1
|
Li H, Cai M, Tuladhar AM. Cognition, apathy, and gait dysfunction in cerebral small vessel disease: A shared neural basis? Neural Regen Res 2026; 21:302-303. [PMID: 39665831 PMCID: PMC12094553 DOI: 10.4103/nrr.nrr-d-24-00925] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/13/2024] [Revised: 10/23/2024] [Accepted: 11/04/2024] [Indexed: 12/13/2024] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Hao Li
- Radboud University Medical Center, Department of Neurology; Radboud Institute for Medical Research and Innovation and Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behavior, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | - Mengfei Cai
- Radboud University Medical Center, Department of Neurology; Radboud Institute for Medical Research and Innovation and Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behavior, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
- Department of Neurology, Guangdong Neuroscience Institute, Guangdong Provincial People’s Hospital, Guangdong Academy of Medical Sciences, Southern Medical University, Guangzhou, Guangdong Province, China
| | - Anil Man Tuladhar
- Radboud University Medical Center, Department of Neurology; Radboud Institute for Medical Research and Innovation and Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behavior, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Morris LA, Horne KL, Manohar S, Paermentier L, Buchanan CM, MacAskill MR, Myall DJ, Apps M, Roxburgh R, Anderson TJ, Husain M, Le Heron CJ. Decision cost hypersensitivity underlies Huntington's disease apathy. Brain 2025; 148:861-874. [PMID: 39269457 PMCID: PMC11884755 DOI: 10.1093/brain/awae296] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/14/2024] [Accepted: 08/28/2024] [Indexed: 09/15/2024] Open
Abstract
The neuropsychiatric syndrome of apathy is now recognized to be a common and disabling condition in Huntington's disease. However, the mechanisms underlying it are poorly understood. One way to investigate apathy is to use a theoretical framework of normal motivated behaviour, to determine where breakdown has occurred in people with this behavioural disruption. A fundamental computation underlying motivated, goal-directed behaviour across species is weighing up the costs and rewards associated with actions. Here, we asked whether people with apathy are more sensitive to costs of actions (physical effort and time delay), less sensitive to rewarding outcomes, or both. Based on the unique anatomical substrates associated with Huntington's disease pathology, we hypothesized that a general hypersensitivity to costs would underpin Huntington's disease apathy. Genetically confirmed carriers of the expanded Huntingtin gene (premanifest to mild motor manifest disease, n = 53) were compared to healthy controls (n = 38). Participants performed a physical effort-based decision-making task (Apple Gathering Task) and a delay discounting task (Money Choice Questionnaire). Choice data was analysed using linear regression and drift diffusion models that also accounted for the time taken to make decisions. Apathetic people with Huntington's disease accepted fewer offers overall on the Apple Gathering Task, specifically driven by increased sensitivity to physical effort costs, and not explained by motor severity, mood, cognition or medication. Drift diffusion modelling provided further evidence of effort hypersensitivity, with apathy associated with a faster drift rate towards rejecting offers as a function of varying effort. Increased delay sensitivity was also associated with apathy, both when analysing raw choice and drift rate, where there was moderate evidence of Huntington's disease apathy drifting faster towards the immediately available (low-cost) option. Furthermore, the effort and delay sensitivity parameters from these tasks were positively correlated. The results demonstrate a clear mechanism for apathy in Huntington's disease, cost hypersensitivity, which manifests in both the effort and time costs associated with actions towards rewarding goals. This suggests that Huntington's disease pathology may cause a domain-general disruption of cost processing, which is distinct from apathy occurrence in other brain disorders and may require different therapeutic approaches.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lee-Anne Morris
- Department of Medicine, University of Otago, Christchurch 8011, New Zealand
- New Zealand Brain Research Institute, Christchurch 8011, New Zealand
| | - Kyla-Louise Horne
- Department of Medicine, University of Otago, Christchurch 8011, New Zealand
- New Zealand Brain Research Institute, Christchurch 8011, New Zealand
| | - Sanjay Manohar
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford OX2 6GG, UK
- Nuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Oxford, Oxford OX3 9DU, UK
| | - Laura Paermentier
- New Zealand Brain Research Institute, Christchurch 8011, New Zealand
| | - Christina M Buchanan
- Department of Neurology, Auckland City Hospital, Te Whatu Ora Health New Zealand, Auckland 1023, New Zealand
- Centre for Brain Research Neurogenetics Research Clinic, University of Auckland, Auckland 1023, New Zealand
| | | | - Daniel J Myall
- New Zealand Brain Research Institute, Christchurch 8011, New Zealand
| | - Matthew Apps
- Centre for Human Brain Health, School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, Birmingham B15 2SQ, UK
| | - Richard Roxburgh
- Department of Neurology, Auckland City Hospital, Te Whatu Ora Health New Zealand, Auckland 1023, New Zealand
- Centre for Brain Research Neurogenetics Research Clinic, University of Auckland, Auckland 1023, New Zealand
| | - Tim J Anderson
- Department of Medicine, University of Otago, Christchurch 8011, New Zealand
- New Zealand Brain Research Institute, Christchurch 8011, New Zealand
- Department of Neurology, Christchurch Hospital, Te Whatu Ora Health New Zealand, Christchurch 8011, New Zealand
| | - Masud Husain
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford OX2 6GG, UK
- Nuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Oxford, Oxford OX3 9DU, UK
| | - Campbell J Le Heron
- Department of Medicine, University of Otago, Christchurch 8011, New Zealand
- New Zealand Brain Research Institute, Christchurch 8011, New Zealand
- Department of Neurology, Christchurch Hospital, Te Whatu Ora Health New Zealand, Christchurch 8011, New Zealand
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Fang Y, Chao X, Wang J, Lu Z, Yin D, Shi R, Wang P, Liu X, Sun W. Reward Decision Network Disconnection in Poststroke Apathy: A Prospective Multimodality Imaging Study. Hum Brain Mapp 2025; 46:e70139. [PMID: 39835577 PMCID: PMC11747988 DOI: 10.1002/hbm.70139] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/23/2024] [Revised: 11/29/2024] [Accepted: 01/04/2025] [Indexed: 01/22/2025] Open
Abstract
Apathy is a common neuropsychiatric symptom following stroke, characterized by reduced goal-directed behavior. The reward decision network (RDN), which plays a crucial role in regulating goal-directed behaviors, is closely associated with apathy. However, the relationship between poststroke apathy (PSA) and RDN dysfunction remains unclear due to apathy heterogeneity, the confounding effect of depression and individual variability in lesion impacts. This study aims to dissect the heterogeneity of PSA and explore the link between lesion-induced RDN damage and PSA. We prospectively recruited 207 patients with acute ischemic infarction and 60 demographically matched healthy controls. Participants underwent neuroimaging and longitudinal neuropsychiatric assessments. To characterize PSA heterogeneity, we employed multivariate analysis and clustering algorithms based on whole-brain functional connectivity and clinical assessments to classify patients into different PSA biotypes. We embedded each patient's lesion into a structural connectome atlas to obtain white matter (WM) disconnection maps. On this basis, WM disconnection scores were calculated for each brain region to quantify lesion-induced WM damage. We employed the XGBoost model to predict PSA biotypes based on WM disconnection scores, comparing the performance of models focusing on RDN-specific versus whole-brain WM disconnection. Additionally, we explored WM damage patterns across different biotypes by comparing disconnection scores in critical brain regions. We identified four PSA biotypes with unique clinical trajectories and neurobiological underpinnings. Biotype 4 was characterized by persistent apathy with depressive symptoms. Biotype 2 showed persistent apathy. Biotype 3 was non-apathetic. Biotype 1 exhibited delayed-onset apathy. The XGBoost models, when focused on the RDN-specific WM disconnection, performed significantly better in predicting PSA biotypes compared to the whole-brain WM disconnection model (t(164.66) = 8.871, p < 0.001). Analysis of WM disconnection patterns revealed that Biotype 4 exhibited more extensive RDN damage in crucial regions, Biotype 1 had a unique pattern of damage in the anterior cingulate cortex (t(61) = 1.874, p = 0.032), and Biotype 2 had a unique pattern of damage in the orbitofrontal cortex (t(53)= 1.827, p = 0.036). This study dissected PSA heterogeneity and demonstrated that RDN damage is a critical factor in PSA variability. We found that lesion-induced WM disconnections in anterior cingulate cortex and orbitofrontal cortex can lead to delayed-onset and persistent apathy, respectively. Furthermore, our findings revealed that apathy not only has distinct pathogenic mechanisms, but also shares neurobiological substrates with depression.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yirong Fang
