1
|
Moriguchi Y, Watanabe R, Sakata C, Zeleznikow-Johnston A, Wang J, Saji N, Tsuchiya N. Comparing color qualia structures through a similarity task in young children versus adults. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2025; 122:e2415346122. [PMID: 40067901 PMCID: PMC11929397 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.2415346122] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/04/2024] [Accepted: 02/10/2025] [Indexed: 03/25/2025] Open
Abstract
Examination of the subjective qualitative aspects of an experience, or "qualia" in short, is a fundamental and core aspect of consciousness research. How can we characterize the particular quality of redness, i.e. a red quale? Based on a recent proposal of the structural characterization of qualia, which did not rely on verbal descriptions, we developed a task that obtained pairwise similarity judgments at four graded levels, with easy and intuitive visual interfaces designed to engage young children. We examined color qualia structures in children (3 to 12-y-old in Japan and 6 to 8-y-old in China) and compared these with those of Japanese adults. Approximately half of the assessed 3-y-old children completed the experiment via our touch panel device version of the task and had reliable responses. Despite known developmental and/or cultural effects of color term usage, we found that color qualia structures were quite similar across the age groups and cultures. Our finding supports the view that color qualia structures emerge early. We also observed age-related differences in the evaluations of some color pairs, which implied subtle changes in the structures behind color experience.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yusuke Moriguchi
- Graduation School of Letters, Kyoto University, Kyoto6068501, Japan
| | - Ryoichi Watanabe
- Graduation School of Letters, Kyoto University, Kyoto6068501, Japan
| | - Chifumi Sakata
- Japan Society for the Promotion of Science, Tokyo1020083, Japan
- Department of Cognitive Science, Central European University, Wien1100, Austria
| | - Ariel Zeleznikow-Johnston
- School of Psychological Sciences, Faculty of Medicine, Nursing and Health Science, Monash University, Victoria3800, Australia
- Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, Faculty of Medicine, Nursing and Health Science, Monash University, Victoria3800, Australia
| | - Jue Wang
- Graduation School of Letters, Kyoto University, Kyoto6068501, Japan
| | - Noburo Saji
- School of Human Sciences, Waseda University, Tokorozawa3591192, Japan
| | - Naotsugu Tsuchiya
- School of Psychological Sciences, Faculty of Medicine, Nursing and Health Science, Monash University, Victoria3800, Australia
- Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, Faculty of Medicine, Nursing and Health Science, Monash University, Victoria3800, Australia
- Center for Information and Neural Networks, National Institute of Information and Communications Technology, Osaka5650871, Japan
- Advanced Telecommunications Research Computational Neuroscience Laboratories, Kyoto6190288, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Virtanen LS, Saarela TP, Olkkonen M. Ensemble percepts of colored targets among distractors are influenced by hue similarity, not categorical identity. J Vis 2024; 24:12. [PMID: 39412766 PMCID: PMC11498646 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.11.12] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/27/2024] [Accepted: 08/26/2024] [Indexed: 10/25/2024] Open
Abstract
Color can be used to group similar elements, and ensemble percepts of color can be formed for such groups. In real-life settings, however, elements of similar color are often spatially interspersed among other elements and seen against a background. Forming an ensemble percept of these elements would require the segmentation of the correct color signals for integration. Can the human visual system do this? We examined whether observers can extract the ensemble mean hue from a target hue distribution among distractors and whether a color category boundary between target and distractor hues facilitates ensemble hue formation. Observers were able to selectively judge the target ensemble mean hue, but the presence of distractor hues added noise to the ensemble estimates and caused perceptual biases. The more similar the distractor hues were to the target hues, the noisier the estimates became, possibly reflecting incomplete or inaccurate segmentation of the two hue ensembles. Asymmetries between nominally equidistant distractors and substantial individual variability, however, point to additional factors beyond simple mixing of target and distractor distributions. Finally, we found no evidence for categorical facilitation in selective ensemble hue formation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lari S Virtanen
- Department of Psychology, Faculty of Medicine, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland
| | - Toni P Saarela
- Department of Psychology, Faculty of Medicine, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland
| | - Maria Olkkonen
- Department of Psychology, Faculty of Medicine, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Kaicher CM, Conti JJ, Dedhe AM, Aulet LS, Cantlon JF. Is core knowledge a natural subdivision of infant cognition? Behav Brain Sci 2024; 47:e133. [PMID: 38934427 DOI: 10.1017/s0140525x23003229] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/28/2024]
Abstract
We examine Spelke's core knowledge taxonomy and test its boundaries. We ask whether Spelke's core knowledge is a distinct type of cognition in the sense that the cognitive processes it includes and excludes are biologically and mechanically coherent.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Caroline M Kaicher
- Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, USA ; ; ; ; https://www.cmu.edu/dietrich/psychology/kidneurolab/
| | - Julia J Conti
- Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, USA ; ; ; ; https://www.cmu.edu/dietrich/psychology/kidneurolab/
| | - Abhishek M Dedhe
- Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, USA ; ; ; ; https://www.cmu.edu/dietrich/psychology/kidneurolab/
| | - Lauren S Aulet
- Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, USA ; ; ; ; https://www.cmu.edu/dietrich/psychology/kidneurolab/
| | - Jessica F Cantlon
- Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, USA ; ; ; ; https://www.cmu.edu/dietrich/psychology/kidneurolab/
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Mihajlović N, Zdravković S. Contingent capture by color is sensitive to categorical color perception. Atten Percept Psychophys 2024; 86:36-48. [PMID: 37985593 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-023-02806-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 10/05/2023] [Indexed: 11/22/2023]
Abstract
Contingent capture (CC) theory postulates that attention can only be captured by top-down matching stimuli. Although the contingent capture of attention is a well-known and thoroughly studied phenomenon, there is still no consensus on the characteristics of the top-down template which guides the search for colors. We tried to replicate the classical contingent capture effect on color (Experiment 1) and then added linguistic processing to this perceptual effect (Experiment 2). In Experiment 1, attention was indeed captured by the cues of the same color as the target, while the cues of different colors were successfully ignored. In Experiment 2, the cue color was never identical to the target color but would either belong to the same linguistic category or not (i.e., linguistic matching and linguistic nonmatching cues). In both cases, cues were made to be equally perceptually distant from the target. Although, attention was captured by both cue types, the degree of capture was significantly higher for linguistic matching cues. Our research replicated the classic contingent capture effect but on color, and also demonstrated the effect of color categories in the search task. In short, we demonstrated the effect of color categories in the search task. Results show that the template for color search contains physical characteristics of color, as well as information about color category names.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nataša Mihajlović
- Laboratory for Experimental Psychology, Department of Psychology, Faculty of Philosophy, University of Novi Sad, Novi Sad, Serbia.
| | - Sunčica Zdravković
- Laboratory for Experimental Psychology, Department of Psychology, Faculty of Philosophy, University of Novi Sad, Novi Sad, Serbia
- Laboratory for Experimental Psychology, Department of Psychology, Faculty of Philosophy, University of Belgrade, Belgrade, Serbia
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Forbes SH, Plunkett K. Colour perception changes with basic colour word comprehension. Dev Sci 2023; 26:e13406. [PMID: 37127947 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13406] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/06/2022] [Revised: 04/11/2023] [Accepted: 04/19/2023] [Indexed: 05/03/2023]
Abstract
Recent work has investigated the origin of infant colour categories, showing pre-linguistic infants categorise colour even in the absence of colour words. These infant categories are similar but not identical to adult categories, giving rise to an important question about how infant colour perception changes with the learning of colour words. Here we present two novel paradigms in which 12- and 19-month-old participants learning English as their first language were assessed on their perception of colour, while data on their colour word comprehension were also collected. Results indicate that participants' perception of colours close to the colour category boundaries dramatically change after colour word learning. The results highlight the shift made from infant colour categories to adult-like linguistically mediated colour categories that accompanies colour word learning. RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS: We aimed to test whether colour perception is linguistically mediated in infants. We used novel eye-tracking and pupillometry paradigms to test infant colour perception either side of learning colour words. Infants' discrimination of colour changes after learning colour words, suggesting a shift due to colour word learning. A shift from pre-linguistic colour representation to linguistically mediated colour representation is discussed.
