1
|
Trinidad J, Scharf HR, Ngakan PO, Riley EP. Roadside Dining: The Collective Movement Behavior of Sulawesi Moor Macaques in a Provisioning Context. Am J Primatol 2025; 87:e23727. [PMID: 39801015 PMCID: PMC11725772 DOI: 10.1002/ajp.23727] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/01/2024] [Revised: 12/14/2024] [Accepted: 12/22/2024] [Indexed: 01/16/2025]
Abstract
How group-living primates come to a consensus about navigating their environment is a result of their decision-making processes. Although decision-making has been examined in several primate taxa, it remains underexplored for primates living in anthropogenic landscapes. To shed light on consensus decision-making and flexibility in this process, we examined collective movement behavior in a group of wild moor macaques (Macaca maura) experiencing a risk-reward tradeoff as a result of roadside provisioning within Bantimurung Bulusaraung National Park in South Sulawesi, Indonesia. Our goal was to determine whether individual characteristics (e.g., sex, dominance rank, and/or social network centrality) predict the likelihood of initiating a collective movement and if the opportunity to receive food provisions along the road alters these patterns. Using the all-occurrences method, we recorded the location, time, and identity of initiators and followers of each collective movement observed from April to June 2023 (N = 61). We used conditional logistic regression models to examine which individual characteristics predicted initiation overall and based on two destination categories: forest- and road-directed collective movements. Initiation was distributed amongst most of the group, indicating a partially-shared decision-making style. Overall, adult males were more likely to initiate collective movements than adult females. However, for collective movements directed toward the risky roadside, dominance, rather than sex, was a better predictor of initiation, with higher ranked individuals being more likely to initiate collective movements. Examining the decision-making processes in this species through collective movements can provide insight into how primates come to a consensus and the extent to which anthropogenic factors shape these processes. By shedding light on how moor macaques navigate the risk-reward tradeoff at this site, our results can also inform the management of human-macaque interfaces.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Joshua Trinidad
- Department of AnthropologySan Diego State UniversitySan DiegoCaliforniaUSA
| | - Henry R. Scharf
- Department of MathematicsUniversity of ArizonaTucsonArizonaUSA
| | - Putu Oka Ngakan
- Department of ForestryHasanuddin UniversityMakassarIndonesia
| | - Erin P. Riley
- Department of AnthropologySan Diego State UniversitySan DiegoCaliforniaUSA
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Miller RL, Kaser FVE, Belmont RE, Ennis M, Voss KA, Bolt LM, Schreier AL. Mantled howler monkeys (Alouatta palliata) alter activity and spatial cohesion across a continuous forest and forest fragment in Costa Rica. Primates 2025; 66:117-127. [PMID: 39352619 DOI: 10.1007/s10329-024-01160-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/10/2024] [Accepted: 09/23/2024] [Indexed: 01/16/2025]
Abstract
Habitat loss due to deforestation is a primary threat to global biodiversity. Clearing tropical rainforests for agriculture or development leads to forest fragmentation. Forest fragments contain fewer large trees and provide lower food availability for primates compared to continuous forests. Mantled howler monkeys (Alouatta palliata) inhabit fragmented rainforests and may need to alter their activity budgets and spatial cohesion to mitigate competition and conserve energy in fragments where food quality is lower. We compared howler monkey activity and spatial cohesion across a small forest fragment (La Suerte Biological Research Station, LSBRS) and a large, continuous forest (La Selva Research Station) in Costa Rica. We predicted that monkeys at LSBRS would rest more, feed more, travel less, and be less spatially cohesive compared to La Selva to contend with fewer resources in the small fragment. Using instantaneous scan sampling at 2-min intervals during 30-min focal samples, we recorded activity and the number of individuals within 5 m of the focal animal. We collected 1505 h of data from 2017-2024. Monkey activity and spatial cohesion differed significantly across sites. As predicted, monkeys at LSBRS spent more time feeding than at La Selva, but contrary to our predictions, they rested less and traveled more. The mean number of individuals within 5 m was significantly lower at LSBRS compared to La Selva. The ability to modify their activity and spatial cohesion in response to fragmentation provides insight into how primates can contend with fewer resources and higher competition in changing ecosystems worldwide.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Ryan E Belmont
- Department of Biology, Regis University, Denver, CO, USA
| | - Michael Ennis
- Department of Biology, Regis University, Denver, CO, USA
| | | | - Laura M Bolt
- Department of Anthropology, University of Toronto Mississauga, Mississauga, ON, Canada
- The Maderas Rainforest Conservancy, P.O. Box 55-7519, Miami, FL, USA
| | - Amy L Schreier
- Department of Biology, Regis University, Denver, CO, USA.