- Department of Neurology, Centre for Leading Medicine and Advanced Technologies of IHM, the First Affiliated Hospital of USTC, Division of Life Sciences and MedicineUniversity of Science and Technology of ChinaHefeiAnhuiChina
| | - Xian Chao
- Department of Neurology, Centre for Leading Medicine and Advanced Technologies of IHM, the First Affiliated Hospital of USTC, Division of Life Sciences and MedicineUniversity of Science and Technology of ChinaHefeiAnhuiChina
| | - Jinjing Wang
- Department of Neurology, Centre for Leading Medicine and Advanced Technologies of IHM, the First Affiliated Hospital of USTC, Division of Life Sciences and MedicineUniversity of Science and Technology of ChinaHefeiAnhuiChina
| | - Zeyu Lu
- Department of Neurology, Centre for Leading Medicine and Advanced Technologies of IHM, the First Affiliated Hospital of USTC, Division of Life Sciences and MedicineUniversity of Science and Technology of ChinaHefeiAnhuiChina
| | - Dawei Yin
- Department of Radiology, the First Affiliated Hospital of USTC, Division of Life Sciences and MedicineUniversity of Science and Technology of ChinaHefeiAnhuiChina
| | - Ran Shi
- Department of Radiology, the First Affiliated Hospital of USTC, Division of Life Sciences and MedicineUniversity of Science and Technology of ChinaHefeiAnhuiChina
| | - Peng Wang
- Department of Radiology, the First Affiliated Hospital of USTC, Division of Life Sciences and MedicineUniversity of Science and Technology of ChinaHefeiAnhuiChina
| | - Xinfeng Liu
- Department of Neurology, Centre for Leading Medicine and Advanced Technologies of IHM, the First Affiliated Hospital of USTC, Division of Life Sciences and MedicineUniversity of Science and Technology of ChinaHefeiAnhuiChina
| | - Wen Sun
- Department of Neurology, Centre for Leading Medicine and Advanced Technologies of IHM, the First Affiliated Hospital of USTC, Division of Life Sciences and MedicineUniversity of Science and Technology of ChinaHefeiAnhuiChina
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Li H, Jacob MA, Cai M, Kessels RPC, Norris DG, Duering M, de Leeuw FE, Tuladhar AM. Meso-cortical pathway damage in cognition, apathy and gait in cerebral small vessel disease. Brain 2024; 147:3804-3816. [PMID: 38709856 PMCID: PMC11531843 DOI: 10.1093/brain/awae145] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/11/2023] [Revised: 04/08/2024] [Accepted: 04/12/2024] [Indexed: 05/08/2024] Open
Abstract
Cerebral small vessel disease (SVD) is known to contribute to cognitive impairment, apathy and gait dysfunction. Although associations between cognitive impairment and either apathy or gait dysfunction have been shown in SVD, the inter-relations among these three clinical features and their potential common neural basis remain unexplored. The dopaminergic meso-cortical and meso-limbic pathways have been known as the important brain circuits for both cognitive control, emotion regulation and motor function. Here, we investigated the potential inter-relations between cognitive impairment, apathy and gait dysfunction, with a specific focus on determining whether these clinical features are associated with damage to the meso-cortical and meso-limbic pathways in SVD. In this cross-sectional study, we included 213 participants with SVD for whom MRI and comprehensive neurobehavioural assessments were performed. These assessments comprised six clinical measures: processing speed, executive function, memory, apathy (based on the Apathy Evaluation Scale) and gait function (based on the time and steps in the Timed Up and Go Test). We reconstructed five tracts connecting the ventral tegmental area (VTA) and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (PFC), ventral lateral PFC, medial orbitofrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and nucleus accumbens within meso-cortical and meso-limbic pathways using diffusion weighted imaging. The damage along the five tracts was quantified using the free water (FW) and FW-corrected mean diffusivity indices. Furthermore, we explored the inter-correlations among the six clinical measures and identified their common components using principal component analysis (PCA). Linear regression analyses showed that higher FW values of tracts within meso-cortical pathways were related to these clinical measures in cognition, apathy, and gait (all P-corrected values < 0.05). The PCA showed strong inter-associations among these clinical measures and identified a common component wherein all six clinical measures loaded on. Higher FW values of tracts within meso-cortical pathways were related to the PCA-derived common component (all P-corrected values < 0.05). Moreover, FW values of the VTA-ACC tract showed the strongest contribution to the PCA-derived common component over all other neuroimaging features. In conclusion, our study showed that the three clinical features (cognitive impairment, apathy, and gait dysfunction) of SVD are strongly inter-related and that the damage in meso-cortical pathway could be the common neural basis underlying the three features in SVD. These findings advance our understanding of the mechanisms behind these clinical features of SVD and have the potential to inform novel management and intervention strategies for SVD.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hao Li
- Department of Neurology, Radboud Institute for Medical research and Innovation and Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical Center, 6525 GA Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | - Mina A Jacob
- Department of Neurology, Radboud Institute for Medical research and Innovation and Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical Center, 6525 GA Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | - Mengfei Cai
- Department of Neurology, Radboud Institute for Medical research and Innovation and Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical Center, 6525 GA Nijmegen, The Netherlands
- Department of Neurology, Guangdong Neuroscience Institute, Guangdong Provincial People’s Hospital, Guangdong Academy of Medical Sciences, Southern Medical University, 510000 Guangzhou, China
| | - Roy P C Kessels
- Radboud University, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Centre for Cognition, Radboud University, 6525 GD Nijmegen, The Netherlands
- Department of Medical Psychology and Radboudumc Alzheimer Center, Radboud University Medical Center, 6525 GA Nijmegen, The Netherlands
- Centre of Excellence for Korsakoff and Alcohol-Related Cognitive Disorders, Vincent van Gogh Institute for Psychiatry, 5804 AV Venray, The Netherlands
| | - David G Norris
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging, Radboud University, 6525 GD Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | - Marco Duering
- Medical Image Analysis Center (MIAC AG) and Department of Biomedical Engineering, University of Basel, 4051 Basel, Switzerland
- Institute for Stroke and Dementia Research (ISD), LMU University Hospital, 81377 LMU Munich, Germany
| | - Frank-Erik de Leeuw
- Department of Neurology, Radboud Institute for Medical research and Innovation and Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical Center, 6525 GA Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | - Anil M Tuladhar
- Department of Neurology, Radboud Institute for Medical research and Innovation and Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical Center, 6525 GA Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Barch DM, Culbreth AJ, Sheffield JM. Cognitive Control in Schizophrenia: Advances in Computational Approaches. CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE 2024; 33:35-42. [PMID: 38371195 PMCID: PMC10871692 DOI: 10.1177/09637214231205220] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/20/2024]
Abstract