Collapse
|
6
|
Abstract
Color is a pervasive feature of our psychological experience, having a role in many aspects of human mind and behavior such as basic vision, scene perception, object recognition, aesthetics, and communication. Understanding how humans encode, perceive, talk about, and use color has been a major interdisciplinary effort. Here, we present the current state of knowledge on how color perception and cognition develop. We cover the development of various aspects of the psychological experience of color, ranging from low-level color vision to perceptual mechanisms such as color constancy to phenomena such as color naming and color preference. We also identify neurodiversity in the development of color perception and cognition and implications for clinical and educational contexts. We discuss the theoretical implications of the research for understanding mature color perception and cognition, for identifying the principles of perceptual and cognitive development, and for fostering a broader debate in the psychological sciences.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- John Maule
- The Sussex Colour Group & Baby Lab, School of Psychology, University of Sussex, Falmer, United Kingdom;
| | - Alice E Skelton
- The Sussex Colour Group & Baby Lab, School of Psychology, University of Sussex, Falmer, United Kingdom;
| | - Anna Franklin
- The Sussex Colour Group & Baby Lab, School of Psychology, University of Sussex, Falmer, United Kingdom;
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Abstract
Categorical perception (CP) of color claims that colors from different linguistic categories are discriminated more easily than those from the same category, suggesting that language may interact with visual perception. However, controversy remains regarding whether CP effects derive from language. Recently, CP effects were found in a dynamic paradigm named multiple object tracking (MOT). Here, we investigated whether this kind of CP is dependent on online use of language. We found that the CP effects are reduced by verbal interference when the participants were required to memorize color words during tracking (Experiment 2) but not when the interference stimuli were digits (Experiment 1). Our study suggested that the CP effects in tracking depend partly on online use of language and that the difficulty of verbal interference tasks influences the capability of disrupting CP.
Collapse
|
8
|
Roy S, Banerjee A, Roy C, Nag S, Sanyal S, Sengupta R, Ghosh D. Brain response to color stimuli: an EEG study with nonlinear approach. Cogn Neurodyn 2021; 15:1023-1053. [PMID: 34790269 DOI: 10.1007/s11571-021-09692-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/12/2020] [Revised: 05/22/2021] [Accepted: 06/11/2021] [Indexed: 11/26/2022] Open
Abstract
Color perception is a major guiding factor in the evolutionary process of human civilization, but most of the neurological background of the same are yet unknown. This work attempts to address this area with an EEG based neuro-cognitive study on response of brain to different color stimuli. With respect to a Grey baseline seven colors of the VIBGYOR were shown to 16 participants with normal color vision and corresponding EEG signals from different lobes (Frontal, Occipital & Parietal) were recorded. In an attempt to quantify the brain response while watching these colors, the corresponding EEG signals were analysed using two of the latest state of the art non-linear techniques (MFDFA and MFDXA) of dealing complex time series. MFDFA revealed that for all the participants the spectral width, and hence the complexity of the EEG signals, reaches a maximum while viewing color Blue, followed by colors Red and Green in all the brain lobes. MFDXA, on the other hand, suggests a lower degree of inter and intra lobe correlation while watching the VIBGYOR colors compared to baseline Grey, hinting towards a post processing of visual information. We hope that along with the novelty of methodologies, the unique outcomes of this study may leave a long term impact in the domain of color perception research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Souparno Roy
- Department of Physics, Jadavpur University, Kolkata, India
- Sir C.V. Raman Centre for Physics and Music, Jadavpur University, Kolkata, India
| | - Archi Banerjee
- Sir C.V. Raman Centre for Physics and Music, Jadavpur University, Kolkata, India
- Rekhi Centre of Excellence for the Science of Happiness, IIT Kharagpur, Kharagpur, India
| | - Chandrima Roy
- Sir C.V. Raman Centre for Physics and Music, Jadavpur University, Kolkata, India
- Department of Electronics & Communication Engineering, Heritage Institute of Technology, Kolkata, India
| | - Sayan Nag
- Department of Medical Biophysics, University of Toronto, Toronto, Canada
| | - Shankha Sanyal
- Sir C.V. Raman Centre for Physics and Music, Jadavpur University, Kolkata, India
- School of Languages and Linguistics, Jadavpur University, Kolkata, India
| | - Ranjan Sengupta
- Sir C.V. Raman Centre for Physics and Music, Jadavpur University, Kolkata, India
| | - Dipak Ghosh
- Sir C.V. Raman Centre for Physics and Music, Jadavpur University, Kolkata, India
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Abstract
Color is a continuous variable, and humans can distinguish more than a million colors, yet world color lexicons contain no more than a dozen basic color terms. It has been understood for 160 years that the number of color terms in a lexicon varies greatly across languages, yet the lexical color categories defined by these terms are similar worldwide. Starting with the seminal study by Berlin and Kay, this review considers how and why this is so. Evidence from psychological, linguistic, and computational studies has advanced our understanding of how color categories came into being, how they contribute to our shared understanding of color, and how the resultant categories influence color perception and cognition. A key insight from the last 50 years of research is how human perception and the need for communication within a society worked together to create color lexicons that are somewhat diverse, yet show striking regularities worldwide.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Delwin T Lindsey
- Department of Psychology, Ohio State University, Mansfield, Ohio 44906, USA.,College of Optometry, Ohio State University, Columbus, Ohio 43210, USA
| | - Angela M Brown
- College of Optometry, Ohio State University, Columbus, Ohio 43210, USA
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Langus A, Höhle B. Object individuation and labelling in 6-month-old infants. Infant Behav Dev 2021; 65:101627. [PMID: 34438253 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2021.101627] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/14/2020] [Revised: 08/03/2021] [Accepted: 08/04/2021] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
Abstract
The ability to determine how many objects are involved in physical events is fundamental for reasoning about the world that surrounds us. Previous studies suggest that infants can fail to individuate objects in ambiguous occlusion events until their first birthday and that learning words for the objects may play a crucial role in the development of this ability. The present eye-tracking study tested whether the classical object individuation experiments underestimate young infants' ability to individuate objects and the role word learning plays in this process. Three groups of 6-month-old infants (N = 72) saw two opaque boxes side by side on the eye-tracker screen so that the content of the boxes was not visible. During a familiarization phase, two visually identical objects emerged sequentially from one box and two visually different objects from the other box. For one group of infants the familiarization was silent (Visual Only condition). For a second group of infants the objects were accompanied with nonsense words so that objects' shape and linguistic labels indicated the same number of objects in the two boxes (Visual & Language condition). For the third group of infants, objects' shape and linguistic labels were in conflict (Visual vs. Language condition). Following the familiarization, it was revealed that both boxes contained the same number of objects (e.g. one or two). In the Visual Only condition, infants looked longer to the box with incorrect number of objects at test, showing that they could individuate objects using visual cues alone. In the Visual & Language condition infants showed the same looking pattern. However, in the Visual vs Language condition infants looked longer to the box with incorrect number of objects according to linguistic labels. The results show that infants can individuate objects in a complex object individuation paradigm considerably earlier than previously thought and that linguistic cues enforce their own preference in object individuation. The results are consistent with the idea that when language and visual information are in conflict, language can exert an influence on how young infants reason about the visual world.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alan Langus
- University of Potsdam, Cognitive Sciences, Department of Linguistics, Karl-Liebknecht-Str. 24-25, D-14476 Potsdam, Germany.
| | - Barbara Höhle
- University of Potsdam, Cognitive Sciences, Department of Linguistics, Karl-Liebknecht-Str. 24-25, D-14476 Potsdam, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Cathodal tDCS stimulation of left anterior temporal lobe eliminates cross-category color discrimination response time advantage. Behav Brain Res 2020; 391:112682. [DOI: 10.1016/j.bbr.2020.112682] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/27/2020] [Revised: 04/24/2020] [Accepted: 04/27/2020] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
|
12
|
Caves EM, Nowicki S, Johnsen S. Von Uexküll Revisited: Addressing Human Biases in the Study of Animal Perception. Integr Comp Biol 2020; 59:1451-1462. [PMID: 31127268 DOI: 10.1093/icb/icz073] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/02/2023] Open
Abstract
More than 100 years ago, the biologist Jakob von Uexküll suggested that, because sensory systems are diverse, animals likely inhabit different sensory worlds (umwelten) than we do. Since von Uexküll, work across sensory modalities has confirmed that animals sometimes perceive sensory information that humans cannot, and it is now well-established that one must account for this fact when studying an animal's behavior. We are less adept, however, at recognizing cases in which non-human animals may not detect or perceive stimuli the same way we do, which is our focus here. In particular, we discuss three ways in which our own perception can result in misinformed hypotheses about the function of various stimuli. In particular, we may (1) make untested assumptions about how sensory information is perceived, based on how we perceive or measure it, (2) attribute undue significance to stimuli that we perceive as complex or striking, and (3) assume that animals divide the sensory world in the same way that we as scientists do. We discuss each of these biases and provide examples of cases where animals cannot perceive or are not attending to stimuli in the same way that we do, and how this may lead us to mistaken assumptions. Because what an animal perceives affects its behavior, we argue that these biases are especially important for researchers in sensory ecology, cognition, and animal behavior and communication to consider. We suggest that studying animal umwelten requires integrative approaches that combine knowledge of sensory physiology with behavioral assays.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Sönke Johnsen
- Biology Department, Duke University, Durham, NC, USA
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Siuda-Krzywicka K, Boros M, Bartolomeo P, Witzel C. The biological bases of colour categorisation: From goldfish to the human brain. Cortex 2019; 118:82-106. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.04.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/07/2018] [Revised: 12/03/2018] [Accepted: 04/12/2019] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
|
14
|
Zipple MN, Caves EM, Green PA, Peters S, Johnsen S, Nowicki S. Categorical colour perception occurs in both signalling and non-signalling colour ranges in a songbird. Proc Biol Sci 2019; 286:20190524. [PMID: 31138066 DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2019.0524] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Although perception begins when a stimulus is transduced by a sensory neuron, numerous perceptual mechanisms can modify sensory information as it is processed by an animal's nervous system. One such mechanism is categorical perception, in which (1) continuously varying stimuli are labelled as belonging to a discrete number of categories and (2) there is enhanced discrimination between stimuli from different categories as compared with equally different stimuli from within the same category. We have shown previously that female zebra finches ( Taeniopygia guttata) categorically perceive colours along an orange-red continuum that aligns with the carotenoid-based coloration of male beaks, a trait that serves as an assessment signal in female mate choice. Here, we demonstrate that categorical perception occurs along a blue-green continuum as well, suggesting that categorical colour perception may be a general feature of zebra finch vision. Although we identified two categories in both the blue-green and the orange-red ranges, we also found that individuals could better differentiate colours from within the same category in the blue-green as compared with the orange-red range, indicative of less clear categorization in the blue-green range. We discuss reasons why categorical perception may vary across the visible spectrum, including the possibility that such differences are linked to the behavioural or ecological function of different colour ranges.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | | | - Susan Peters
- Biology Department, Duke University , Durham NC , USA
| | - Sönke Johnsen
- Biology Department, Duke University , Durham NC , USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
15
|
Scott KM. Split-Brain Babies? Differences in Representation of Bilaterally and Unilaterally Presented Visual Stimuli in Infancy. Front Psychol 2019; 9:2758. [PMID: 30778325 PMCID: PMC6369161 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02758] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/21/2018] [Accepted: 12/20/2018] [Indexed: 11/17/2022] Open
Abstract
Information needed for perception and action is often distributed across the two hemispheres of the human brain. During development, representations lateralized due to topographic sensory maps may be available independently before they can be integrated across hemispheres. These studies (total N = 211) investigate visual interhemispheric integration in two domains in infancy. In Experiment 1, infants (8-14 months) showed stronger evidence of representing the equality of two shapes when the shapes were presented in the same visual hemifield. In Experiments 2-4, infants (10-19 months) showed evidence of greater familiarization when shown 16 dots in one hemifield than when shown 8 dots in each hemifield. The possibility that interhemispheric integration poses an unusually late-resolved challenge in infant vision is discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kimberly M. Scott
- Early Childhood Cognition Lab, Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, United States
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Categorical perception of color can be instantly influenced by color vision fatigue and semantic satiation. ACTA PSYCHOLOGICA SINICA 2019. [DOI: 10.3724/sp.j.1041.2019.00196] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
|
17
|
Sun VC, Chen CC. Basic color categories and Mandarin Chinese color terms. PLoS One 2018; 13:e0206699. [PMID: 30485289 PMCID: PMC6261562 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0206699] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/18/2018] [Accepted: 10/17/2018] [Indexed: 11/28/2022] Open
Abstract
Basic color terms used in Mandarin Chinese have been controversial since first discussed by Berlin and Kay in 1969. Previous studies showed much inconsistency on what should be considered as basic color terms in Mandarin Chinese. In the present study, we investigated categories of color rather than merely the color terms used by Taiwanese native Mandarin speakers. Using samples conforming to the Berlin and Kay survey, various colors were chosen from a collection of Natural Color System (NCS) colored papers and mounted on a piece of neutral gray card. The card was then mounted on a touch-screen, under D65 illumination. Thirty-two single-character color related Mandarin terms were selected from a Chinese character database according to frequency of use. Participants were required to select the color sample that matched the term by pressing a virtual button on the touch screen. The results show that certain terms can be directly correlated to basic color terms in English, comparable with the results of Berlin and Kay’s original study and those that followed. However, some terms, such as Mo (墨 ink), Tie (鐵 iron), and Cai (菜vegetable), show a wide spread of term maps and inconsistent use among subjects. Principle component analysis (PCA) procedures were used to analysis the commodity of data among subjects. The findings suggest that the basic color categories among Mandarin Chinese speakers are similar to those found in the World Color Survey (WCS), but are represented by wide-spread and inconsistent color terms among speakers.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Vincent C. Sun
- Department of Mass Communication, Chinese Culture University, Taipei, Taiwan
| | - Chien-Chung Chen
- Department of Psychology, National Taiwan University, Taipei, Taiwan
- * E-mail:
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Majid A, Roberts SG, Cilissen L, Emmorey K, Nicodemus B, O'Grady L, Woll B, LeLan B, de Sousa H, Cansler BL, Shayan S, de Vos C, Senft G, Enfield NJ, Razak RA, Fedden S, Tufvesson S, Dingemanse M, Ozturk O, Brown P, Hill C, Le Guen O, Hirtzel V, van Gijn R, Sicoli MA, Levinson SC. Differential coding of perception in the world's languages. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2018; 115:11369-11376. [PMID: 30397135 PMCID: PMC6233065 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1720419115] [Citation(s) in RCA: 79] [Impact Index Per Article: 11.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Is there a universal hierarchy of the senses, such that some senses (e.g., vision) are more accessible to consciousness and linguistic description than others (e.g., smell)? The long-standing presumption in Western thought has been that vision and audition are more objective than the other senses, serving as the basis of knowledge and understanding, whereas touch, taste, and smell are crude and of little value. This predicts that humans ought to be better at communicating about sight and hearing than the other senses, and decades of work based on English and related languages certainly suggests this is true. However, how well does this reflect the diversity of languages and communities worldwide? To test whether there is a universal hierarchy of the senses, stimuli from the five basic senses were used to elicit descriptions in 20 diverse languages, including 3 unrelated sign languages. We found that languages differ fundamentally in which sensory domains they linguistically code systematically, and how they do so. The tendency for better coding in some domains can be explained in part by cultural preoccupations. Although languages seem free to elaborate specific sensory domains, some general tendencies emerge: for example, with some exceptions, smell is poorly coded. The surprise is that, despite the gradual phylogenetic accumulation of the senses, and the imbalances in the neural tissue dedicated to them, no single hierarchy of the senses imposes itself upon language.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Asifa Majid
- Language & Cognition Department, Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, 6525 XD Nijmegen, The Netherlands;
- Centre for Language Studies, Radboud University, 6525 HP Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | - Seán G Roberts
- Department of Archaeology and Anthropology, University of Bristol, Bristol BS8 1TH, United Kingdom
| | - Ludy Cilissen
- Language & Cognition Department, Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, 6525 XD Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | - Karen Emmorey
- School of Speech, Language, and Hearing Sciences, San Diego State University, San Diego, CA 92182
| | - Brenda Nicodemus
- Department of Interpretation & Translation, Gallaudet University, Washington, DC 20002
| | - Lucinda O'Grady
- School of Speech, Language, and Hearing Sciences, San Diego State University, San Diego, CA 92182
| | - Bencie Woll
- Deafness Cognition and Language Research Centre, University College London, London WC1E 6BT, United Kingdom
| | - Barbara LeLan
- English Studies, Université Paris IV-Sorbonne, 75005 Paris, France
| | - Hilário de Sousa
- Language & Cognition Department, Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, 6525 XD Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | - Brian L Cansler
- Department of Linguistics, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, NC 27514
| | - Shakila Shayan
- Education & Pedagogy, Utrecht University, 3512 JE Utrecht, The Netherlands
| | - Connie de Vos
- Language & Cognition Department, Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, 6525 XD Nijmegen, The Netherlands
- Centre for Language Studies, Radboud University, 6525 HP Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | - Gunter Senft
- Language & Cognition Department, Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, 6525 XD Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | - N J Enfield
- Department of Linguistics, The University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW 2006, Australia
| | - Rogayah A Razak
- Centre for Rehabilitation and Specials Needs, Universiti Kebangsaan Malaysia, 43600 Bangi, Selangor, Malaysia
| | - Sebastian Fedden
- Institute of General and Applied Linguistics and Phonetics, Université Paris 3 (Sorbonne-Nouvelle), 75005 Paris, France
| | - Sylvia Tufvesson
- Language & Cognition Department, Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, 6525 XD Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | - Mark Dingemanse
- Language & Cognition Department, Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, 6525 XD Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | - Ozge Ozturk
- Theoretical and Applied Linguistics, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 1TN, United Kingdom
| | - Penelope Brown
- Language & Cognition Department, Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, 6525 XD Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | - Clair Hill
- Language & Cognition Department, Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, 6525 XD Nijmegen, The Netherlands
- Department of Linguistics, The University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW 2006, Australia
- Centre for Languages and Literature, Lund University, SE-221 00 Lund, Sweden
| | - Olivier Le Guen
- Linguistics Department, Centro de Investigación y Estudios Superiores en Antropología Social, 14000 Mexico City, Mexico
| | - Vincent Hirtzel
- Laboratory of Ethnology and Comparative Sociology, CNRS/Paris Nanterre University, 92000 Nanterre, France
| | - Rik van Gijn
- Center for Linguistics, University of Zurich, 8006 Zurich, Switzerland
| | - Mark A Sicoli
- Department of Anthropology, University of Virginia, Charlottesville, VA 22904
| | - Stephen C Levinson
- Language & Cognition Department, Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, 6525 XD Nijmegen, The Netherlands;
- Centre for Language Studies, Radboud University, 6525 HP Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Behavioral and ERP study of color categorical perception in proficient and nonproficient bilinguals. ACTA PSYCHOLOGICA SINICA 2018. [DOI: 10.3724/sp.j.1041.2018.01259] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
|
20
|
Forbes SH, Plunkett K. Linguistic and Cultural Variation in Early Color Word Learning. Child Dev 2018; 91:28-42. [PMID: 30315727 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.13164] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
When and how do infants learn color words? It is generally supposed that color words are learned late and with a great deal of difficulty. By examining infant language surveys in British English and 11 other languages, this study shows that color word learning occurs earlier than has been previously suggested and that the order of acquisition of color words is similar in related languages. This study also demonstrates that frequency and syllabic complexity can be used to predict variability in infant color word learning across languages. In light of recent evidence indicating that color categories have universal biological foundations, these findings suggest that infants' experience and linguistic exposure drive their shift to culturally and linguistically mediated adult-like understandings of color words.
Collapse
|
21
|
Abstract
Color has been scientifically investigated by linking color appearance to colorimetric measurements of the light that enters the eye. However, the main purpose of color perception is not to determine the properties of incident light, but to aid the visual perception of objects and materials in our environment. We review the state of the art on object colors, color constancy, and color categories to gain insight into the functional aspects of color perception. The common ground between these areas of research is that color appearance is tightly linked to the identification of objects and materials and the communication across observers. In conclusion, we argue that research should focus on how color processing is adapted to the surface properties of objects in the natural environment in order to bridge the gap between the known early stages of color perception and the subjective appearance of color.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Christoph Witzel
- Department of Psychology, University of Giessen, 35394 Giessen, Germany;,
| | | |
Collapse
|
22
|
Costa M, Frumento S, Nese M, Predieri I. Interior Color and Psychological Functioning in a University Residence Hall. Front Psychol 2018; 9:1580. [PMID: 30210407 PMCID: PMC6120989 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01580] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/06/2018] [Accepted: 08/08/2018] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
The research exploited a unique architectural setting of a university residence hall composed by six separate buildings that matched for every architectural detail and differed only for the interior color (violet, blue, green, yellow, orange, and red). Four hundred and forty-three students living in the six buildings for an average of 13.33 months participated in a study that assessed color preference (hue and lightness), lightness preference, and the effects of color on studying and mood. The results showed a preference for blue interiors, followed by green, violet, orange, yellow, and red. A preference bias was found for the specific color in which the student lived. Gender differences emerged for the preference of blue and violet. Room-lightness was significantly affected by the interior color. Room ceiling was preferred white. Blue as interior color was considered to facilitate studying activity. The use of differentiated colors in the six buildings was evaluated to significantly facilitate orienting and wayfinding. A significant relation was found between a calm mood and preference for blue.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marco Costa
- Department of Psychology, University of Bologna, Bologna, Italy
| | - Sergio Frumento
- Department of Surgery, Medical, Molecular, and Critical Area Pathology, University of Pisa, Pisa, Italy
| | - Mattia Nese
- Department of Psychology, University of Bologna, Bologna, Italy
| | - Iacopo Predieri
- Department of Psychology, University of Bologna, Bologna, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
|
24
|
Cong YQ, Junge C, Aktar E, Raijmakers M, Franklin A, Sauter D. Pre-verbal infants perceive emotional facial expressions categorically. Cogn Emot 2018; 33:391-403. [PMID: 29607731 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2018.1455640] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/17/2022]
Abstract
Adults perceive emotional expressions categorically, with discrimination being faster and more accurate between expressions from different emotion categories (i.e. blends with two different predominant emotions) than between two stimuli from the same category (i.e. blends with the same predominant emotion). The current study sought to test whether facial expressions of happiness and fear are perceived categorically by pre-verbal infants, using a new stimulus set that was shown to yield categorical perception in adult observers (Experiments 1 and 2). These stimuli were then used with 7-month-old infants (N = 34) using a habituation and visual preference paradigm (Experiment 3). Infants were first habituated to an expression of one emotion, then presented with the same expression paired with a novel expression either from the same emotion category or from a different emotion category. After habituation to fear, infants displayed a novelty preference for pairs of between-category expressions, but not within-category ones, showing categorical perception. However, infants showed no novelty preference when they were habituated to happiness. Our findings provide evidence for categorical perception of emotional expressions in pre-verbal infants, while the asymmetrical effect challenges the notion of a bias towards negative information in this age group.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yong-Qi Cong
- a Department of Social Psychology , University of Amsterdam , Amsterdam , The Netherlands
| | - Caroline Junge
- b Department of Developmental Psychology , Utrecht University , Utrecht , The Netherlands
| | - Evin Aktar
- c Department of Clinical Psychology , Leiden University , Leiden , The Netherlands
| | - Maartje Raijmakers
- c Department of Clinical Psychology , Leiden University , Leiden , The Netherlands.,d Department of Developmental Psychology , University of Amsterdam , Amsterdam , The Netherlands.,f Present address: Department of Educational Studies , Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam , Amsterdam , The Netherlands
| | - Anna Franklin
- e School of Psychology , University of Sussex , Falmer Brighton , UK
| | - Disa Sauter
- a Department of Social Psychology , University of Amsterdam , Amsterdam , The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Kibbe MM, Kaldy Z, Blaser E. Rules infants look by: Testing the assumption of transitivity in visual salience. INFANCY 2018; 23:156-172. [PMID: 29662430 PMCID: PMC5898438 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12219] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
What drives infants' attention in complex visual scenes? Early models of infant attention suggested that the degree to which different visual features were detectable determines their attentional priority. Here, we tested this by asking whether two targets - defined by different features, but each equally salient when evaluated independently - would drive attention equally when pitted head-to-head. In Experiment 1, we presented 6-month-old infants with an array of gabor patches in which a target region varied either in color or spatial frequency from the background. Using a forced-choice preferential-looking method, we measured how readily infants fixated the target as its featural difference from the background was parametrically increased. Then, in Experiment 2, we used these psychometric preference functions to choose values for color and spatial frequency targets that were equally salient (preferred), and pitted them against each other within the same display. We reasoned that, if salience is transitive, then the stimuli should be iso-salient and infants should therefore show no systematic preference for either stimulus. On the contrary, we found that infants consistently preferred the color-defined stimulus. This suggests that computing visual salience in more complex scenes needs to include factors above and beyond local salience values.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Melissa M. Kibbe
- Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, Boston University, 64 Cummington Mall, Boston, MA 02215
| | - Zsuzsa Kaldy
- Department of Psychology, University of Massachusetts Boston, 100 Morrissey Blvd., Boston, MA 02125
| | - Erik Blaser
- Department of Psychology, University of Massachusetts Boston, 100 Morrissey Blvd., Boston, MA 02125
| |
Collapse
|
26
|
Abstract
What is the relationship between language, emotion concepts, and perceptual categories? Here I compare the strong Whorfian view of linguistic relativity, which argues that language plays a necessary role in the perception of emotions, to the alternative view that different levels of processing (e.g., linguistic, conceptual, perceptual) are relatively independent and thus, that language does not play a foundational role in emotion perception. I examine neuropsychological studies that have tested strong claims of linguistic relativity, and discuss research on categorical perception of emotional expressions, where the two accounts have been directly tested against each other. As in other perceptual domains, there is little evidence that language plays a foundational role in the perception of emotion.
Collapse
|
27
|
de Schonen S, Bertoncini J, Petroff N, Couloigner V, Van Den Abbeele T. Visual cortical activity before and after cochlear implantation: A follow up ERP prospective study in deaf children. Int J Psychophysiol 2017; 123:88-102. [PMID: 29108924 DOI: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2017.10.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/15/2016] [Revised: 10/20/2017] [Accepted: 10/23/2017] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
ERPs were recorded in response to presentation of static colored patterned stimuli in 25 children (19 to 80months of age at cochlear implantation, CI) with very early prelingual profound deafness (PreLD), 21 postlingual profoundly deaf children (PostLD) (34 to 180months of age at CI) and gender- and age-matched control hearing children. Recording sessions were performed before CI, then 6 and 24months after CI. Results showed that prelingual and, at a lesser degree, postlingual auditory deprivation altered cortical visual neural activity associated to colored shapes from both P1 and N1 cortical processing stages. The P1 and N1 amplitude modifications vanished about 24months after CI in both PreLD and PostLD deaf children. In PreLD the visual processing pattern becomes similar to the typical one essentially by an amplitude decrease of P1 on the left hemisphere together with an amplitude increase of the N1 on the right hemisphere. Finally, in PreLD, increased LH advantage over the RH in N1 amplitude on the cerebellar-occipito-parietal region before CI showed a significant inverse relationship with speech perception outcomes 3years after CI. Investigating early visual processing development and its neural substrates in deaf children would help to understand the variability of CI outcome, because their cortical visual organization diverged from the one of typically developing hearing children, and cannot be predicted from what is observed in deaf adults.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Scania de Schonen
- Laboratory Psychology of Perception, University Paris Descartes-CNRS (UMR8242), Neuroscience and Cognition Institute, Paris, France.
| | - Josiane Bertoncini
- Laboratory Psychology of Perception, University Paris Descartes-CNRS (UMR8242), Neuroscience and Cognition Institute, Paris, France.
| | - Nathalie Petroff
- Dpt of Otorhinolaryngology and ENT Surgery, University Hospital (CHU), Hôpital Robert Debré, Paris, France.
| | - Vincent Couloigner
- Dpt of Otorhinolaryngology and ENT Surgery, University Hospital (CHU), Hôpital Robert Debré, Paris, France.
| | - Thierry Van Den Abbeele
- Dpt of Otorhinolaryngology and ENT Surgery, University Hospital (CHU), Hôpital Robert Debré, Paris, France.
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
Abstract
The biological basis of the commonality in color lexicons across languages has been hotly debated for decades. Prior evidence that infants categorize color could provide support for the hypothesis that color categorization systems are not purely constructed by communication and culture. Here, we investigate the relationship between infants' categorization of color and the commonality across color lexicons, and the potential biological origin of infant color categories. We systematically mapped infants' categorical recognition memory for hue onto a stimulus array used previously to document the color lexicons of 110 nonindustrialized languages. Following familiarization to a given hue, infants' response to a novel hue indicated that their recognition memory parses the hue continuum into red, yellow, green, blue, and purple categories. Infants' categorical distinctions aligned with common distinctions in color lexicons and are organized around hues that are commonly central to lexical categories across languages. The boundaries between infants' categorical distinctions also aligned, relative to the adaptation point, with the cardinal axes that describe the early stages of color representation in retinogeniculate pathways, indicating that infant color categorization may be partly organized by biological mechanisms of color vision. The findings suggest that color categorization in language and thought is partially biologically constrained and have implications for broader debate on how biology, culture, and communication interact in human cognition.
Collapse
|
29
|
Gainotti G. The Differential Contributions of Conceptual Representation Format and Language Structure to Levels of Semantic Abstraction Capacity. Neuropsychol Rev 2017; 27:134-145. [DOI: 10.1007/s11065-016-9339-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/08/2016] [Accepted: 12/21/2016] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
|
30
|
Zhong W, Li Y, Huang Y, Li H, Mo L. Is the Lateralized Categorical Perception of Color a Situational Effect of Language on Color Perception? Cogn Sci 2017; 42:350-364. [PMID: 28345795 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.12493] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/17/2015] [Revised: 01/21/2017] [Accepted: 01/26/2017] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Abstract
This study investigated whether and how a person's varied series of lexical categories corresponding to different discriminatory characteristics of the same colors affect his or her perception of colors. In three experiments, Chinese participants were primed to categorize four graduated colors-specifically dark green, light green, light blue, and dark blue-into green and blue; light color and dark color; and dark green, light green, light blue, and dark blue. The participants were then required to complete a visual search task. Reaction times in the visual search task indicated that different lateralized categorical perceptions (CPs) of color corresponded to the various priming situations. These results suggest that all of the lexical categories corresponding to different discriminatory characteristics of the same colors can influence people's perceptions of colors and that color perceptions can be influenced differently by distinct types of lexical categories depending on the context.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Weifang Zhong
- Center for Studies of Psychological Application and School of Psychology, South China Normal University.,Guangdong Justice Police Vocational College
| | - You Li
- Center for Studies of Psychological Application and School of Psychology, South China Normal University
| | - Yulan Huang
- Center for Studies of Psychological Application and School of Psychology, South China Normal University
| | - He Li
- Center for Studies of Psychological Application and School of Psychology, South China Normal University
| | - Lei Mo
- Center for Studies of Psychological Application and School of Psychology, South China Normal University
| |
Collapse
|
31
|
Right away: A late, right-lateralized category effect complements an early, left-lateralized category effect in visual search. Psychon Bull Rev 2017; 24:1611-1619. [DOI: 10.3758/s13423-017-1246-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
32
|
Word and object recognition during reading acquisition: MEG evidence. Dev Cogn Neurosci 2017; 24:21-32. [PMID: 28119183 PMCID: PMC5437840 DOI: 10.1016/j.dcn.2017.01.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/30/2016] [Revised: 11/28/2016] [Accepted: 01/04/2017] [Indexed: 11/20/2022] Open
Abstract
Studies on adults suggest that reading-induced brain changes might not be limited to linguistic processes. It is still unclear whether these results can be generalized to reading development. The present study shows to which extent neural responses to verbal and nonverbal stimuli are reorganized while children learn to read. MEG data of thirty Basque children (4-8y) were collected while they were presented with written words, spoken words and visual objects. The evoked fields elicited by the experimental stimuli were compared to their scrambled counterparts. Visual words elicited left posterior (200-300ms) and temporal activations (400-800ms). The size of these effects increased as reading performance improved, suggesting a reorganization of children's visual word responses. Spoken words elicited greater left temporal responses relative to scrambles (300-700ms). No evidence for the influence of reading expertise was observed. Brain responses to objects were greater than to scrambles in bilateral posterior regions (200-500ms). There was a greater left hemisphere involvement as reading errors decreased, suggesting a strengthened verbal decoding of visual configurations with reading acquisition. The present results reveal that learning to read not only influences written word processing, but also affects visual object recognition, suggesting a non-language specific impact of reading on children's neural mechanisms.
Collapse
|
33
|
Variations in normal color vision. VII. Relationships between color naming and hue scaling. Vision Res 2017; 141:66-75. [PMID: 28042057 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2016.12.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/01/2016] [Revised: 12/02/2016] [Accepted: 12/06/2016] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
A longstanding and unresolved question is how observers construct a discrete set of color categories to partition and label the continuous variations in light spectra, and how these categories might reflect the neural representation of color. We explored the properties of color naming and its relationship to color appearance by analyzing individual differences in color-naming and hue-scaling patterns, using factor analysis of individual differences to identify separate and shared processes underlying hue naming (labeling) and hue scaling (color appearance). Observers labeled the hues of 36 stimuli spanning different angles in cone-opponent space, using a set of eight terms corresponding to primary (red, green, blue, yellow) or binary (orange, purple, blue-green, yellow-green) hues. The boundaries defining different terms varied mostly independently, reflecting the influence of at least seven to eight factors. This finding is inconsistent with conventional color-opponent models in which all colors derive from the relative responses of underlying red-green and blue-yellow dimensions. Instead, color categories may reflect qualitatively distinct attributes that are free to vary with the specific spectral stimuli they label. Inter-observer differences in color naming were large and systematic, and we examined whether these differences were associated with differences in color appearance by comparing the hue naming to color percepts assessed by hue scaling measured in the same observers (from Emery et al., 2017). Variability in both tasks again depended on multiple (7 or 8) factors, with some Varimax-rotated factors specific to hue naming or hue scaling, but others common to corresponding stimuli for both judgments. The latter suggests that at least some of the differences in how individuals name or categorize color are related to differences in how the stimuli are perceived.
Collapse
|
34
|
Yu M, Mo C, Zeng T, Zhao S, Mo L. Short-term trained lexical categories affect preattentive shape perception: Evidence from vMMN. Psychophysiology 2016; 54:462-468. [DOI: 10.1111/psyp.12797] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/19/2016] [Accepted: 10/24/2016] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Mengxia Yu
- Center for Studies of Psychological Application; South China Normal University; Guangzhou China
| | - Ce Mo
- Peking-Tsinghua Center for Life Sciences, Academy for Advanced Interdisciplinary Studies; Peking University; Beijing China
| | - Tianyu Zeng
- Center for Studies of Psychological Application; South China Normal University; Guangzhou China
| | - Sasa Zhao
- Center for Studies of Psychological Application; South China Normal University; Guangzhou China
| | - Lei Mo
- Center for Studies of Psychological Application; South China Normal University; Guangzhou China
| |
Collapse
|
35
|
Abstract
Is it possible to turn psychology into “hard science”? Physics of the mind follows the fundamental methodology of physics in all areas where physics have been developed. What is common among Newtonian mechanics, statistical physics, quantum physics, thermodynamics, theory of relativity, astrophysics… and a theory of superstrings? The common among all areas of physics is a methodology of physics discussed in the first few lines of the paper. Is physics of the mind possible? Is it possible to describe the mind based on the few first principles as physics does? The mind with its variabilities and uncertainties, the mind from perception and elementary cognition to emotions and abstract ideas, to high cognition. Is it possible to turn psychology and neuroscience into “hard” sciences? The paper discusses established first principles of the mind, their mathematical formulations, and a mathematical model of the mind derived from these first principles, mechanisms of concepts, emotions, instincts, behavior, language, cognition, intuitions, conscious and unconscious, abilities for symbols, functions of the beautiful and musical emotions in cognition and evolution. Some of the theoretical predictions have been experimentally confirmed. This research won national and international awards. In addition to summarizing existing results the paper describes new development theoretical and experimental. The paper discusses unsolved theoretical problems as well as experimental challenges for future research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Leonid I Perlovsky
- MGH/HST Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Medical School, Harvard UniversityCambridge, MA, USA; Psychology and Engineering Departments, Northeastern UniversityBoston, MA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
36
|
Franklin A. The Role of Culture and Language in the Development of Color Categorization. MINNESOTA SYMPOSIA ON CHILD PSYCHOLOGY 2016. [DOI: 10.1002/9781119301981.ch2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
|
37
|
Thierry G. Neurolinguistic Relativity: How Language Flexes Human Perception and Cognition. LANGUAGE LEARNING 2016; 66:690-713. [PMID: 27642191 PMCID: PMC5006882 DOI: 10.1111/lang.12186] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 03/06/2016] [Indexed: 05/10/2023]
Abstract
The time has come, perhaps, to go beyond merely acknowledging that language is a core manifestation of the workings of the human mind and that it relates interactively to all aspects of thinking. The issue, thus, is not to decide whether language and human thought may be ineluctably linked (they just are), but rather to determine what the characteristics of this relationship may be and to understand how language influences-and may be influenced by-nonverbal information processing. In an attempt to demystify linguistic relativity, I review neurolinguistic studies from our research group showing a link between linguistic distinctions and perceptual or conceptual processing. On the basis of empirical evidence showing effects of terminology on perception, language-idiosyncratic relationships in semantic memory, grammatical skewing of event conceptualization, and unconscious modulation of executive functioning by verbal input, I advocate a neurofunctional approach through which we can systematically explore how languages shape human thought.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Guillaume Thierry
- School of Psychology and Centre for Research on Bilingualism Bangor University
| |
Collapse
|
38
|
Holmes KJ, Regier T. Categorical Perception Beyond the Basic Level: The Case of Warm and Cool Colors. Cogn Sci 2016; 41:1135-1147. [PMID: 27404377 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.12393] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/31/2015] [Revised: 02/10/2016] [Accepted: 03/22/2016] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
Abstract
Categories can affect our perception of the world, rendering between-category differences more salient than within-category ones. Across many studies, such categorical perception (CP) has been observed for the basic-level categories of one's native language. Other research points to categorical distinctions beyond the basic level, but it does not demonstrate CP for such distinctions. Here we provide such a demonstration. Specifically, we show CP in English speakers for the non-basic distinction between "warm" and "cool" colors, claimed to represent the earliest stage of color lexicon evolution. Notably, the advantage for discriminating colors that straddle the warm-cool boundary was restricted to the right visual field-the same behavioral signature previously observed for basic-level categories. This pattern held in a replication experiment with increased power. Our findings show that categorical distinctions beyond the basic-level repertoire of one's native language are psychologically salient and may be spontaneously accessed during normal perceptual processing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Terry Regier
- Department of Linguistics and Cognitive Science Program, University of California, Berkeley
| |
Collapse
|
39
|
Timeo S, Farroni T, Maass A. Race and Color: Two Sides of One Story? Development of Biases in Categorical Perception. Child Dev 2016; 88:83-102. [DOI: 10.1111/cdev.12564] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
|
40
|
Grandison A, Sowden PT, Drivonikou VG, Notman LA, Alexander I, Davies IRL. Chromatic Perceptual Learning but No Category Effects without Linguistic Input. Front Psychol 2016; 7:731. [PMID: 27252669 PMCID: PMC4879779 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00731] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/29/2015] [Accepted: 05/02/2016] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Perceptual learning involves an improvement in perceptual judgment with practice, which is often specific to stimulus or task factors. Perceptual learning has been shown on a range of visual tasks but very little research has explored chromatic perceptual learning. Here, we use two low level perceptual threshold tasks and a supra-threshold target detection task to assess chromatic perceptual learning and category effects. Experiment 1 investigates whether chromatic thresholds reduce as a result of training and at what level of analysis learning effects occur. Experiment 2 explores the effect of category training on chromatic thresholds, whether training of this nature is category specific and whether it can induce categorical responding. Experiment 3 investigates the effect of category training on a higher level, lateralized target detection task, previously found to be sensitive to category effects. The findings indicate that performance on a perceptual threshold task improves following training but improvements do not transfer across retinal location or hue. Therefore, chromatic perceptual learning is category specific and can occur at relatively early stages of visual analysis. Additionally, category training does not induce category effects on a low level perceptual threshold task, as indicated by comparable discrimination thresholds at the newly learned hue boundary and adjacent test points. However, category training does induce emerging category effects on a supra-threshold target detection task. Whilst chromatic perceptual learning is possible, learnt category effects appear to be a product of left hemisphere processing, and may require the input of higher level linguistic coding processes in order to manifest.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | | | | | - Iona Alexander
- School of Psychology, University of SurreyGuildford, UK
- Nuffield Laboratory of Ophthalmology, University of OxfordOxford, UK
| | | |
Collapse
|
41
|
Parraga CA, Akbarinia A. NICE: A Computational Solution to Close the Gap from Colour Perception to Colour Categorization. PLoS One 2016; 11:e0149538. [PMID: 26954691 PMCID: PMC4783039 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0149538] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/15/2015] [Accepted: 02/02/2016] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
The segmentation of visible electromagnetic radiation into chromatic categories by the human visual system has been extensively studied from a perceptual point of view, resulting in several colour appearance models. However, there is currently a void when it comes to relate these results to the physiological mechanisms that are known to shape the pre-cortical and cortical visual pathway. This work intends to begin to fill this void by proposing a new physiologically plausible model of colour categorization based on Neural Isoresponsive Colour Ellipsoids (NICE) in the cone-contrast space defined by the main directions of the visual signals entering the visual cortex. The model was adjusted to fit psychophysical measures that concentrate on the categorical boundaries and are consistent with the ellipsoidal isoresponse surfaces of visual cortical neurons. By revealing the shape of such categorical colour regions, our measures allow for a more precise and parsimonious description, connecting well-known early visual processing mechanisms to the less understood phenomenon of colour categorization. To test the feasibility of our method we applied it to exemplary images and a popular ground-truth chart obtaining labelling results that are better than those of current state-of-the-art algorithms.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- C. Alejandro Parraga
- Centre de Visió per Computador (CVC), Barcelona, Spain
- Departament de Ciències de la Computació, Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona (UAB), Barcelona, Spain
- * E-mail:
| | - Arash Akbarinia
- Centre de Visió per Computador (CVC), Barcelona, Spain
- Departament de Ciències de la Computació, Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona (UAB), Barcelona, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
42
|
Cortical response to categorical color perception in infants investigated by near-infrared spectroscopy. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2016; 113:2370-5. [PMID: 26858441 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1512044113] [Citation(s) in RCA: 37] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Perceptual color space is continuous; however, we tend to divide it into only a small number of categories. It is unclear whether categorical color perception is obtained solely through the development of the visual system or whether it is affected by language acquisition. To address this issue, we recruited prelinguistic infants (5- to 7-mo-olds) to measure changes in brain activity in relation to categorical color differences by using near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS). We presented two sets of geometric figures to infants: One set altered in color between green and blue, and the other set altered between two different shades of green. We found a significant increase in hemodynamic responses during the between-category alternations, but not during the within-category alternations. These differences in hemodynamic response based on categorical relationship were observed only in the bilateral occipitotemporal regions, and not in the occipital region. We confirmed that categorical color differences yield behavioral differences in infants. We also observed comparable hemodynamic responses to categorical color differences in adults. The present study provided the first evidence, to our knowledge, that colors of different categories are represented differently in the visual cortex of prelinguistic infants, which implies that color categories may develop independently before language acquisition.
Collapse
|
43
|
Hoyau E, Cousin E, Jaillard A, Baciu M. Modulation of the inter-hemispheric processing of semantic information during normal aging. A divided visual field experiment. Neuropsychologia 2016; 93:425-436. [PMID: 26724229 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.12.027] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/18/2015] [Revised: 11/12/2015] [Accepted: 12/22/2015] [Indexed: 12/14/2022]
Abstract
We evaluated the effect of normal aging on the inter-hemispheric processing of semantic information by using the divided visual field (DVF) method, with words and pictures. Two main theoretical models have been considered, (a) the HAROLD model which posits that aging is associated with supplementary recruitment of the right hemisphere (RH) and decreased hemispheric specialization, and (b) the RH decline theory, which assumes that the RH becomes less efficient with aging, associated with increased LH specialization. Two groups of subjects were examined, a Young Group (YG) and an Old Group (OG), while participants performed a semantic categorization task (living vs. non-living) in words and pictures. The DVF was realized in two steps: (a) unilateral DVF presentation with stimuli presented separately in each visual field, left or right, allowing for their initial processing by only one hemisphere, right or left, respectively; (b) bilateral DVF presentation (BVF) with stimuli presented simultaneously in both visual fields, followed by their processing by both hemispheres. These two types of presentation permitted the evaluation of two main characteristics of the inter-hemispheric processing of information, the hemispheric specialization (HS) and the inter-hemispheric cooperation (IHC). Moreover, the BVF allowed determining the driver-hemisphere for processing information presented in BVF. Results obtained in OG indicated that: (a) semantic categorization was performed as accurately as YG, even if more slowly, (b) a non-semantic RH decline was observed, and (c) the LH controls the semantic processing during the BVF, suggesting an increased role of the LH in aging. However, despite the stronger involvement of the LH in OG, the RH is not completely devoid of semantic abilities. As discussed in the paper, neither the HAROLD nor the RH decline does fully explain this pattern of results. We rather suggest that the effect of aging on the hemispheric specialization and inter-hemispheric cooperation during semantic processing is explained not by only one model, but by an interaction between several complementary mechanisms and models.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- E Hoyau
- University Grenoble Alpes, LPNC, F-38040 Grenoble, France; CNRS, LPNC UMR 5105, F-38040 Grenoble, France
| | - E Cousin
- University Grenoble Alpes, LPNC, F-38040 Grenoble, France; CNRS, LPNC UMR 5105, F-38040 Grenoble, France; UMS IRMaGe, IRM 3T, CHU Grenoble, University Grenoble Alpes, F-38043 Grenoble, France
| | - A Jaillard
- UMS IRMaGe, IRM 3T, CHU Grenoble, University Grenoble Alpes, F-38043 Grenoble, France
| | - M Baciu
- University Grenoble Alpes, LPNC, F-38040 Grenoble, France; CNRS, LPNC UMR 5105, F-38040 Grenoble, France.
| |
Collapse
|
44
|
Ambrosini E, Vallesi A. Asymmetry in prefrontal resting-state EEG spectral power underlies individual differences in phasic and sustained cognitive control. Neuroimage 2016; 124:843-857. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.09.035] [Citation(s) in RCA: 50] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/26/2015] [Revised: 09/05/2015] [Accepted: 09/11/2015] [Indexed: 10/23/2022] Open
|
45
|
Styles SJ, Plunkett K, Duta MD. Infant VEPs reveal neural correlates of implicit naming: Lateralized differences between lexicalized versus name-unknown pictures. Neuropsychologia 2015; 77:177-84. [PMID: 26232744 PMCID: PMC4617475 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.07.027] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/29/2013] [Revised: 07/25/2015] [Accepted: 07/27/2015] [Indexed: 12/03/2022]
Abstract
Recent behavioural studies with toddlers have demonstrated that simply viewing a picture in silence triggers a cascade of linguistic processing which activates a representation of the picture’s name (Mani and Plunkett, 2010, 2011). Electrophysiological studies have also shown that viewing a picture modulates the auditory evoked potentials (AEPs) triggered by later speech, from early in the second year of life (Duta et al., 2012; Friedrich and Friederici, 2005; Mani et al., 2011) further supporting the notion that picture viewing gives rise to a representation of the picture’s name against which later speech can be matched. However, little is known about how and when the implicit name arises during picture viewing, or about the electrophysiological activity which supports this linguistic process. We report differences in the visual evoked potentials (VEPs) of fourteen-month-old infants who saw photographs of animals and objects, some of which were name-known (lexicalized), while waiting for an auditory label to be presented. During silent picture viewing, lateralized neural activity was selectively triggered by lexicalized items, as compared to nameless items. Lexicalized items generated a short-lasting negative-going deflection over frontal, left centro-temporal, and left occipital regions shortly after the picture appeared (126–225 ms). A positive deflection was also observed over the right hemisphere (particularly centro-temporal regions) in a later, longer-lasting window (421–720 ms). The lateralization of these differences in the VEP suggests the possible involvement of linguistic processes during picture viewing, and may reflect activity involved in the implicit activation of the picture’s name. Infants’ visual evoked potentials (VEPs) to pictures differ when name is known. Lateralization suggests infant linguistic processing during silent picture viewing. Two stages identified in the neural correlates of implicit naming in 14-month-olds.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Suzy J Styles
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Tinbergen Building, 9 South Parks Road, OX1 3UD, UK; Division of Psychology, Nanyang Technological University, 14 Nanyang Drive, 637332, Singapore.
| | - Kim Plunkett
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Tinbergen Building, 9 South Parks Road, OX1 3UD, UK
| | - Mihaela D Duta
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Tinbergen Building, 9 South Parks Road, OX1 3UD, UK
| |
Collapse
|
46
|
Gainotti G. Is the difference between right and left ATLs due to the distinction between general and social cognition or between verbal and non-verbal representations? Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2015; 51:296-312. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.02.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 53] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/09/2014] [Revised: 12/24/2014] [Accepted: 02/07/2015] [Indexed: 01/16/2023]
|
47
|
Sun Y, Cai Y, Lu S. Hemispheric asymmetry in the influence of language on visual perception. Conscious Cogn 2015; 34:16-27. [PMID: 25840357 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2015.03.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/27/2014] [Revised: 03/04/2015] [Accepted: 03/05/2015] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
Abstract
Many studies have shown that language can affect visual perception; however, our understanding of the neural basis of linguistic influence is inadequate. This can be investigated by examining the hemispheric asymmetry of linguistic influence. The left and right hemispheres are dominant in close and distant semantic processing, respectively. In this study, we investigated whether the hemispheric asymmetry of semantic processing led to hemispheric asymmetry for concept priming on the detection of objects degraded by continuous flash suppression. We combined a priming paradigm with the divided visual field paradigm and used continuous flash suppression, which renders objects invisible. The results indicated that the hemispheric asymmetry of semantic processing led to a right lateralization in the influence of more abstract concepts on visual perception. The lateralization of brain connectomes may be the underlying neural basis of this effect.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yanliang Sun
- Department of Psychology and Behavioral Sciences, Zhejiang University, Hangzhou, China
| | - Yongchun Cai
- Department of Psychology and Behavioral Sciences, Zhejiang University, Hangzhou, China.
| | - Shena Lu
- Department of Psychology and Behavioral Sciences, Zhejiang University, Hangzhou, China.
| |
Collapse
|
48
|
|
49
|
Zhong W, Li Y, Li P, Xu G, Mo L. Short-term trained lexical categories produce preattentive categorical perception of color: Evidence from ERPs. Psychophysiology 2014; 52:98-106. [DOI: 10.1111/psyp.12294] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/14/2013] [Accepted: 06/10/2014] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Weifang Zhong
- Center for Studies of Psychological Application and School of Psychology; South China Normal University; Guangzhou China
| | - You Li
- Center for Studies of Psychological Application and School of Psychology; South China Normal University; Guangzhou China
| | - Peixin Li
- Center for Studies of Psychological Application and School of Psychology; South China Normal University; Guangzhou China
| | - Guiping Xu
- Center for Studies of Psychological Application and School of Psychology; South China Normal University; Guangzhou China
| | - Lei Mo
- Center for Studies of Psychological Application and School of Psychology; South China Normal University; Guangzhou China
| |
Collapse
|
50
|
Abstract
In this review, we synthesize the existing literature demonstrating the dynamic interplay between conceptual knowledge and visual perceptual processing. We consider two theoretical frameworks that demonstrate interactions between processes and brain areas traditionally considered perceptual or conceptual. Specifically, we discuss categorical perception, in which visual objects are represented according to category membership, and highlight studies showing that category knowledge can penetrate early stages of visual analysis. We next discuss the embodied account of conceptual knowledge, which holds that concepts are instantiated in the same neural regions required for specific types of perception and action, and discuss the limitations of this framework. We additionally consider studies showing that gaining abstract semantic knowledge about objects and faces leads to behavioral and electrophysiological changes that are indicative of more efficient stimulus processing. Finally, we consider the role that perceiver goals and motivation may play in shaping the interaction between conceptual and perceptual processing. We hope to demonstrate how pervasive such interactions between motivation, conceptual knowledge, and perceptual processing are in our understanding of the visual environment, and to demonstrate the need for future research aimed at understanding how such interactions arise in the brain.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jessica A Collins
- Department of Psychology, Temple University, Weiss Hall, 1701 North 13th Street, Philadelphia, PA, 19122, USA,
| | | |
Collapse
|