- The Maderas Rainforest Conservancy, P.O. Box 55-7519, Miami, FL, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Benson JF, Keiter DA, Mahoney PJ, Allen BL, Allen L, Álvares F, Anderson ML, Barber‐Meyer SM, Barocas A, Beasley JC, Behrendorff L, Belant JL, Beyer DE, Boitani L, Borg BL, Boutin S, Boydston EE, Brown JL, Bump JK, Cepek JD, Chamberlain MJ, Chenaux‐Ibrahim YM, Cherry SG, Ćirović D, Ciucci P, Cluff HD, Cooper SM, Crooks KR, Dupont DLJ, Fisher RN, Fortin D, Gable TD, García E, Geffen E, Gehrt SD, Gillingham M, Heard DC, Hebblewhite M, Hinton JW, Homkes AT, Howden CG, Huber D, Jackson PJ, Joly K, Kelly A, Kelly MJ, Kingdon KA, Kulkarni A, Kusak J, Kuzyk GW, Lake BC, Llaneza L, López‐Bao JV, MacNulty DR, McLaren AAD, McLoughlin PD, Merrill EH, Mills KJ, Mitchell N, Moore SA, Mumma MA, Murray MH, Musiani M, Nakamura M, Neilson EW, Neufeld LM, Newsome TM, Oakleaf JK, Palacios V, Perdicas MM, Perry T, Petroelje TR, Piper CB, Prokopenko CM, Prugh LR, Riley SPD, Rio‐Maior H, Roffler GH, Rollins D, Sand H, Schmiegelow FKA, Seip DR, Sorum MS, St. Clair CC, Steenweg R, Strohbach MW, Tatler J, Thaker M, Thompson CA, Turner JW, Vanak AT, Vander Wal E, Wabakken P, Walter SE, Webster SC, Wheeldon TJ, Wikenros C, Windels SK, Young JK, Zabihi‐Seissan S, et alBenson JF, Keiter DA, Mahoney PJ, Allen BL, Allen L, Álvares F, Anderson ML, Barber‐Meyer SM, Barocas A, Beasley JC, Behrendorff L, Belant JL, Beyer DE, Boitani L, Borg BL, Boutin S, Boydston EE, Brown JL, Bump JK, Cepek JD, Chamberlain MJ, Chenaux‐Ibrahim YM, Cherry SG, Ćirović D, Ciucci P, Cluff HD, Cooper SM, Crooks KR, Dupont DLJ, Fisher RN, Fortin D, Gable TD, García E, Geffen E, Gehrt SD, Gillingham M, Heard DC, Hebblewhite M, Hinton JW, Homkes AT, Howden CG, Huber D, Jackson PJ, Joly K, Kelly A, Kelly MJ, Kingdon KA, Kulkarni A, Kusak J, Kuzyk GW, Lake BC, Llaneza L, López‐Bao JV, MacNulty DR, McLaren AAD, McLoughlin PD, Merrill EH, Mills KJ, Mitchell N, Moore SA, Mumma MA, Murray MH, Musiani M, Nakamura M, Neilson EW, Neufeld LM, Newsome TM, Oakleaf JK, Palacios V, Perdicas MM, Perry T, Petroelje TR, Piper CB, Prokopenko CM, Prugh LR, Riley SPD, Rio‐Maior H, Roffler GH, Rollins D, Sand H, Schmiegelow FKA, Seip DR, Sorum MS, St. Clair CC, Steenweg R, Strohbach MW, Tatler J, Thaker M, Thompson CA, Turner JW, Vanak AT, Vander Wal E, Wabakken P, Walter SE, Webster SC, Wheeldon TJ, Wikenros C, Windels SK, Young JK, Zabihi‐Seissan S, Zimmermann B, Patterson BR. Intrinsic and environmental drivers of pairwise cohesion in wild Canis social groups. Ecology 2025; 106:e4492. [PMID: 39663892 PMCID: PMC11739853 DOI: 10.1002/ecy.4492] [Show More Authors] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/22/2024] [Revised: 08/20/2024] [Accepted: 09/30/2024] [Indexed: 12/13/2024]
Abstract
Animals within social groups respond to costs and benefits of sociality by adjusting the proportion of time they spend in close proximity to other individuals in the group (cohesion). Variation in cohesion between individuals, in turn, shapes important group-level processes such as subgroup formation and fission-fusion dynamics. Although critical to animal sociality, a comprehensive understanding of the factors influencing cohesion remains a gap in our knowledge of cooperative behavior in animals. We tracked 574 individuals from six species within the genus Canis in 15 countries on four continents with GPS telemetry to estimate the time that pairs of individuals within social groups spent in close proximity and test hypotheses regarding drivers of cohesion. Pairs of social canids (Canis spp.) varied widely in the proportion of time they spent together (5%-100%) during seasonal monitoring periods relative to both intrinsic characteristics and environmental conditions. The majority of our data came from three species of wolves (gray wolves, eastern wolves, and red wolves) and coyotes. For these species, cohesion within social groups was greatest between breeding pairs and varied seasonally as the nature of cooperative activities changed relative to annual life history patterns. Across species, wolves were more cohesive than coyotes. For wolves, pairs were less cohesive in larger groups, and when suitable, small prey was present reflecting the constraints of food resources and intragroup competition on social associations. Pair cohesion in wolves declined with increased anthropogenic modification of the landscape and greater climatic variability, underscoring challenges for conserving social top predators in a changing world. We show that pairwise cohesion in social groups varies strongly both within and across Canis species, as individuals respond to changing ecological context defined by resources, competition, and anthropogenic disturbance. Our work highlights that cohesion is a highly plastic component of animal sociality that holds significant promise for elucidating ecological and evolutionary mechanisms underlying cooperative behavior.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- John F. Benson
- School of Natural Resources, University of Nebraska‐LincolnLincolnNebraskaUSA
| | - David A. Keiter
- School of Natural Resources, University of Nebraska‐LincolnLincolnNebraskaUSA
- Present address:
Oregon Department of Fish and WildlifeLa GrandeOregonUSA
| | - Peter J. Mahoney
- School of Natural Resources, University of Nebraska‐LincolnLincolnNebraskaUSA
- Department of Wildland Resources and Ecology CenterUtah State UniversityLoganUtahUSA
| | - Benjamin L. Allen
- Institute for Life Sciences and the Environment, University of Southern QueenslandToowoombaQueenslandAustralia
- Centre for African Conservation Ecology, Nelson Mandela UniversityPort ElizabethSouth Africa
| | - Lee Allen
- Department of Agriculture and FisheriesQueensland GovernmentToowoombaAustralia
| | - Francisco Álvares
- CIBIO/InBIO Research Center in Biodiversity and Genetic ResourcesUniversity of PortoVairãoPortugal
- BIOPOLIS Program in Genomics, Biodiversity and Land Planning, CIBIO, Campus de VairãoVairãoPortugal
| | - Morgan L. Anderson
- British Columbia Ministry of Forests Lands, and Natural Resource OperationsPrince GeorgeBritish ColumbiaCanada
| | | | - Adi Barocas
- Hula Research Center, Department of Animal SciencesTel‐Hai CollegeQiryat ShemonaIsrael
- MIGAL‐Galilee Research InstituteKiryat ShmonaIsrael
| | - James C. Beasley
- University of Georgia Savannah River Ecology LaboratoryWarnell School of Forestry and Natural ResourcesAikenSouth CarolinaUSA
| | - Linda Behrendorff
- Department of Environment and ScienceK'gari (Fraser Island)Fraser CoastQueenslandAustralia
| | - Jerrold L. Belant
- Department of Fisheries and WildlifeMichigan State UniversityEast LansingMichiganUSA
| | - Dean E. Beyer
- Department of Fisheries and WildlifeMichigan State UniversityEast LansingMichiganUSA
| | - Luigi Boitani
- Department of Biology and BiotechnologiesUniversity of Rome La SapienzaRomaItaly
| | - Bridget L. Borg
- Denali National Park and Preserve, National Park ServiceDenali ParkAlaskaUSA
| | - Stan Boutin
- Department of Biological SciencesUniversity of AlbertaEdmontonAlbertaCanada
| | | | - Justin L. Brown
- Santa Monica Mountains National Recreation Area, National Park ServiceThousand OaksCaliforniaUSA
| | - Joseph K. Bump
- Department of Fisheries, Wildlife, and Conservation BiologyUniversity of Minnesota, Twin CitiesSaint PaulMinnesotaUSA
| | | | | | | | - Seth G. Cherry
- Parks Canada AgencyRadium Hot SpringsBritish ColumbiaCanada
| | - Duško Ćirović
- Faculty of BiologyUniversity of BelgradeBelgradeSerbia
| | - Paolo Ciucci
- Department of Biology and BiotechnologiesUniversity of Rome La SapienzaRomaItaly
| | - H. Dean Cluff
- Environment and Natural Resources, Government of the Northwest TerritoriesYellowknifeNorthwest TerritoriesCanada
| | | | - Kevin R. Crooks
- Department of Fish, Wildlife, and Conservation BiologyColorado State UniversityFort CollinsColoradoUSA
| | - Daniel L. J. Dupont
- Department of BiologyMemorial University of Newfoundland and LabradorSt. John'sNewfoundland and LabradorCanada
| | - Robert N. Fisher
- Western Ecological Research Center, U.S. Geological SurveySan DiegoCaliforniaUSA
| | - Daniel Fortin
- Department of BiologyUniversité LavalQuebec CityCanada
| | - Thomas D. Gable
- Department of Fisheries, Wildlife, and Conservation BiologyUniversity of MinnesotaSt. PaulMinnesotaUSA
| | - Emilio García
- Biodiversity Research Institute (CSIC – Oviedo University – Principality of Asturias), Oviedo UniversityMieresSpain
| | - Eli Geffen
- School of Zoology, Tel Aviv UniversityTel AvivIsrael
| | - Stanley D. Gehrt
- School of Environment and Natural Resources, The Ohio State UniversityColumbusOhioUSA
| | - Michael Gillingham
- Ecosystem Science and ManagementUniversity of Northern British ColumbiaPrince GeorgeBritish ColumbiaCanada
| | | | - Mark Hebblewhite
- Wildlife Biology Program, WA Franke College of Forestry and Conservation, University of MontanaMissoulaMontanaUSA
| | | | - Austin T. Homkes
- Department of Fisheries, Wildlife, and Conservation BiologyUniversity of MinnesotaSt. PaulMinnesotaUSA
| | | | - Djuro Huber
- Faculty of Veterinary MedicineUniversity of ZagrebZagrebCroatia
| | | | - Kyle Joly
- Yukon‐Charley Rivers National Preserve, National Park ServiceFairbanksAlaskaUSA
| | - Allicia Kelly
- Department of Environment and Natural Resources, Government of the Northwest TerritoriesFort SmithNorthwest TerritoriesCanada
| | - Marcella J. Kelly
- Department of Fish and Wildlife ConservationVirginia Polytechnic and State UniversityBlacksburgVirginiaUSA
| | - Katrien A. Kingdon
- Department of BiologyMemorial University of Newfoundland and LabradorSt. John'sNewfoundland and LabradorCanada
| | - Abhijeet Kulkarni
- Ashoka Trust for Research in Ecology and the EnvironmentBangaloreIndia
| | - Josip Kusak
- Faculty of Veterinary MedicineUniversity of ZagrebZagrebCroatia
| | - Gerald W. Kuzyk
- Ministry of Environment, Government of SaskatchewanMeadow LakeSaskatchewanCanada
| | - Bryce C. Lake
- Yukon Flats National Wildlife Refuge, U.S. Fish and Wildlife ServiceFairbanksAlaskaUSA
| | - Luis Llaneza
- A.RE.NA Asesores en Recursos Naturales S.L.LugoSpain
- Área de Zooloxía, Departamento de Bioloxía, Facultade de CienciasUniversidade da Coruña, Campus da ZapateiraA CoruñaSpain
| | - José Vicente López‐Bao
- Biodiversity Research Institute (CSIC – Oviedo University – Principality of Asturias), Oviedo UniversityMieresSpain
| | - Daniel R. MacNulty
- Department of Wildland Resources and Ecology CenterUtah State UniversityLoganUtahUSA
| | - Ashley A. D. McLaren
- Wildlife Research and Monitoring Section, Ministry of Natural Resources and ForestryPeterboroughOntarioCanada
| | | | - Evelyn H. Merrill
- Department of Biological SciencesUniversity of AlbertaEdmontonAlbertaCanada
| | | | - Numi Mitchell
- The Conservation AgencyJamestownRhode IslandUSA
- Natural Resources ScienceUniversity of Rhode IslandKingstonRhode IslandUSA
| | - Seth A. Moore
- Grand Portage Band of Lake Superior ChippewaGrand PortageMinnesotaUSA
| | | | - Maureen H. Murray
- Department of Biological SciencesUniversity of AlbertaEdmontonAlbertaCanada
- Present address:
Urban Wildlife Institute, Lincoln Park ZooChicagoIllinoisUSA
| | - Marco Musiani
- Dipartimento di Scienze BiologicheGeologiche e Ambientali (BiGeA), Università di BolognaBolognaItaly
| | - Mónia Nakamura
- CIBIO/InBIO Research Center in Biodiversity and Genetic ResourcesUniversity of PortoVairãoPortugal
| | | | - Lalenia M. Neufeld
- Department of Renewable ResourcesUniversity of AlbertaEdmontonAlbertaCanada
- Present address:
Parks Canada Agency, Jasper National ParkJasperAlbertaCanada
| | - Thomas M. Newsome
- School of Life and Environmental Sciences, The University of SydneySydneyNew South WalesAustralia
| | | | - Vicente Palacios
- Association for the Conservation of Nature in Human EnvironmentsPaternaSpain
| | | | - Thomas Perry
- Prince Albert National Park, Parks CanadaWaskesiu LakeSaskatchewanCanada
| | - Tyler R. Petroelje
- Wildlife DivisionMichigan Department of Natural ResourcesMarquetteMichiganUSA
| | | | - Christina M. Prokopenko
- Department of BiologyMemorial University of Newfoundland and LabradorSt. John'sNewfoundland and LabradorCanada
| | - Laura R. Prugh
- School of Environmental and Forest Sciences, University of WashingtonSeattleWashingtonUSA
| | - Seth P. D. Riley
- Santa Monica Mountains National Recreation Area, National Park ServiceThousand OaksCaliforniaUSA
| | - Helena Rio‐Maior
- CIBIO/InBIO Research Center in Biodiversity and Genetic ResourcesUniversity of PortoVairãoPortugal
| | - Gretchen H. Roffler
- Division of Wildlife ConservationAlaska Department of Fish and GameDouglasAlaskaUSA
| | - Dale Rollins
- Rolling Plains Quail Research FoundationRobyTexasUSA
| | - Håkan Sand
- Grimsö Wildlife Research Station, