Peter E Keller
- Center for Music in the Brain, Aarhus University, Denmark; Department of Clinical Medicine, Center for Music in the Brain, Department of Clinical Medicine, Aarhus University, Denmark; The MARCS Institute for Brain, Behaviour and Development, Western Sydney University, Australia
| | - Giacomo Novembre
- Neuroscience of Perception and Action Lab, Italian Institute of Technology, Rome, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Wang SV, Pottegård A. Building transparency and reproducibility into the practice of pharmacoepidemiology and outcomes research. Am J Epidemiol 2024; 193:1625-1631. [PMID: 38794897 PMCID: PMC11538565 DOI: 10.1093/aje/kwae087] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/28/2023] [Revised: 01/11/2024] [Accepted: 05/10/2024] [Indexed: 05/26/2024] Open
Abstract
Real-world evidence (RWE) studies are increasingly used to inform policy and clinical decisions. However, there remain concerns about the credibility and reproducibility of RWE studies. While there is universal agreement on the critical importance of transparent and reproducible science, the building blocks of open science practice that are common across many disciplines have not yet been built into routine workflows for pharmacoepidemiology and outcomes researchers. Observational researchers should highlight the level of transparency of their studies by providing a succinct statement addressing study transparency with the publication of every paper, poster, or presentation that reports on an RWE study. In this paper, we propose a framework for an explicit transparency statement that declares the level of transparency a given RWE study has achieved across 5 key domains: (1) protocol, (2) preregistration, (3) data, (4) code-sharing, and (5) reporting checklists. The transparency statement outlined in the present paper can be used by research teams to proudly display the open science practices that were used to generate evidence designed to inform public health policy and practice. While transparency does not guarantee validity, such a statement signals confidence from the research team in the scientific choices that were made.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Shirley V Wang
- Division of Pharmacoepidemiology and Pharmacoeconomics, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Boston, MA 02120, United States
- Department of Medicine, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA 02115, United States
| | - Anton Pottegård
- Clinical Pharmacology, Pharmacy and Environmental Medicine, Department of Public Health, Faculty of Health Sciences, University of Southern Denmark, 5230 Odense M, Denmark
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Hillman C, Fontana BD, Amstislavskaya TG, Gorbunova MA, Altenhofen S, Barthelson K, Bastos LM, Borba JV, Bonan CD, Brennan CH, Farias-Cea A, Cooper A, Corcoran J, Dondossola ER, Martinez-Duran LM, Gallas-Lopes M, Galstyan DS, Garcia EO, Gerken E, Hindges R, Kenney JW, Kleshchev MA, Kolesnikova TO, Leggieri A, Khatsko SL, Lardelli M, Lodetti G, Lombardelli G, Luchiari AC, Portela SM, Medan V, Moutinho LM, Nekhoroshev EV, Petersen BD, Petrunich-Rutherford ML, Piato A, Porfiri M, Read E, Resmim CM, Rico EP, Rosemberg DB, de Abreu MS, Salazar CA, Stahloher-Buss T, Teixeira JR, Valentim AM, Zhdanov AV, Iturriaga-Vásquez P, Wang X, Wong RY, Kalueff AV, Parker MO. Housing and Husbandry Factors Affecting Zebrafish ( Danio rerio) Novel Tank Test Responses: A Global Multi-Laboratory Study. RESEARCH SQUARE 2024:rs.3.rs-4849877. [PMID: 39483890 PMCID: PMC11527349 DOI: 10.21203/rs.3.rs-4849877/v1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/03/2024]
Abstract
The reproducibility crisis in bioscience, characterized by inconsistent study results, impedes our understanding of biological processes and global collaborative studies offer a unique solution. This study is the first global collaboration using the zebrafish (Danio rerio) novel tank test, a behavioral assay for anxiety-like responses. We analyzed data from 20 laboratories worldwide, focusing on housing conditions and experimental setups. Our study included 488 adult zebrafish, tested for 5 min, focusing on a variety of variables. Key findings show females exhibit more anxiety-like behavior than males, underscoring sex as a critical variable. Housing conditions, including higher stocking densities and specific feed types, influenced anxiety levels. Optimal conditions (5 fish/L) and nutritionally rich feeds (e.g., rotifers), mitigated anxiety-like behaviors. Environmental stressors, like noise and transportation, significantly impacted behavior. We recommend standardizing protocols to account for sex differences, optimal stocking densities, nutritionally rich feeds, and minimizing stressors to improve zebrafish behavioral study reliability.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Courtney Hillman
- Surrey Sleep Research Centre, Department of Clinical and Experimental Medicine, School of Biosciences, University of Surrey, Guildford, Surrey, GU2 7XH, UK
| | - Barbara D. Fontana
- Laboratory of Experimental Neuropsychobiology, Department of Biochemistry and Molecular Biology, Natural and Exact Sciences Center, Federal University of Santa Maria, Santa Maria, RS 97105-900, Brazil
- Department of Biological Sciences, Wayne State University, Detroit, Michigan, 48202, USA
| | - Tamara G. Amstislavskaya
- Laboratory of experimental models of neuropsychiatric disorders, Scientific Research Institute of Neurosciences and Medicine, Novosibirsk State University, Novosibirsk, Russia
| | | | - Stefani Altenhofen
- Laboratório de Neuroquímica e Psicofarmacologia, Escola de Ciências da Saúde e da Vida, Pontifícia Universidade Católica do Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre, 90619-900, Porto Alegre, RS, BR
| | - Karissa Barthelson
- Alzheimer’s Disease Genetics Laboratory, School of Biological Sciences, Faculty of Sciences, Engineering and Technology, The University of Adelaide, North Terrace Campus, Adelaide, SA, Australia 5005
- Childhood Dementia Research Group, College of Medicine and Public Health, Flinders Health and Medical Research Institute, Flinders University, Bedford Park, SA 5042, Australia
| | - Leonardo M. Bastos
- Laboratório de Psicofarmacologia e Comportamento, Departamento de Farmacologia, Instituto de Ciências Básicas da Saúde, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul, Ramiro Barcelos 2600, Porto Alegre, RS, 90035-003, Brazil
| | - João V. Borba
- Laboratory of Experimental Neuropsychobiology, Department of Biochemistry and Molecular Biology, Natural and Exact Sciences Center, Federal University of Santa Maria, Santa Maria, RS 97105-900, Brazil
| | - Carla D. Bonan
- Laboratório de Neuroquímica e Psicofarmacologia, Escola de Ciências da Saúde e da Vida, Pontifícia Universidade Católica do Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre, 90619-900, Porto Alegre, RS, BR
| | - Caroline H. Brennan
- School of Biological and Behavioural Sciences, Queen Mary University of London, London, E1 4NS, UK
| | - Amaury Farias-Cea
- Molecular Pharmacology and Medicinal Chemistry Lab, Facultad de Ingeniería y Ciencias, Universidad de la Frontera, Temuco, Chile
| | - Austin Cooper
- Surrey Sleep Research Centre, Department of Clinical and Experimental Medicine, School of Biosciences, University of Surrey, Guildford, Surrey, GU2 7XH, UK
| | - Jamie Corcoran
- Department of Psychology, University of Nebraska at Omaha, Omaha, Nebraska, 68182 USA
| | - Eduardo R. Dondossola
- Translational Psychiatry Laboratory, Graduate Program in Health Sciences, University of Southern Santa Catarina (UNESC), Criciúma, SC, Brazil
| | - Luis M. Martinez-Duran
- Molecular Pharmacology and Medicinal Chemistry Lab, Facultad de Ingeniería y Ciencias, Universidad de la Frontera, Temuco, Chile
| | - Matheus Gallas-Lopes
- Laboratório de Psicofarmacologia e Comportamento, Departamento de Farmacologia, Instituto de Ciências Básicas da Saúde, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul, Ramiro Barcelos 2600, Porto Alegre, RS, 90035-003, Brazil
| | | | - Ella O. Garcia
- Department of Psychology, Indiana University Northwest, Gary, Indiana, 46408, USA
| | - Ewan Gerken
- Alzheimer’s Disease Genetics Laboratory, School of Biological Sciences, Faculty of Sciences, Engineering and Technology, The University of Adelaide, North Terrace Campus, Adelaide, SA, Australia 5005
| | - Robert Hindges
- Centre for Developmental Neurobiology & MRC Centre for Neurodevelopmental Disorders, King’s College London, London, England, UK
| | - Justin W. Kenney
- Department of Biological Sciences, Wayne State University, Detroit, Michigan, 48202, USA
| | - Maxim A. Kleshchev
- Laboratory of experimental models of neuropsychiatric disorders, Scientific Research Institute of Neurosciences and Medicine, Novosibirsk State University, Novosibirsk, Russia
| | | | - Adele Leggieri
- School of Biological and Behavioural Sciences, Queen Mary University of London, London, E1 4NS, UK
| | | | - Michael Lardelli
- Alzheimer’s Disease Genetics Laboratory, School of Biological Sciences, Faculty of Sciences, Engineering and Technology, The University of Adelaide, North Terrace Campus, Adelaide, SA, Australia 5005
| | - Guilherme Lodetti
- Translational Psychiatry Laboratory, Graduate Program in Health Sciences, University of Southern Santa Catarina (UNESC), Criciúma, SC, Brazil
| | - Giulia Lombardelli
- Department of Mechanical and Aerospace Engineering, Department of Biomedical Engineering, Center for Urban Science and Progress, New York University, Tandon School of Engineering, New York, 11201, USA
| | - Ana C. Luchiari
- FishLab, Department of Physiology and Behavior, Federal University of Rio Grande do Norte, Natal, Brazil
| | - Stefani M. Portela
- Laboratório de Psicofarmacologia e Comportamento, Departamento de Farmacologia, Instituto de Ciências Básicas da Saúde, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul, Ramiro Barcelos 2600, Porto Alegre, RS, 90035-003, Brazil
| | - Violeta Medan
- Instituto de Fisiología, Biología Molecular y Neurociencias, Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Tecnológicas. Facultad de Ciencias Exactas y Naturales, Universidad de Buenos Aires, Buenos Aires, Argentina
| | - Lirane M. Moutinho
- Instituto de Fisiología, Biología Molecular y Neurociencias, Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Tecnológicas. Facultad de Ciencias Exactas y Naturales, Universidad de Buenos Aires, Buenos Aires, Argentina
| | - Evgeny V. Nekhoroshev
- Laboratory of experimental models of neuropsychiatric disorders, Scientific Research Institute of Neurosciences and Medicine, Novosibirsk State University, Novosibirsk, Russia
| | - Barbara D. Petersen
- Laboratório de Neuroquímica e Psicofarmacologia, Escola de Ciências da Saúde e da Vida, Pontifícia Universidade Católica do Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre, 90619-900, Porto Alegre, RS, BR
| | | | - Angelo Piato
- Laboratório de Psicofarmacologia e Comportamento, Departamento de Farmacologia, Instituto de Ciências Básicas da Saúde, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul, Ramiro Barcelos 2600, Porto Alegre, RS, 90035-003, Brazil
| | - Maurizio Porfiri
- Department of Mechanical and Aerospace Engineering, Department of Biomedical Engineering, Center for Urban Science and Progress, New York University, Tandon School of Engineering, New York, 11201, USA
| | - Emily Read
- Centre for Developmental Neurobiology & MRC Centre for Neurodevelopmental Disorders, King’s College London, London, England, UK
| | - Cássio M. Resmim
- Laboratory of Experimental Neuropsychobiology, Department of Biochemistry and Molecular Biology, Natural and Exact Sciences Center, Federal University of Santa Maria, Santa Maria, RS 97105-900, Brazil
| | - Eduardo P. Rico
- Translational Psychiatry Laboratory, Graduate Program in Health Sciences, University of Southern Santa Catarina (UNESC), Criciúma, SC, Brazil
| | - Denis B. Rosemberg
- Laboratory of Experimental Neuropsychobiology, Department of Biochemistry and Molecular Biology, Natural and Exact Sciences Center, Federal University of Santa Maria, Santa Maria, RS 97105-900, Brazil
| | - Murilo S. de Abreu
- Graduate Program in Health Sciences, Federal University of Health Sciences of Porto Alegre, Porto Alegre, Brazil
- Western Caspian University, Baku, Azerbaijan
- The International Zebrafish Neuroscience Research Consortium (ZNRC), Slidell, LA 70458, USA
| | - Catia A. Salazar
- Department of Psychology, Indiana University Northwest, Gary, Indiana, 46408, USA
| | - Thaliana Stahloher-Buss
- Laboratório de Psicofarmacologia e Comportamento, Departamento de Farmacologia, Instituto de Ciências Básicas da Saúde, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul, Ramiro Barcelos 2600, Porto Alegre, RS, 90035-003, Brazil
| | - Júlia R. Teixeira
- FishLab, Department of Physiology and Behavior, Federal University of Rio Grande do Norte, Natal, Brazil
| | - Ana M. Valentim
- Laboratory Animal Science, Instituto de Investigação e Inovação em Saúde, Universidade do Porto, (i3S), 4200-135 Porto, Portugal
| | | | - Patricio Iturriaga-Vásquez
- Molecular Pharmacology and Medicinal Chemistry Lab, Facultad de Ingeniería y Ciencias, Universidad de la Frontera, Temuco, Chile
| | - Xian Wang
- School of Biological and Behavioural Sciences, Queen Mary University of London, London, E1 4NS, UK
| | - Ryan Y. Wong
- Department of Psychology, University of Nebraska at Omaha, Omaha, Nebraska, 68182 USA
- Department of Biology, University of Nebraska at Omaha, Omaha, Nebraska, 68182 USA
| | - Allan V. Kalueff
- Laboratory of experimental models of neuropsychiatric disorders, Scientific Research Institute of Neurosciences and Medicine, Novosibirsk State University, Novosibirsk, Russia
- St. Petersburg State University, St. Petersburg, Russia
- Neurobiology Program, Sirius University of Science and Technology, Sochi, Russia
- Western Caspian University, Baku, Azerbaijan
| | - Matthew O. Parker
- Surrey Sleep Research Centre, Department of Clinical and Experimental Medicine, School of Biosciences, University of Surrey, Guildford, Surrey, GU2 7XH, UK
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Liddle J, Redman B, Frost D, Worthy P, Jamieson P, Wallace SJ. Inclusive research: Making more impact through accessibility and collaboration. Aust Occup Ther J 2024; 71:641-643. [PMID: 39267347 DOI: 10.1111/1440-1630.12990] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/14/2024] [Revised: 08/16/2024] [Accepted: 08/24/2024] [Indexed: 09/17/2024]
Affiliation(s)
- Jacki Liddle
- School of Health and Rehabilitation Sciences, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia
- Princess Alexandra Hospital, Brisbane, Australia
- Associate Editor, Australian Occupational Therapy Journal, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia
| | | | | | - Peter Worthy
- School of Electrical Engineering and Computer Science, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia
| | - Phil Jamieson
- Queensland Aphasia Research Centre, The University of Queensland, Herston, Australia
- Surgical, Treatment and Rehabilitation Service Education and Research Alliance, The University of Queensland and Metro North Hospital and Health Service, Herston, Australia
| | - Sarah J Wallace
- Queensland Aphasia Research Centre, School of Health and Rehabilitation Sciences, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia
- Surgical Treatment and Rehabilitation Service (STARS) Education and Research Alliance, The University of Queensland and Metro North Health, Brisbane, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Jakubovics NS, Schwendicke F. Toward Better Reporting in Oral Health Research. J Dent Res 2024; 103:1045-1046. [PMID: 39305178 DOI: 10.1177/00220345241275459] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/25/2024] Open
Affiliation(s)
- N S Jakubovics
- School of Dental Sciences, Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, Tyne and Wear, UK
| | - F Schwendicke
- Operative Dentistry and Periodontology, LMU Hospital, LMU Munich, Munchen, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Brown DMY, Porter CD, Huong C, Groves CI, Kwan MYW. Predictive Utility of the Multi-Process Action Control Framework for Self-Reported and Device-Measured Physical Activity Behavior of Adolescents. Behav Sci (Basel) 2024; 14:841. [PMID: 39336056 PMCID: PMC11428231 DOI: 10.3390/bs14090841] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/10/2024] [Revised: 09/10/2024] [Accepted: 09/17/2024] [Indexed: 09/30/2024] Open
Abstract
Understanding the correlates of physical activity behavior is imperative for informing the development of interventions to address the low rates of physical activity guideline adherence among adolescents living in the United States. This cross-sectional study examined the predictive utility of the Multi-Process Action Control (M-PAC) framework for explaining self-reported and device-measured physical activity behavior among a Hispanic-majority sample of adolescents. A total of 1849 high school students (mean age = 16.0 ± 1.22 SD years; 52.3% women; 87.8% Hispanic) enrolled in one school district in south-central Texas completed a survey including instruments to assess M-PAC framework constructs (instrumental and affective attitudes, perceived capability and opportunity, behavioral regulation, habit, identity) and moderate-to-vigorous physical activity (MVPA) behavior. A subsample (n = 435) wore accelerometers for seven days. The results from robust linear regression models revealed role identity and habit were significant predictors of self-reported MVPA. Role identity was a significant predictor of accelerometer-derived daily MVPA and raw acceleration in the most active hour but not daily raw acceleration. The findings indicated reflexive processes are robust predictors of adolescent physical activity and should be the focus of interventions designed to promote adoption and maintenance of physical activity during this developmental life stage.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Denver M. Y. Brown
- Department of Kinesiology, Kansas State University, 1105 Sunset Ave, Manhattan, KS 66502, USA;
| | - Carah D. Porter
- Department of Kinesiology, Kansas State University, 1105 Sunset Ave, Manhattan, KS 66502, USA;
| | - Christopher Huong
- Department of Psychology, The University of Texas at San Antonio, 1 UTSA Circle, San Antonio, TX 78249, USA; (C.H.); (C.I.G.)
| | - Claire I. Groves
- Department of Psychology, The University of Texas at San Antonio, 1 UTSA Circle, San Antonio, TX 78249, USA; (C.H.); (C.I.G.)
| | - Matthew Y. W. Kwan
- Department of Child and Youth Studies, Brock University, 1812 Sir Isaac Brock Way, St. Catharines, ON L2S 3A1, Canada;
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Mayrhofer R, Büchner IC, Hevesi J. The quantitative paradigm and the nature of the human mind. The replication crisis as an epistemological crisis of quantitative psychology in view of the ontic nature of the psyche. Front Psychol 2024; 15:1390233. [PMID: 39328812 PMCID: PMC11424412 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1390233] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/22/2024] [Accepted: 08/19/2024] [Indexed: 09/28/2024] Open
Abstract
Many suggestions for dealing with the so-called replication crisis in psychology revolve around the idea that better and more complex statistical-mathematical tools or stricter procedures are required in order to obtain reliable findings and prevent cheating or publication biases. While these aspects may play an exacerbating role, we interpret the replication crisis primarily as an epistemological crisis in psychology caused by an inadequate fit between the ontic nature of the psyche and the quantitative approach. On the basis of the philosophers of science Karl Popper, Thomas Kuhn, and Imre Lakatos we suggest that the replication crisis is therefore a symptom of a fundamental problem in psychology, but at the same time it is also an opportunity to advance psychology as a science. In a first step, against the background of Popper's Critical Rationalism, the replication crisis is interpreted as an opportunity to eliminate inaccurate theories from the pool of theories and to correct problematic developments. Continuing this line of thought, in an interpretation along the lines of Thomas Kuhn, the replication crisis might signify a model drift or even model crisis, thus possibly heralding a new paradigm in psychology. The reasons for this are located in the structure of academic psychology on the basis of Lakatos's assumption about how sciences operate. Accordingly, one hard core that lies at the very basis of psychology may be found in the assumption that the human psyche can and is to be understood in quantitative terms. For this to be possible, the ontic structure of the psyche, i.e., its very nature, must also in some way be quantitatively constituted. Hence, the replication crisis suggests that the ontic structure of the psyche in some way (also) contains a non-quantitative dimension that can only be grasped incompletely or fragmentarily using quantitative research methods. Fluctuating and inconsistent results in psychology could therefore also be the expression of a mismatch between the ontic level of the object of investigation and the epistemic level of the investigation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Roland Mayrhofer
- Department of Psychology, University of Regensburg, Regensburg, Germany
| | - Isabel C Büchner
- Department of Psychology, University of Regensburg, Regensburg, Germany
| | - Judit Hevesi
- Department of Psychology, University of Regensburg, Regensburg, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
A year of growth. COMMUNICATIONS PSYCHOLOGY 2024; 2:76. [PMID: 39242948 PMCID: PMC11335757 DOI: 10.1038/s44271-024-00126-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 09/09/2024]
|
15
|
Stranks J, Heistermann M, Sangmaneedet S, Schülke O, Ostner J. The dynamics of sociality and glucocorticoids in wild male Assamese macaques. Horm Behav 2024; 164:105604. [PMID: 39013354 DOI: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2024.105604] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/27/2023] [Revised: 05/22/2024] [Accepted: 07/09/2024] [Indexed: 07/18/2024]
Abstract
For males of gregarious species, dominance status and the strength of affiliative relationships can have major fitness consequences. Social dynamics also impose costs by affecting glucocorticoids, mediators of homeostasis and indicators of the physiological response to challenges and within-group competition. We investigated the relationships between dominance, social bonds, seasonal challenges, and faecal glucocorticoid metabolite (fGC) measures in wild Assamese macaques (Macaca assamensis) at Phu Khieo Wildlife Sanctuary, Thailand, combining behavioural data with 4129 samples from 62 adult males over 15 years. Our previous work on this population suggested that increased competition during the mating season was associated with elevated fGC levels and that, unusually for male primates, lower rank position correlated with higher fGC levels. With a much larger dataset and dynamic measures of sociality, we re-examined these relationships and additionally tested the potentially fGC-attenuating effect of social support. Contrary to our previous study, yet consistent with the majority of work on male primates, dominance rank had a positive relationship with fGC levels, as high status correlated with elevated glucocorticoid measures. fGC levels were increased at the onset of the mating season. We demonstrated an fGC-reducing effect of supportive relationships in males and showed that dynamics in affiliation can correlate with dynamics in physiological responses. Our results suggest that in a system with intermediate contest potential, high dominance status can impose physiological costs on males that may potentially be moderated by social relationships. We highlight the need to consider the dynamics of sociality and competition that influence hormonal processes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- James Stranks
- Behavioral Ecology Department, University of Goettingen, Goettingen, Germany; Primate Social Evolution Group, German Primate Center, Leibniz Institute for Primate Research, Goettingen, Germany; Leibniz ScienceCampus Primate Cognition, German Primate Center, Leibniz Institute for Primate Research, Goettingen, Germany.
| | - Michael Heistermann
- Endocrinology Laboratory, German Primate Center, Leibniz Institute for Primate Research, Goettingen, Germany
| | - Somboon Sangmaneedet
- Department of Pathobiology, Faculty of Veterinary Medicine, Khon Kaen University, Khon Kaen, Thailand
| | - Oliver Schülke
- Behavioral Ecology Department, University of Goettingen, Goettingen, Germany; Primate Social Evolution Group, German Primate Center, Leibniz Institute for Primate Research, Goettingen, Germany; Leibniz ScienceCampus Primate Cognition, German Primate Center, Leibniz Institute for Primate Research, Goettingen, Germany
| | - Julia Ostner
- Behavioral Ecology Department, University of Goettingen, Goettingen, Germany; Primate Social Evolution Group, German Primate Center, Leibniz Institute for Primate Research, Goettingen, Germany; Leibniz ScienceCampus Primate Cognition, German Primate Center, Leibniz Institute for Primate Research, Goettingen, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Lopez DA, Cardenas-Iniguez C, Subramaniam P, Adise S, Bottenhorn KL, Badilla P, Mukwekwerere E, Tally L, Ahanmisi O, Bedichek IL, Matera SD, Perez-Tamayo GM, Sissons N, Winters O, Harkness A, Nakiyingi E, Encizo J, Xiang Z, Wilson IG, Smith AN, Hill AR, Adames AK, Robertson E, Boughter JR, Lopez-Flores A, Skoler ER, Dorholt L, Nagel BJ, Huber RS. Transparency and reproducibility in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study. Dev Cogn Neurosci 2024; 68:101408. [PMID: 38924835 PMCID: PMC11254940 DOI: 10.1016/j.dcn.2024.101408] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/28/2024] [Revised: 05/27/2024] [Accepted: 06/11/2024] [Indexed: 06/28/2024] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND Transparency can build trust in the scientific process, but scientific findings can be undermined by poor and obscure data use and reporting practices. The purpose of this work is to report how data from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study has been used to date, and to provide practical recommendations on how to improve the transparency and reproducibility of findings. METHODS Articles published from 2017 to 2023 that used ABCD Study data were reviewed using more than 30 data extraction items to gather information on data use practices. Total frequencies were reported for each extraction item, along with computation of a Level of Completeness (LOC) score that represented overall endorsement of extraction items. Univariate linear regression models were used to examine the correlation between LOC scores and individual extraction items. Post hoc analysis included examination of whether LOC scores were correlated with the logged 2-year journal impact factor. RESULTS There were 549 full-length articles included in the main analysis. Analytic scripts were shared in 30 % of full-length articles. The number of participants excluded due to missing data was reported in 60 % of articles, and information on missing data for individual variables (e.g., household income) was provided in 38 % of articles. A table describing the analytic sample was included in 83 % of articles. A race and/or ethnicity variable was included in 78 % of reviewed articles, while its inclusion was justified in only 41 % of these articles. LOC scores were highly correlated with extraction items related to examination of missing data. A bottom 10 % of LOC score was significantly correlated with a lower logged journal impact factor when compared to the top 10 % of LOC scores (β=-0.77, 95 % -1.02, -0.51; p-value < 0.0001). CONCLUSION These findings highlight opportunities for improvement in future papers using ABCD Study data to readily adapt analytic practices for better transparency and reproducibility efforts. A list of recommendations is provided to facilitate adherence in future research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Daniel A Lopez
- Department of Psychiatry, Oregon Health & Science University, Portland, OR, United States; Center for Mental Health Innovation, Oregon Health & Science University, Portland, OR, United States
| | - Carlos Cardenas-Iniguez
- Department of Population and Public Health Sciences, Keck School of Medicine of the University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, United States
| | - Punitha Subramaniam
- Department of Psychiatry, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT, United States
| | - Shana Adise
- Division of Endocrinology, Diabetes and Metabolism, Children's Hospital of Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, United States
| | - Katherine L Bottenhorn
- Department of Population and Public Health Sciences, Keck School of Medicine of the University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, United States
| | - Paola Badilla
- Institute for Behavioral Genetics, University of Colorado, Boulder, CO, United States
| | - Ellen Mukwekwerere
- Department of Psychology, Psychiatry, and Radiology, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO, United States
| | - Laila Tally
- Center for Children and Families and Department of Psychology, Florida International University, Miami, FL, United States
| | - Omoengheme Ahanmisi
- Department of Diagnostic Radiology & Nuclear Medicine, University of Maryland, Baltimore, MD, United States
| | - Isabelle L Bedichek
- Institute for Drug and Alcohol Studies, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA, United States
| | - Serena D Matera
- The Frederick J. and Marion A. Schindler Cognitive Neurophysiology Laboratory Department of Neuroscience and The Ernest J. Del Monte Institute for Neuroscience University of Rochester School of Medicine and Dentistry, Rochester, NY, United States
| | | | - Nicholas Sissons
- Departments of Psychiatry and Radiology, University of Vermont, Burlington, VT, United States
| | - Owen Winters
- Department of Psychiatry, Medical University of South Carolina, Charleston, SC, United States
| | - Anya Harkness
- Center for Health Sciences, SRI International, Menlo Park, CS, United States
| | - Elizabeth Nakiyingi
- Department of Psychology, Psychiatry, and Radiology, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO, United States
| | - Jennell Encizo
- Department of Psychology, Psychiatry, and Radiology, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO, United States
| | - Zhuoran Xiang
- Department of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, CT, United States
| | - Isabelle G Wilson
- Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee, Milwaukee, WI, United States
| | - Allison N Smith
- Department of Psychiatry, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, United States
| | - Anthony R Hill
- Department of Psychiatry, Oregon Health & Science University, Portland, OR, United States
| | - Amanda K Adames
- Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, San Diego, CA, United States
| | - Elizabeth Robertson
- Department of Psychiatry, Medical University of South Carolina, Charleston, SC, United States
| | - Joseph R Boughter
- Department of Psychology, Psychiatry, and Radiology, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO, United States
| | - Arturo Lopez-Flores
- Department of Psychiatry, Oregon Health & Science University, Portland, OR, United States
| | - Emma R Skoler
- Institute for Behavioral Genetics, University of Colorado, Boulder, CO, United States
| | - Lyndsey Dorholt
- Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, United States
| | - Bonnie J Nagel
- Department of Psychiatry, Oregon Health & Science University, Portland, OR, United States; Center for Mental Health Innovation, Oregon Health & Science University, Portland, OR, United States
| | - Rebekah S Huber
- Department of Psychiatry, Oregon Health & Science University, Portland, OR, United States; Department of Psychiatry, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT, United States; Center for Mental Health Innovation, Oregon Health & Science University, Portland, OR, United States.
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Lopez DA, Cardenas-Iniguez C, Subramaniam P, Adise S, Bottenhorn KL, Badilla P, Mukwekwerere E, Tally L, Ahanmisi O, Bedichek IL, Matera SD, Perez-Tamayo GM, Sissons N, Winters O, Harkness A, Nakiyingi E, Encizo J, Xiang Z, Wilson IG, Smith AN, Hill AR, Adames AK, Robertson E, Boughter JR, Lopez-Flores A, Skoler ER, Dorholt L, Nagel BJ, Huber RS. Transparency and Reproducibility in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study. MEDRXIV : THE PREPRINT SERVER FOR HEALTH SCIENCES 2024:2024.05.30.24308222. [PMID: 38854118 PMCID: PMC11160844 DOI: 10.1101/2024.05.30.24308222] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/11/2024]
Abstract
Background Transparency can build trust in the scientific process, but scientific findings can be undermined by poor and obscure data use and reporting practices. The purpose of this work is to report how data from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study has been used to date, and to provide practical recommendations on how to improve the transparency and reproducibility of findings. Methods Articles published from 2017 to 2023 that used ABCD Study data were reviewed using more than 30 data extraction items to gather information on data use practices. Total frequencies were reported for each extraction item, along with computation of a Level of Completeness (LOC) score that represented overall endorsement of extraction items. Univariate linear regression models were used to examine the correlation between LOC scores and individual extraction items. Post hoc analysis included examination of whether LOC scores were correlated with the logged 2-year journal impact factor. Results There were 549 full-length articles included in the main analysis. Analytic scripts were shared in 30% of full-length articles. The number of participants excluded due to missing data was reported in 60% of articles, and information on missing data for individual variables (e.g., household income) was provided in 38% of articles. A table describing the analytic sample was included in 83% of articles. A race and/or ethnicity variable was included in 78% of reviewed articles, while its inclusion was justified in only 41% of these articles. LOC scores were highly correlated with extraction items related to examination of missing data. A bottom 10% of LOC score was significantly correlated with a lower logged journal impact factor when compared to the top 10% of LOC scores (β=-0.77, 95% -1.02, -0.51; p-value < 0.0001). Conclusion These findings highlight opportunities for improvement in future papers using ABCD Study data to readily adapt analytic practices for better transparency and reproducibility efforts. A list of recommendations is provided to facilitate adherence in future research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Daniel A. Lopez
- Department of Psychiatry, Oregon Health & Science University, Portland, Oregon
- Center for Mental Health Innovation, Oregon Health & Science University, Portland, Oregon
| | - Carlos Cardenas-Iniguez
- Department of Population and Public Health Sciences, Keck School of Medicine of the University of Southern California, Los Angeles, California
| | | | - Shana Adise
- Division of Endocrinology, Diabetes and Metabolism, Children’s Hospital of Los Angeles, Los Angeles, California
| | - Katherine L. Bottenhorn
- Department of Population and Public Health Sciences, Keck School of Medicine of the University of Southern California, Los Angeles, California
| | - Paola Badilla
- Institute for Behavioral Genetics, University of Colorado, Boulder, Colorado
| | - Ellen Mukwekwerere
- Department of Psychology, Psychiatry, and Radiology, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, Missouri
| | - Laila Tally
- Center for Children and Families and Department of Psychology, Florida International University, Miami, Florida
| | - Omoengheme Ahanmisi
- Department of Diagnostic Radiology & Nuclear Medicine, University of Maryland, Baltimore, Maryland
| | - Isabelle L. Bedichek
- Institute for Drug and Alcohol Studies, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, Virginia
| | - Serena D. Matera
- The Frederick J. and Marion A. Schindler Cognitive Neurophysiology Laboratory Department of Neuroscience and The Ernest J. Del Monte Institute for Neuroscience University of Rochester School of Medicine and Dentistry, Rochester, New York
| | | | - Nicholas Sissons
- Departments of Psychiatry and Radiology, University of Vermont, Burlington, Vermont
| | - Owen Winters
- Department of Psychiatry, Medical University of South Carolina, Charleston, South Carolina
| | - Anya Harkness
- Center for Health Sciences, SRI International, Menlo Park, California
| | - Elizabeth Nakiyingi
- Department of Psychology, Psychiatry, and Radiology, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, Missouri
| | - Jennell Encizo
- Department of Psychology, Psychiatry, and Radiology, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, Missouri
| | - Zhuoran Xiang
- Department of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, Connecticut
| | - Isabelle G. Wilson
- Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee, Milwaukee, Wisconsin
| | - Allison N. Smith
- Department of Psychiatry, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan
| | - Anthony R. Hill
- Department of Psychiatry, Oregon Health & Science University, Portland, Oregon
| | - Amanda K. Adames
- Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, San Diego, California
| | - Elizabeth Robertson
- Department of Psychiatry, Medical University of South Carolina, Charleston, South Carolina
| | - Joseph R. Boughter
- Department of Psychology, Psychiatry, and Radiology, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, Missouri
| | - Arturo Lopez-Flores
- Department of Psychiatry, Oregon Health & Science University, Portland, Oregon
| | - Emma R. Skoler
- Institute for Behavioral Genetics, University of Colorado, Boulder, Colorado
| | - Lyndsey Dorholt
- Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, Minnesota
| | - Bonnie J. Nagel
- Department of Psychiatry, Oregon Health & Science University, Portland, Oregon
- Center for Mental Health Innovation, Oregon Health & Science University, Portland, Oregon
| | - Rebekah S. Huber
- Department of Psychiatry, Oregon Health & Science University, Portland, Oregon
- Department of Psychiatry, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, Utah
- Center for Mental Health Innovation, Oregon Health & Science University, Portland, Oregon
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Erden YJ. Hyper-ambition and the Replication Crisis: Why Measures to Promote Research Integrity can Falter. JOURNAL OF ACADEMIC ETHICS 2024; 23:25-38. [PMID: 40093653 PMCID: PMC11903532 DOI: 10.1007/s10805-024-09528-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 03/20/2024] [Indexed: 03/19/2025]
Abstract
This paper introduces the concept of 'hyper-ambition' in academia as a contributing factor to what has been termed a 'replication crisis' across some sciences. The replication crisis is an umbrella term that covers a range of 'questionable research practices', from sloppy reporting to fraud. There are already many proposals to address questionable research practices, some of which focus on the values, norms, and motivations of researchers and institutes, and suggest measures to promote research integrity. Yet it is not easy to promote integrity in hyper-competitive academic environments that value high levels of ambition. I argue that in such contexts, it is as likely that a kind of hyper-ambition is fostered that (inadvertently or otherwise) prioritises individual success above all, including to the detriment of scientific quality. In addition, efforts to promote values like integrity falter because they rely on sufficient uniformity in motivations or tendencies. Codes and guidance promoting integrity are, however, likely to influence those for whom such values are not optional, while others simply find ways around them. To demonstrate this I offer a thought experiment in which we consider the imaginary working situations of two ordinary academics. I conclude that tackling questionable research practices in the light of the replication crisis requires robust 'top down' measures that expect and accommodate a broader range of academic values, motivations, and tendencies, while challenging those that help to promote hyper-ambition.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yasemin J. Erden
- Philosophy, Faculty of Behavioural, Management and Social Sciences (BMS), University of Twente, Enschede, Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Wang MY, Zöllner HJ, Yücel MA, Specht K. Editorial: Variability and reproducibility of brain imaging. Front Psychol 2024; 15:1386948. [PMID: 38544520 PMCID: PMC10965770 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1386948] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/16/2024] [Accepted: 03/04/2024] [Indexed: 11/11/2024] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Meng-Yun Wang
- Department of Biological and Medical Psychology, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway
- Mohn Medical Imaging and Visualization Centre (MMIV), Department of Radiology, Haukeland University Hospital, Bergen, Norway
| | - Helge J. Zöllner
- The Russell H. Morgan Department of Radiology and Radiological Science, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, United States
- F.M. Kirby Research Center for Functional Brain Imaging, Kennedy Krieger Institute, Baltimore, MD, United States
| | - Meryem A. Yücel
- Neurophotonics Center, Department of Biomedical Engineering, Boston University, Boston, MA, United States
| | - Karsten Specht
- Department of Biological and Medical Psychology, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway
- Mohn Medical Imaging and Visualization Centre (MMIV), Department of Radiology, Haukeland University Hospital, Bergen, Norway
- Department of Education, UiT The Arctic University of Norway, Tromsø, Norway
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Crombez G, Scott W, De Paepe AL. Knowing What We Are Talking About: The Case of Pain Catastrophizing. THE JOURNAL OF PAIN 2024; 25:591-594. [PMID: 38211846 DOI: 10.1016/j.jpain.2023.12.014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/29/2023] [Accepted: 12/31/2023] [Indexed: 01/13/2024]
Affiliation(s)
- Geert Crombez
- Department of Experimental - Clinical and Health Psychology, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium
| | - Whitney Scott
- Health Psychology Section, Institute of Psychology, Psychiatry, and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, United Kingdom; INPUT Pain Management Unit, Guy's and St Thomas' Hospital NHS Foundation Trust, London, United Kingdom
| | - Annick L De Paepe
- Department of Experimental - Clinical and Health Psychology, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Löscher W. On hidden factors and design-associated errors that may lead to data misinterpretation: An example from preclinical research on the potential seasonality of neonatal seizures. Epilepsia 2024; 65:287-292. [PMID: 38037258 DOI: 10.1111/epi.17840] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/26/2023] [Revised: 11/20/2023] [Accepted: 11/28/2023] [Indexed: 12/02/2023]
Abstract
Unintentional misinterpretation of research in published biomedical reports that is not based on statistical flaws is often underrecognized, despite its possible impact on science, clinical practice, and public health. Important causes of such misinterpretation of scientific data, resulting in either false positive or false negative conclusions, include design-associated errors and hidden (or latent) variables that are not easily recognized during data analysis. Furthermore, cognitive biases, such as the inclination to seek patterns in data whether they exist or not, may lead to misinterpretation of data. Here, we give an example of these problems from hypothesis-driven research on the potential seasonality of neonatal seizures in a rat model of birth asphyxia. This commentary aims to raise awareness among the general scientific audience about the issues related to the presence of unintentional misinterpretation in published reports.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Wolfgang Löscher
- Translational Neuropharmacology Lab, NIFE, Department of Experimental Otology of the ENT Clinics, Hannover Medical School, Hannover, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Kreamer LM, Cobb HR, Castille C, Cogswell J. Big team science initiatives: A catalyst for trustworthy advancements in IO psychology. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2024; 242:104101. [PMID: 38064907 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2023.104101] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/30/2023] [Revised: 11/29/2023] [Accepted: 11/30/2023] [Indexed: 01/16/2024] Open
Abstract
Keener et al. (2023) raise concerns about the trustworthiness of Industrial/Organizational (IO) Psychology research and related fields due to the low reproducibility and replicability of research findings. The authors provide various solutions to resolve this crisis, such as improving training, realigning incentives, and adopting open science practices. Our commentary elaborates on one solution to which they briefly allude: Big Team Science Initiatives (BTSIs). BTSIs allow scholars to address the trustworthiness of our science by facilitating large sample theory testing, sharing and allocating resources, and selecting appropriate research strategies, all of which support the reproducibility and replication of research. Further, we propose that BTSIs may facilitate researcher training, encourage data sharing and materials, and realign incentives in our field. We discuss how BTSIs could be implemented in IO psychology and related fields, identifying and drawing upon similar BTSIs in related disciplines. Thus, our commentary is an extension of the focal article, encouraging scholars to collaboratively address the "crisis of confidence" facing our field using a big team science approach.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Haley R Cobb
- Louisiana State University, United States of America
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
23
|
König LM, Krukowski RA, Kuntsche E, Busse H, Gumbert L, Gemesi K, Neter E, Mohamed NF, Ross KM, John-Akinola YO, Cooper R, Allmeta A, Silva AM, Forbes CC, Western MJ. Reducing intervention- and research-induced inequalities to tackle the digital divide in health promotion. Int J Equity Health 2023; 22:249. [PMID: 38049789 PMCID: PMC10696856 DOI: 10.1186/s12939-023-02055-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/01/2023] [Accepted: 11/06/2023] [Indexed: 12/06/2023] Open
Abstract
Social inequalities are an important contributor to the global burden of disease within and between countries. Using digital technology in health promotion and healthcare is seen by some as a potential lever to reduce these inequalities; however, research suggests that digital technology risks re-enacting or evening widening disparities. Most research on this digital health divide focuses on a small number of social inequality indicators and stems from Western, educated, industrialized, rich, and democratic (WEIRD) countries. There is a need for systematic, international, and interdisciplinary contextualized research on the impact of social inequality indicators in digital health as well as the underlying mechanisms of this digital divide across the globe to reduce health disparities. In June 2023, eighteen multi-disciplinary researchers representing thirteen countries from six continents came together to discuss current issues in the field of digital health promotion and healthcare contributing to the digital divide. Ways that current practices in research contribute to the digital health divide were explored, including intervention development, testing, and implementation. Based on the dialogue, we provide suggestions for overcoming barriers and improving practices across disciplines, countries, and sectors. The research community must actively advocate for system-level changes regarding policy and research to reduce the digital divide and so improve digital health for all.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Laura M König
- Faculty of Life Sciences: Food, Nutrition and Health, University of Bayreuth, Bayreuth, Germany.
- Faculty of Psychology, University of Vienna, Wächtergasse 1/504, 1010, Vienna, Austria.
| | - Rebecca A Krukowski
- Department of Public Health Sciences, School of Medicine, University of Virginia, Charlottesville, USA
| | - Emmanuel Kuntsche
- Centre for Alcohol Policy Research, La Trobe University, Melbourne, Australia
| | - Heide Busse
- Leibniz Institute for Prevention Research and Epidemiology - BIPS, Bremen, Germany
- Leibniz Science Campus Digital Public Health Bremen, Bremen, Germany
| | | | - Kathrin Gemesi
- Institute for Nutritional Medicine, School of Medicine and Health, Technical University of Munich, Munich, Germany
| | | | - Nor Firdous Mohamed
- Department of Psychology, Faculty of Human Development, University Pendidikan Sultan Idris, Tanjong Malim, Malaysia
| | | | - Yetunde O John-Akinola
- Department of Health Promotion and Education, Faculty of Public Health, College of Medicine, University of Ibadan, Ibadan, Nigeria
| | - Rosie Cooper
- Nuffield Department of Primary Care Health Sciences, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
| | - Anila Allmeta
- Faculty of Life Sciences: Food, Nutrition and Health, University of Bayreuth, Bayreuth, Germany
| | - Anabelle Macedo Silva
- Instituto de Saúde Coletiva, Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro, Brazil
| | - Cynthia C Forbes
- Wolfson Palliative Care Research Centre, Hull York Medical School, University of Hull, Hull, UK
| | - Max J Western
- Centre for Motivation and Health Behaviour Change, Department for Health, University of Bath, Bath, UK
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Psychology looking forward. COMMUNICATIONS PSYCHOLOGY 2023; 1:4. [PMID: 39242683 PMCID: PMC11290599 DOI: 10.1038/s44271-023-00004-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 09/09/2024]
|