Psychiatric research is undergoing significant advances in an emerging subspeciality of computational psychiatry, building upon cognitive neuroscience research by expanding to neurocomputational modeling. Here, we illustrate some research trends in this domain using work on proactive cognitive control deficits in schizophrenia as an example. We provide a selective review of formal modeling approaches to understanding cognitive control deficits in psychopathology, focusing primarily on biologically plausible connectionist-level models as well as mathematical models that generate parameter estimates of putatively dissociable psychological or neural processes. We illustrate some of the advantages of these models in terms of understanding both cognitive control deficits in schizophrenia and the potential roles of effort and motivation. Further, we highlight critical future directions for this work, including a focus on establishing psychometric properties, additional work modeling psychotic symptoms and their interaction with cognitive control, and the need to expand both behavioral and neural modeling to samples that include individuals with different mental health conditions, allowing for the examination of dissociable neural or psychological substrates for seemingly similar cognitive impairments across disorders.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Deanna M. Barch
- Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO 63130
- Department of Psychiatry, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO 63110
- Department of Radiology, Mallinckrodt Institute of Radiology, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO 63110
| | - Adam J. Culbreth
- Maryland Psychiatric Research Center, Department of Psychiatry, University of Maryland, Baltimore, MD, 21201
| | - Julia M. Sheffield
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Vanderbilt University Medical School, Nashville, TN, 37212
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Silva AR, Santos I, Fernandes C, Silva C, Pereira D, Galego O, Queiroz H, Almeida MDR, Baldeiras I, Santo G. The relevance of the socio-emotional deficits in cerebral small vessels disease (CSVD): An exploratory study with sporadic CSVD and CADASIL patients. CEREBRAL CIRCULATION - COGNITION AND BEHAVIOR 2023; 5:100186. [PMID: 38162294 PMCID: PMC10757198 DOI: 10.1016/j.cccb.2023.100186] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/19/2023] [Revised: 09/03/2023] [Accepted: 09/25/2023] [Indexed: 01/03/2024]
Abstract
Background Cerebral Small Vessels Disease (CSVD) is categorized in different forms, the most common being the sporadic form and a genetic variant - Cerebral Autosomal Dominant Arteriopathy with Subcortical Infarcts and Leukoencephalopathy (CADASIL). Amongst the most frequent clinical manifestations are the neuropsychological changes of cognitive, behavioral, and emotional nature, whose features are still under debate. Objective This exploratory study aimed to compare the neuropsychological profile of a sporadic CSVD sample and a CADASIL sample with an age, education, and gender matched control group, between the ages of 30-65 YO (total sample mean age=51.16; SD=4.31). Methods 20 patients with sporadic CSVD, 20 patients with CADASIL and 20 matched controls completed a neuropsychological assessment battery. Global cognitive state, processing speed, working memory, attention, executive dysfunction, episodic memory, social cognition, impulsivity, apathy, alexithymia, depression, and anxiety were measured. White matter hyperintensities (WMH) volume were quantified and measured as lesion burden. Results The cognitive differences found between the clinical groups combined (after confirming no differences between the two clinical groups) and matched controls were restricted to speed processing scores (d = 0.32 95 % CI [.12-.47]). The socio-emotional and behavioral profile revealed significantly higher levels of depression (d = 0.21, 95 % CI [.16-.33]). and anxiety (d = 0.25 95 % CI [.19-.32]) in CADASIL and sporadic CSVD groups, and the same for the alexithymia score (d = 0.533 95 % CI [.32-.65]) were the clinical groups revealed impoverished emotional processing compared to controls. WMH only significantly correlated with the cognitive changes and age. Conclusions In our study, CADASIL and sporadic cSVD patients combined, present multiple emotional-behavioral symptoms - alexithymia, anxiety, depression, and in a lower extent apathy and impulsivity - suggesting for the presence of emotion dysregulation behaviors, present independently of age and of the presence of cognitive deficits. Despite of the small sample size that could underpower some findings, this exploratory research supported that these symptoms may have a significant impact in disease monitoring, progression, and prognosis, requiring further investigation regarding their neurophysiological substrates.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ana Rita Silva
- CINEICC – Center for Research in Neuropsychology and Cognitive Behavioral Interventions of the University of Coimbra, Colegio Novo Street, N/A, Coimbra 3000-115, Portugal
| | - Irina Santos
- Neurology Department, Academic and Clinical Centre – Coimbra University Hospitals, Coimbra, Portugal
| | - Carolina Fernandes
- Department of Neurology, Centro Hospitalar e Universitário de Coimbra, CNC-CIBB, Coimbra, Portugal
| | - Cristiana Silva
- Department of Neurology, Centro Hospitalar e Universitário de Coimbra, CNC-CIBB, Coimbra, Portugal
| | - Daniela Pereira
- Neurorradiology Functional Unit, Coimbra University Hospitals, Coimbra, Portugal
| | - Orlando Galego
- Neurorradiology Functional Unit, Coimbra University Hospitals, Coimbra, Portugal
| | - Henrique Queiroz
- Neurorradiology Functional Unit, Coimbra University Hospitals, Coimbra, Portugal
| | | | - Inês Baldeiras
- Faculty of Medicine of the University of Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal
| | - Gustavo Santo
- Department of Neurology, Centro Hospitalar e Universitário de Coimbra, CNC-CIBB, Coimbra, Portugal
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Clancy U, Radakovic R, Doubal F, Hernández MDCV, Maniega SM, Taylor AM, Corley J, Chappell FM, Russ TC, Cox SR, Bastin ME, Deary IJ, Wardlaw JM. Are neuropsychiatric symptoms a marker of small vessel disease progression in older adults? Evidence from the Lothian Birth Cohort 1936. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2023; 38:e5855. [PMID: 36490272 PMCID: PMC10108049 DOI: 10.1002/gps.5855] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/14/2022] [Accepted: 11/21/2022] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Neuropsychiatric symptoms could form part of an early cerebral small vessel disease prodrome that is detectable before stroke or dementia onset. We aimed to identify whether apathy, depression, anxiety, and subjective memory complaints associate with longitudinal white matter hyperintensity (WMH) progression. METHODS Community-dwelling older adults from the observational Lothian Birth Cohort 1936 attended three visits at mean ages 73, 76, and 79 years, repeating MRI, Mini-Mental State Examination, neuropsychiatric (Dimensional Apathy Scale, Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale), and subjective memory symptoms. We ran regression and mixed-effects models for symptoms and normalised WMH volumes (cube root of WMH:ICV × 10). RESULTS At age 73, 76, and 79, m = 672, n = 476, and n = 382 participants attended MRI respectively. Worse apathy at age 79 was associated with WMH volume increase (β = 0.27, p = 0.04) in the preceding 6 years. A 1SD increase in apathy score at age 79 associated with a 0.17 increase in WMH (β = 0.17 normalised WMH percent ICV, p = 0.009). In apathy subscales, executive (β = 0.13, p = 0.05) and emotional (β = 0.13, p = 0.04) scores associated with increasing WMH more than initiation scores (β = 0.11, p = 0.08). Increasing WMH also associated with age (β = 0.40, p = 0.002) but not higher depression (β = -0.01, p = 0.78), anxiety (β = 0.05, p = 0.13) scores, or subjective memory complaints (β = 1.12, p = 0.75). CONCLUSIONS Apathy independently associates with preceding longitudinal WMH progression, while depression, anxiety, and subjective memory complaints do not. Patients with apathy should be considered for enrolment to small vessel disease trials.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Una Clancy
- Centre for Clinical Brain SciencesUniversity of EdinburghEdinburghUK
- UK Dementia Research Institute at The University of EdinburghEdinburghUK
| | - Ratko Radakovic
- Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychological TherapiesUniversity of East AngliaNorwichUK
- Lothian Birth CohortsDepartment of PsychologyUniversity of EdinburghEdinburghUK
- Alzheimer Scotland Dementia Research CentreUniversity of EdinburghEdinburghUK
- Euan MacDonald Centre for MND ResearchUniversity of EdinburghEdinburghUK
| | - Fergus Doubal
- Centre for Clinical Brain SciencesUniversity of EdinburghEdinburghUK
- UK Dementia Research Institute at The University of EdinburghEdinburghUK
| | - Maria del C. Valdés Hernández
- Centre for Clinical Brain SciencesUniversity of EdinburghEdinburghUK
- UK Dementia Research Institute at The University of EdinburghEdinburghUK
- Lothian Birth CohortsDepartment of PsychologyUniversity of EdinburghEdinburghUK
- Scottish Imaging Network, a Platform for Scientific Excellence (SINAPSE) CollaborationEdinburghUK
| | - Susana Muñoz Maniega
- Centre for Clinical Brain SciencesUniversity of EdinburghEdinburghUK
- UK Dementia Research Institute at The University of EdinburghEdinburghUK
- Lothian Birth CohortsDepartment of PsychologyUniversity of EdinburghEdinburghUK
- Scottish Imaging Network, a Platform for Scientific Excellence (SINAPSE) CollaborationEdinburghUK
| | - Adele M. Taylor
- Lothian Birth CohortsDepartment of PsychologyUniversity of EdinburghEdinburghUK
| | - Janie Corley
- Lothian Birth CohortsDepartment of PsychologyUniversity of EdinburghEdinburghUK
| | - Francesca M. Chappell
- Centre for Clinical Brain SciencesUniversity of EdinburghEdinburghUK
- UK Dementia Research Institute at The University of EdinburghEdinburghUK
| | - Tom C. Russ
- Lothian Birth CohortsDepartment of PsychologyUniversity of EdinburghEdinburghUK
- Alzheimer Scotland Dementia Research CentreUniversity of EdinburghEdinburghUK
- Division of PsychiatryCentre for Clinical Brain SciencesUniversity of EdinburghEdinburghUK
| | - Simon R. Cox
- Lothian Birth CohortsDepartment of PsychologyUniversity of EdinburghEdinburghUK
- Scottish Imaging Network, a Platform for Scientific Excellence (SINAPSE) CollaborationEdinburghUK
| | - Mark E. Bastin
- Centre for Clinical Brain SciencesUniversity of EdinburghEdinburghUK
- UK Dementia Research Institute at The University of EdinburghEdinburghUK
- Lothian Birth CohortsDepartment of PsychologyUniversity of EdinburghEdinburghUK
- Scottish Imaging Network, a Platform for Scientific Excellence (SINAPSE) CollaborationEdinburghUK
| | - Ian J. Deary
- Lothian Birth CohortsDepartment of PsychologyUniversity of EdinburghEdinburghUK
- Alzheimer Scotland Dementia Research CentreUniversity of EdinburghEdinburghUK
| | - Joanna M. Wardlaw
- Centre for Clinical Brain SciencesUniversity of EdinburghEdinburghUK
- UK Dementia Research Institute at The University of EdinburghEdinburghUK
- Lothian Birth CohortsDepartment of PsychologyUniversity of EdinburghEdinburghUK
- Scottish Imaging Network, a Platform for Scientific Excellence (SINAPSE) CollaborationEdinburghUK
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Dissociable behavioural signatures of co-existing impulsivity and apathy in decision-making. Sci Rep 2022; 12:21476. [PMID: 36509827 PMCID: PMC9744918 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-022-25882-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/15/2022] [Accepted: 12/06/2022] [Indexed: 12/14/2022] Open
Abstract
Apathy and impulsivity are expressed in a wide range of neuropsychiatric disorders, and, to a less severe extent, in healthy people too. Although traditionally considered to be opposite extremes of a single motivational spectrum, recent epidemiological questionnaire-based data suggest that both traits can in fact co-exist within the same individual. Here, we sought to investigate the relationship between these constructs in healthy people within a controlled task environment that examines the ability to make a decision under temporal uncertainty and measures the vigour of the response. Sixty participants performed a new version of the Traffic Light Task and completed self-report questionnaire measures of apathy and impulsivity. The task required individuals to make rapid decision-making for time-sensitive reward by squeezing a hand-held dynamometer as quickly as possible after a predictable event occurred (a traffic light turning green). Although apathy and impulsivity were positively correlated in questionnaire assessments, the two traits were associated with distinct behavioural signatures on the task. Impulsivity was expressed as an inflexible tendency to generate rapid anticipatory responses, regardless of cost-benefit information. Apathy, on the other hand, was associated with a blunted effect of reward on response vigour. These findings reveal how apathy and impulsivity are related to distinct dimensions of goal-directed behaviour, explaining how these traits might co-exist in the same individuals.
Collapse
|
9
|
Morris L, O'Callaghan C, Le Heron C. Disordered Decision Making: A Cognitive Framework for Apathy and Impulsivity in Huntington's Disease. Mov Disord 2022; 37:1149-1163. [PMID: 35491758 PMCID: PMC9322688 DOI: 10.1002/mds.29013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/16/2021] [Revised: 02/17/2022] [Accepted: 03/15/2022] [Indexed: 01/12/2023] Open
Abstract
A caregiver's all‐too‐familiar narrative ‐ “He doesn't think through what he does, but mostly he does nothing.” Apathy and impulsivity, debilitating and poorly understood, commonly co‐occur in Huntington's disease (HD). HD is a neurodegenerative disease with manifestations bridging clinical neurology and psychiatry. In addition to movement and cognitive symptoms, neurobehavioral disturbances, particularly apathy and impulsivity, are prevalent features of HD, occurring early in the disease course, often worsening with disease progression, and substantially reducing quality of life. Treatments remain limited, in part because of limited mechanistic understanding of these behavioral disturbances. However, emerging work within the field of decision‐making neuroscience and beyond points to common neurobiological mechanisms underpinning these seemingly disparate problems. These insights bridge the gap between underlying disease pathology and clinical phenotype, offering new treatment strategies, novel behavioral and physiological biomarkers of HD, and deeper understanding of human behavior. In this review, we apply the neurobiological framework of cost‐benefit decision making to the problems of apathy and impulsivity in HD. Through this decision‐making lens, we develop a mechanistic model that elucidates the occurrence of these behavioral disturbances and points to potential treatment strategies and crucial research priorities. © 2022 The Authors. Movement Disorders published by Wiley Periodicals LLC on behalf of International Parkinson Movement Disorder Society.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lee‐Anne Morris
- Department of Medicine University of Otago Christchurch New Zealand
- New Zealand Brain Research Institute Christchurch New Zealand
| | - Claire O'Callaghan
- Brain and Mind Centre and School of Medical Sciences, Faculty of Medicine and Health University of Sydney Sydney New South Wales Australia
| | - Campbell Le Heron
- Department of Medicine University of Otago Christchurch New Zealand
- New Zealand Brain Research Institute Christchurch New Zealand
- Department of Neurology Canterbury District Health Board Christchurch New Zealand
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
McLauchlan DJ, Lancaster T, Craufurd D, Linden DEJ, Rosser AE. Different depression: motivational anhedonia governs antidepressant efficacy in Huntington's disease. Brain Commun 2022; 4:fcac278. [PMID: 36440100 PMCID: PMC9683390 DOI: 10.1093/braincomms/fcac278] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/06/2022] [Revised: 04/13/2022] [Accepted: 11/04/2022] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
Abstract
Depression is more common in neurodegenerative diseases such as Huntington's disease than the general population. Antidepressant efficacy is well-established for depression within the general population: a recent meta-analysis showed serotonin norepinephrine reuptake inhibitors, tricyclic antidepressants and mirtazapine outperformed other antidepressants. Despite the severe morbidity, antidepressant choice in Huntington's disease is based on Class IV evidence. We used complementary approaches to determine treatment choice for depression in Huntington's disease: propensity score analyses of antidepressant treatment outcome using the ENROLL-HD data set, and a dissection of the cognitive mechanisms underlying depression in Huntington's disease using a cognitive battery based on the Research Domain Criteria for Depression. Study 1 included ENROLL-HD 5486 gene-positive adult patients started on an antidepressant medication for depression. Our outcome measures were depression (Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale or Problem Behaviours Assessment 'Depressed Mood' item) at first follow-up (primary outcome) and all follow-ups (secondary outcome). The intervention was antidepressant class. We used Svyglm&Twang in R to perform propensity scoring, using known variables (disease progression, medical comorbidity, psychiatric morbidity, sedatives, number of antidepressants, demographics and antidepressant contraindications) to determine the probability of receiving different antidepressants (propensity score) and then included the propensity score in a model of treatment efficacy. Study 2 recruited 51 gene-positive adult patients and 26 controls from the South Wales Huntington's Disease Management Service. Participants completed a motor assessment, in addition to measures of depression and apathy, followed by tasks measuring consummatory anhedonia, motivational anhedonia, learning from reward and punishment and reaction to negative outcome. We used generalised linear models to determine the association between task performance and depression scores. Study 1 showed selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors outperformed serotonin norepinephrine reuptake inhibitors on the primary outcome (P = 0.048), whilst both selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (P = 0.00069) and bupropion (P = 0.0045) were superior to serotonin norepinephrine reuptake inhibitors on the secondary outcome. Study 2 demonstrated an association between depression score and effort for reward that was not explained by apathy. No other mechanisms were associated with depression score. We found that selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors and bupropion outperform serotonin norepinephrine reuptake inhibitors at alleviating depression in Huntington's disease. Moreover, motivational anhedonia appears the most significant mechanism underlying depression in Huntington's disease. Bupropion is improves motivational anhedonia and has a synergistic effect with selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors. This work provides the first large-scale, objective evidence to determine treatment choice for depression in Huntington's disease, and provides a model for determining antidepressant efficacy in other neurodegenerative diseases.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Duncan James McLauchlan
- Division of Psychological Medicine and Clinical Neurosciences, Cardiff University, Cardiff CF24 4HQ, UK.,Department of Neurology, Morriston Hospital, Swansea Bay University Health Board, Swansea SA6 6NL, UK
| | - Thomas Lancaster
- Division of Psychological Medicine and Clinical Neurosciences, Cardiff University, Cardiff CF24 4HQ, UK.,Cardiff University Brain Research Imaging Center, Cardiff University, Cardiff CF24 4HQ, UK.,Department of Psychology, University of Bath, Bath BA2 7AY, UK
| | - David Craufurd
- Manchester Center for Genomic Medicine, Division of Evolution and Genomic Sciences, School of Biological Sciences, Faculty of Biology, Medicine and Health, University of Manchester, Manchester Academic Health Science Center, Manchester M13 9PL, UK.,St. Mary's Hospital, Manchester University NHS Foundation Trust, Manchester Academic Health Science Center, Manchester M13 9WL, UK
| | - David E J Linden
- Division of Psychological Medicine and Clinical Neurosciences, Cardiff University, Cardiff CF24 4HQ, UK.,Cardiff University Brain Research Imaging Center, Cardiff University, Cardiff CF24 4HQ, UK.,Department of Psychology, University of Bath, Bath BA2 7AY, UK.,School for Mental Health and Neuroscience, Fac. Health, Medicine and Life Sciences, Maastricht University, Maastricht, Netherlands
| | - Anne E Rosser
- Division of Psychological Medicine and Clinical Neurosciences, Cardiff University, Cardiff CF24 4HQ, UK.,Department of Neurology, Morriston Hospital, Swansea Bay University Health Board, Swansea SA6 6NL, UK.,School of Biosciences, Cardiff University, Cardiff CF10 3AX, UK
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Saleh Y, Jarratt-Barnham I, Fernandez-Egea E, Husain M. Mechanisms Underlying Motivational Dysfunction in Schizophrenia. Front Behav Neurosci 2021; 15:709753. [PMID: 34566594 PMCID: PMC8460905 DOI: 10.3389/fnbeh.2021.709753] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/14/2021] [Accepted: 08/20/2021] [Indexed: 12/24/2022] Open
Abstract
Negative symptoms are a debilitating feature of schizophrenia which are often resistant to pharmacological intervention. The mechanisms underlying them remain poorly understood, and diagnostic methods rely on phenotyping through validated questionnaires. Deeper endo-phenotyping is likely to be necessary in order to improve current understanding. In the last decade, valuable behavioural insights have been gained through the use of effort-based decision making (EBDM) tasks. These have highlighted impairments in reward-related processing in schizophrenia, particularly associated with negative symptom severity. Neuroimaging investigations have related these changes to dysfunction within specific brain networks including the ventral striatum (VS) and frontal brain regions. Here, we review the behavioural and neural evidence associated with negative symptoms, shedding light on potential underlying mechanisms and future therapeutic possibilities. Findings in the literature suggest that schizophrenia is characterised by impaired reward based learning and action selection, despite preserved hedonic responses. Associations between amotivation and reward-processing deficits have not always been clear, and may be mediated by factors including cognitive dysfunction or dysfunctional or self-defeatist beliefs. Successful endo-phenotyping of negative symptoms as a function of objective behavioural and neural measurements is crucial in advancing our understanding of this complex syndrome. Additionally, transdiagnostic research–leveraging findings from other brain disorders, including neurological ones–can shed valuable light on the possible common origins of motivation disorders across diseases and has important implications for future treatment development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Youssuf Saleh
- Nuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, John Radcliffe Hospital, University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom
| | - Isaac Jarratt-Barnham
- Department of Psychiatry, Herchel Smith Building for Brain & Mind Sciences, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom.,Cambridge Psychosis Centre, Cambridgeshire and Peterborough NHS Foundation Trust, Cambridge, United Kingdom
| | - Emilio Fernandez-Egea
- Department of Psychiatry, Herchel Smith Building for Brain & Mind Sciences, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom.,Cambridge Psychosis Centre, Cambridgeshire and Peterborough NHS Foundation Trust, Cambridge, United Kingdom
| | - Masud Husain
- Nuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, John Radcliffe Hospital, University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Petitet P, Attaallah B, Manohar SG, Husain M. The computational cost of active information sampling before decision-making under uncertainty. Nat Hum Behav 2021; 5:935-946. [PMID: 34045719 DOI: 10.1038/s41562-021-01116-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/16/2020] [Accepted: 04/14/2021] [Indexed: 01/30/2023]
Abstract
Humans often seek information to minimize the pervasive effect of uncertainty on decisions. Current theories explain how much knowledge people should gather before a decision, based on the cost-benefit structure of the problem at hand. Here, we demonstrate that this framework omits a crucial agent-related factor: the cognitive effort expended while collecting information. Using an active sampling model, we unveil a speed-efficiency trade-off whereby more informative samples take longer to find. Crucially, under sufficient time pressure, humans can break this trade-off, sampling both faster and more efficiently. Computational modelling demonstrates the existence of a cost of cognitive effort which, when incorporated into theoretical models, provides a better account of people's behaviour and also relates to self-reported fatigue accumulated during active sampling. Thus, the way people seek knowledge to guide their decisions is shaped not only by task-related costs and benefits, but also crucially by the quantifiable computational costs incurred.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Pierre Petitet
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK.
| | | | - Sanjay G Manohar
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
- Nuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
| | - Masud Husain
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK.
- Nuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK.
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Saleh Y, Le Heron C, Petitet P, Veldsman M, Drew D, Plant O, Schulz U, Sen A, Rothwell PM, Manohar S, Husain M. Apathy in small vessel cerebrovascular disease is associated with deficits in effort-based decision making. Brain 2021; 144:1247-1262. [PMID: 33734344 PMCID: PMC8240747 DOI: 10.1093/brain/awab013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 39] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/01/2020] [Revised: 10/23/2020] [Accepted: 11/04/2020] [Indexed: 11/16/2022] Open
Abstract
Patients with small vessel cerebrovascular disease frequently suffer from apathy, a debilitating neuropsychiatric syndrome, the underlying mechanisms of which remain to be established. Here we investigated the hypothesis that apathy is associated with disrupted decision making in effort-based decision making, and that these alterations are associated with abnormalities in the white matter network connecting brain regions that underpin such decisions. Eighty-two patients with MRI evidence of small vessel disease were assessed using a behavioural paradigm as well as diffusion weighted MRI. The decision-making task involved accepting or rejecting monetary rewards in return for performing different levels of physical effort (hand grip force). Choice data and reaction times were integrated into a drift diffusion model that framed decisions to accept or reject offers as stochastic processes approaching a decision boundary with a particular drift rate. Tract-based spatial statistics were used to assess the relationship between white matter tract integrity and apathy, while accounting for depression. Overall, patients with apathy accepted significantly fewer offers on this decision-making task. Notably, while apathetic patients were less responsive to low rewards, they were also significantly averse to investing in high effort. Significant reductions in white matter integrity were observed to be specifically related to apathy, but not to depression. These included pathways connecting brain regions previously implicated in effort-based decision making in healthy people. The drift rate to decision parameter was significantly associated with both apathy and altered white matter tracts, suggesting that both brain and behavioural changes in apathy are associated with this single parameter. On the other hand, depression was associated with an increase in the decision boundary, consistent with an increase in the amount of evidence required prior to making a decision. These findings demonstrate altered effort-based decision making for reward in apathy, and also highlight dissociable mechanisms underlying apathy and depression in small vessel disease. They provide clear potential brain and behavioural targets for future therapeutic interventions, as well as modelling parameters that can be used to measure the effects of treatment at the behavioural level.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Youssuf Saleh
- Nuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Oxford, Level 6, West Wing, John Radcliffe Hospital, Oxford, OX3 9DU, UK.,Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Anna Watts Building, Radcliffe Observatory Quarter, Oxford, OX2 6GG, UK
| | - Campbell Le Heron
- Nuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Oxford, Level 6, West Wing, John Radcliffe Hospital, Oxford, OX3 9DU, UK.,New Zealand Brain Research Institute, Christchurch 8011, New Zealand.,Department of Medicine, University of Otago, Christchurch 8011, New Zealand
| | - Pierre Petitet
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Anna Watts Building, Radcliffe Observatory Quarter, Oxford, OX2 6GG, UK
| | - Michele Veldsman
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Anna Watts Building, Radcliffe Observatory Quarter, Oxford, OX2 6GG, UK
| | - Daniel Drew
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Anna Watts Building, Radcliffe Observatory Quarter, Oxford, OX2 6GG, UK
| | - Olivia Plant
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Anna Watts Building, Radcliffe Observatory Quarter, Oxford, OX2 6GG, UK
| | - Ursula Schulz
- Nuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Oxford, Level 6, West Wing, John Radcliffe Hospital, Oxford, OX3 9DU, UK
| | - Arjune Sen
- Nuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Oxford, Level 6, West Wing, John Radcliffe Hospital, Oxford, OX3 9DU, UK.,Oxford Epilepsy Research Group, NIHR Biomedical Research Centre, Nuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, John Radcliffe Hospital, Oxford, OX3 9DU, UK
| | - Peter M Rothwell
- Nuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Oxford, Level 6, West Wing, John Radcliffe Hospital, Oxford, OX3 9DU, UK.,Wolfson Centre for Prevention of Stroke and Dementia, Nuffield Dept Clinical Neurosciences, University of Oxford, UK
| | - Sanjay Manohar
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Anna Watts Building, Radcliffe Observatory Quarter, Oxford, OX2 6GG, UK
| | - Masud Husain
- Nuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Oxford, Level 6, West Wing, John Radcliffe Hospital, Oxford, OX3 9DU, UK.,Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Anna Watts Building, Radcliffe Observatory Quarter, Oxford, OX2 6GG, UK.,NIHR Oxford Biomedical Research Centre, Oxford, UK.,Wellcome Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging, University of Oxford, UK
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Muhammed K, Ben Yehuda M, Drew D, Manohar S, Husain M. Reward sensitivity and action in Parkinson's disease patients with and without apathy. Brain Commun 2021; 3:fcab022. [PMID: 33855297 PMCID: PMC8024004 DOI: 10.1093/braincomms/fcab022] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/27/2020] [Revised: 12/09/2020] [Accepted: 01/04/2021] [Indexed: 11/14/2022] Open
Abstract
Clinical apathy results in dysfunction of goal directed behaviour, a key component of which is the initiation of action. Previous work has suggested that blunting of reward sensitivity is an important mechanism underlying apathy. However, an additional component might be impoverished initiation of action itself. This study aims to investigate the link between motivation and motor output and its association with apathy and dopamine. An oculomotor task that measures pupillary and saccadic response to monetary incentives was used to assess reward sensitivity, first in 23 young and 18 elderly controls, and then in 22 patients with Parkinson's disease tested ON and OFF dopaminergic medication. To distinguish between pupillary responses to anticipated reward alone versus responses associated with motor preparation, a saccadic 'go/no-go' task was performed. Half of the trials required a saccade to be initiated to receive a reward and in the remaining trials no action was required but reward was still obtained. No significant difference in pupil response was demonstrated between the two conditions in all groups tested, suggesting pupillary responses to rewards are not contingent upon motor preparation in Parkinson's disease. Being ON or OFF dopamine did not influence this response either. Previous work demonstrated associations between apathy and pupillary reward insensitivity in Parkinson's disease. Here we observed this effect only when an action was required to receive a reward, and only in the ON state. These findings suggest that apathy in Parkinson's disease is linked to reduced reward sensitivity and that this is most prominently observed when actions have to be initiated to rewarding goals, with the effect modulated by being ON dopaminergic medication. OFF medication, there was no such strong relationship, and similarly in the 'no-go' conditions, either ON or OFF dopaminergic drugs. The results provide preliminary data which suggest that apathy in Parkinson's disease is associated with a reduction in reward sensitivity and this is most evident when associated with initiation of goal directed actions in the presence of adequate dopamine.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kinan Muhammed
- Nuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Oxford, Oxford OX3 9DU, UK
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford OX2 6GG, UK
| | - Michael Ben Yehuda
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford OX2 6GG, UK
| | - Daniel Drew
- Nuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Oxford, Oxford OX3 9DU, UK
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford OX2 6GG, UK
| | - Sanjay Manohar
- Nuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Oxford, Oxford OX3 9DU, UK
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford OX2 6GG, UK
| | - Masud Husain
- Nuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Oxford, Oxford OX3 9DU, UK
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford OX2 6GG, UK
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Le Heron C, Kolling N, Plant O, Kienast A, Janska R, Ang YS, Fallon S, Husain M, Apps MAJ. Dopamine Modulates Dynamic Decision-Making during Foraging. J Neurosci 2020; 40:5273-5282. [PMID: 32457071 PMCID: PMC7329313 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.2586-19.2020] [Citation(s) in RCA: 41] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/21/2019] [Revised: 03/10/2020] [Accepted: 04/28/2020] [Indexed: 01/11/2023] Open
Abstract
The mesolimbic dopaminergic system exerts a crucial influence on incentive processing. However, the contribution of dopamine in dynamic, ecological situations where reward rates vary, and decisions evolve over time, remains unclear. In such circumstances, current (foreground) reward accrual needs to be compared continuously with potential rewards that could be obtained by traveling elsewhere (background reward rate), to determine the opportunity cost of staying versus leaving. We hypothesized that dopamine specifically modulates the influence of background, but not foreground, reward information when making a dynamic comparison of these variables for optimal behavior. On a novel foraging task based on an ecological account of animal behavior (marginal value theorem), human participants of either sex decided when to leave locations in situations where foreground rewards depleted at different rates, either in rich or poor environments with high or low background reward rates. In line with theoretical accounts, people's decisions to move from current locations were independently modulated by changes in both foreground and background reward rates. Pharmacological manipulation of dopamine D2 receptor activity using the agonist cabergoline significantly affected decisions to move on, specifically modulating the effect of background reward rates. In particular, when on cabergoline, people left patches in poor environments much earlier. These results demonstrate a role of dopamine in signaling the opportunity cost of rewards, not value per se. Using this ecologically derived framework, we uncover a specific mechanism by which D2 dopamine receptor activity modulates decision-making when foreground and background reward rates are dynamically compared.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Many decisions, across economic, political, and social spheres, involve choices to "leave". Such decisions depend on a continuous comparison of a current location's value, with that of other locations you could move on to. However, how the brain makes such decisions is poorly understood. Here, we developed a computerized task, based around theories of how animals make decisions to move on when foraging for food. Healthy human participants had to decide when to leave collecting financial rewards in a location, and travel to collect rewards elsewhere. Using a pharmacological manipulation, we show that the activity of dopamine in the brain modulates decisions to move on, with people valuing other locations differently depending on their dopaminergic state.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Campbell Le Heron
- Nuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Oxford, Oxford OX39DU, United Kingdom
- New Zealand Brain Research Institute, Christchurch 8011, New Zealand
- Department of Medicine, University of Otago, Christchurch 8011, New Zealand
| | - Nils Kolling
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford OX2 6GG, United Kingdom
- Wellcome Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging, University of Oxford, Oxford OX3 9DU, United Kingdom
| | - Olivia Plant
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford OX2 6GG, United Kingdom
| | - Annika Kienast
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford OX2 6GG, United Kingdom
| | - Rebecca Janska
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford OX2 6GG, United Kingdom
| | - Yuen-Siang Ang
- Nuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Oxford, Oxford OX39DU, United Kingdom
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford OX2 6GG, United Kingdom
| | - Sean Fallon
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford OX2 6GG, United Kingdom
- Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, Bristol BS8 1UD, United Kingdom
| | - Masud Husain
- Nuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Oxford, Oxford OX39DU, United Kingdom
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford OX2 6GG, United Kingdom
- Wellcome Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging, University of Oxford, Oxford OX3 9DU, United Kingdom
| | - Matthew A J Apps
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford OX2 6GG, United Kingdom
- Wellcome Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging, University of Oxford, Oxford OX3 9DU, United Kingdom
- Centre for Human Brain Health, School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, Birmingham, B15 2TT, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Muhammed K, Dalmaijer E, Manohar S, Husain M. Voluntary modulation of saccadic peak velocity associated with individual differences in motivation. Cortex 2020; 122:198-212. [PMID: 30638586 PMCID: PMC6970223 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2018.12.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/23/2018] [Revised: 09/21/2018] [Accepted: 12/04/2018] [Indexed: 12/30/2022]
Abstract
Saccadic peak velocity increases in a stereotyped manner with the amplitude of eye movements. This relationship, known as the main sequence, has classically been considered to be fixed, although several recent studies have demonstrated that velocity can be modulated to some extent by external incentives. However, the ability to voluntarily control saccadic velocity and its association with motivation has yet to be investigated. Here, in three separate experimental paradigms, we measured the effects of incentivisation on saccadic velocity, reaction time and preparatory pupillary changes in 53 young healthy participants. In addition, the ability to voluntarily modulate saccadic velocity with and without incentivisation was assessed. Participants varied in their ability to increase and decrease the velocity of their saccades when instructed to do so. This effect correlated with motivation level across participants, and was further modulated by addition of monetary reward and avoidance of loss. The findings show that a degree of voluntary control of saccadic velocity is possible in some individuals, and that the ability to modulate peak velocity is associated with intrinsic levels of motivation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kinan Muhammed
- Nuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Oxford, UK; Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, UK.
| | - Edwin Dalmaijer
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, UK
| | - Sanjay Manohar
- Nuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Oxford, UK; Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, UK
| | - Masud Husain
- Nuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Oxford, UK; Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, UK
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Sundaresan V, Zamboni G, Le Heron C, Rothwell PM, Husain M, Battaglini M, De Stefano N, Jenkinson M, Griffanti L. Automated lesion segmentation with BIANCA: Impact of population-level features, classification algorithm and locally adaptive thresholding. Neuroimage 2019; 202:116056. [PMID: 31376518 PMCID: PMC6996003 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116056] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/18/2019] [Revised: 06/19/2019] [Accepted: 07/24/2019] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
White matter hyperintensities (WMH) or white matter lesions exhibit high variability in their characteristics both at population- and subject-level, making their detection a challenging task. Population-level factors such as age, vascular risk factors and neurodegenerative diseases affect lesion load and spatial distribution. At the individual level, WMH vary in contrast, amount and distribution in different white matter regions. In this work, we aimed to improve BIANCA, the FSL tool for WMH segmentation, in order to better deal with these sources of variability. We worked on two stages of BIANCA by improving the lesion probability map estimation (classification stage) and making the lesion probability map thresholding stage automated and adaptive to local lesion probabilities. Firstly, in order to take into account the effect of population-level factors, we included population-level lesion probabilities, modelled with respect to a parametric factor (e.g. age), in the classification stage. Secondly, we tested BIANCA performance when using four alternative classifiers commonly used in the literature with respect to K-nearest neighbour algorithm (currently used for lesion probability map estimation in BIANCA). Finally, we propose LOCally Adaptive Threshold Estimation (LOCATE), a supervised method for determining optimal local thresholds to apply to the estimated lesion probability map, as an alternative option to global thresholding (i.e. applying the same threshold to the entire lesion probability map). For these experiments we used data from a neurodegenerative cohort, a vascular cohort and the cohorts available publicly as a part of a segmentation challenge. We observed that including population-level parametric lesion probabilities with respect to age and using alternative machine learning techniques provided negligible improvement. However, LOCATE provided a substantial improvement in the lesion segmentation performance, when compared to the global thresholding. It allowed to detect more deep lesions and provided better segmentation of periventricular lesion boundaries, despite the differences in the lesion spatial distribution and load across datasets. We further validated LOCATE on a cohort of CADASIL (Cerebral autosomal dominant arteriopathy with subcortical infarcts and leukoencephalopathy) patients, a genetic form of cerebral small vessel disease, and healthy controls, showing that LOCATE adapts well to wide variations in lesion load and spatial distribution.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Vaanathi Sundaresan
- Wellcome Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging, Oxford Centre for Functional MRI of the Brain, Nuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Oxford, UK; Oxford-Nottingham Centre for Doctoral Training in Biomedical Imaging, University of Oxford, UK; Oxford India Centre for Sustainable Development, Somerville College, University of Oxford, UK.
| | - Giovanna Zamboni
- Wellcome Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging, Oxford Centre for Functional MRI of the Brain, Nuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Oxford, UK; Centre for Prevention of Stroke and Dementia, Nuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Oxford, UK
| | - Campbell Le Heron
- Nuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK; New Zealand Brain Research Institute, Christchurch 8011, New Zealand
| | - Peter M Rothwell
- Centre for Prevention of Stroke and Dementia, Nuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Oxford, UK
| | - Masud Husain
- Nuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK; Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK; Wellcome Centre for Integrative NeuroImaging, University of Oxford, UK
| | - Marco Battaglini
- Department of Medicine, Surgery and Neuroscience, University of Siena, Siena, Italy
| | - Nicola De Stefano
- Department of Medicine, Surgery and Neuroscience, University of Siena, Siena, Italy
| | - Mark Jenkinson
- Wellcome Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging, Oxford Centre for Functional MRI of the Brain, Nuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Oxford, UK
| | - Ludovica Griffanti
- Wellcome Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging, Oxford Centre for Functional MRI of the Brain, Nuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Oxford, UK
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Dopamine-Dependent Loss Aversion during Effort-Based Decision-Making. J Neurosci 2019; 40:661-670. [PMID: 31727795 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.1760-19.2019] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/23/2019] [Revised: 10/31/2019] [Accepted: 11/01/2019] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
From psychology to economics, there has been substantial interest in how costs (e.g., delay, risk) are represented asymmetrically during decision-making when attempting to gain reward or avoid punishment. For example, in decision-making under risk, individuals show a tendency to prefer to avoid punishment rather than to acquire the equivalent reward (loss aversion). Although the cost of physical effort has recently received significant attention, it remains unclear whether loss aversion exists during effort-based decision-making. On the one hand, loss aversion may be hardwired due to asymmetric evolutionary pressure on losses and gains and therefore exists across decision-making contexts. On the other hand, distinct brain regions are involved with different decision costs, making it questionable whether similar asymmetries exist. Here, we demonstrate that young healthy human participants (females, 16; males, 6) exhibit loss aversion during effort-based decision-making by exerting more physical effort to avoid punishment than to gain a same-size reward. Next, we show that medicated Parkinson's disease (PD) patients (females, 9; males, 9) show a reduction in loss aversion compared with age-matched control subjects (females, 11; males, 9). Behavioral and computational analysis revealed that people with PD exerted similar physical effort in return for a reward but were less willing to produce effort to avoid punishment. Therefore, loss aversion is present during effort-based decision-making and can be modulated by altered dopaminergic state. This finding could have important implications for our understanding of clinical disorders that show a reduced willingness to exert effort in the pursuit of reward.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Loss aversion-preferring to avoid punishment rather than to acquire equivalent reward-is an important concept in decision-making under risk. However, little is known about whether loss aversion also exists during decisions where the cost is physical effort. This is surprising given that motor cost shapes human behavior, and a reduced willingness to exert effort is a characteristic of many clinical disorders. Here, we show that healthy human individuals exert more effort to minimize punishment than to maximize reward (loss aversion). We also demonstrate that medicated Parkinson's disease patients exert similar effort to gain reward but less effort to avoid punishment when compared with healthy age-matched control subjects. This indicates that dopamine-dependent loss aversion is crucial for explaining effort-based decision-making.
Collapse
|
19
|
Iadecola C, Duering M, Hachinski V, Joutel A, Pendlebury ST, Schneider JA, Dichgans M. Vascular Cognitive Impairment and Dementia: JACC Scientific Expert Panel. J Am Coll Cardiol 2019; 73:3326-3344. [PMID: 31248555 PMCID: PMC6719789 DOI: 10.1016/j.jacc.2019.04.034] [Citation(s) in RCA: 464] [Impact Index Per Article: 77.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/21/2019] [Revised: 03/09/2019] [Accepted: 04/23/2019] [Indexed: 02/07/2023]
Abstract
Cognitive impairment associated with aging has emerged as one of the major public health challenges of our time. Although Alzheimer's disease is the leading cause of clinically diagnosed dementia in Western countries, cognitive impairment of vascular etiology is the second most common cause and may be the predominant one in East Asia. Furthermore, alterations of the large and small cerebral vasculature, including those affecting the microcirculation of the subcortical white matter, are key contributors to the clinical expression of cognitive dysfunction caused by other pathologies, including Alzheimer's disease. This scientific expert panel provides a critical appraisal of the epidemiology, pathobiology, neuropathology, and neuroimaging of vascular cognitive impairment and dementia, and of current diagnostic and therapeutic approaches. Unresolved issues are also examined to shed light on new basic and clinical research avenues that may lead to mitigating one of the most devastating human conditions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Costantino Iadecola
- Feil Family Brain and Mind Research Institute, Weill Cornell Medicine, New York, New York.
| | - Marco Duering
- Institute for Stroke and Dementia Research (ISD), University Hospital, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität LMU, Munich, Germany
| | - Vladimir Hachinski
- Department of Clinical Neurological Sciences, Western University, London, Ontario, Canada
| | - Anne Joutel
- Institute of Psychiatry and Neurosciences of Paris, INSERM U1266, Université Paris Descartes, Paris, France
| | - Sarah T Pendlebury
- Centre for Prevention of Stroke and Dementia, Nuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, John Radcliffe Hospital and the University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom
| | - Julie A Schneider
- Rush Alzheimer's Disease Center, Rush University Medical Center, Chicago, Illinois
| | - Martin Dichgans
- Institute for Stroke and Dementia Research (ISD), University Hospital, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität LMU, Munich, Germany; German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE, Munich), Munich, Germany; Munich Cluster for Systems Neurology (SyNergy), Munich, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Le Heron C, Holroyd CB, Salamone J, Husain M. Brain mechanisms underlying apathy. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2019; 90:302-312. [PMID: 30366958 PMCID: PMC6518466 DOI: 10.1136/jnnp-2018-318265] [Citation(s) in RCA: 108] [Impact Index Per Article: 18.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/10/2018] [Revised: 09/13/2018] [Accepted: 09/24/2018] [Indexed: 01/13/2023]
Abstract
The past few decades have seen growing interest in the neuropsychiatric syndrome of apathy, conceptualised as a loss of motivation manifesting as a reduction of goal-directed behaviour. Apathy occurs frequently, and with substantial impact on quality of life, in a broad range of neurological and psychiatric conditions. Apathy is also consistently associated with neuroimaging changes in specific medial frontal cortex and subcortical structures, suggesting that disruption of a common systems-level mechanism may underlie its development, irrespective of the condition that causes it. In parallel with this growing recognition of the clinical importance of apathy, significant advances have been made in understanding normal motivated behaviour in humans and animals. These developments have occurred at several different conceptual levels, from work linking neural structures and neuromodulatory systems to specific aspects of motivated behaviour, to higher order computational models that aim to unite these findings within frameworks for normal goal-directed behaviour. In this review we develop a conceptual framework for understanding pathological apathy based on this current understanding of normal motivated behaviour. We first introduce prominent theories of motivated behaviour-which often involves sequences of actions towards a goal that needs to be maintained across time. Next, we outline the behavioural effects of disrupting these processes in animal models, highlighting the specific effects of these manipulations on different components of motivated behaviour. Finally, we relate these findings to clinical apathy, demonstrating the homologies between this basic neuroscience work and emerging behavioural and physiological evidence from patient studies of this syndrome.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Campbell Le Heron
- Nuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK .,Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK.,New Zealand Brain Research Institute, Christchurch, New Zealand
| | - Clay B Holroyd
- Department of Psychology, University of Victoria, Victoria, British Columbia, Canada
| | - John Salamone
- Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Connecticut, Storrs, Connecticut, USA
| | - Masud Husain
- Nuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK.,Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK.,Division of Clinical Neurology, John Radcliffe Hospital, Oxford University Hospitals Trust, Oxford, UK.,Wellcome Trust Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging, Oxford, UK
| |
Collapse
|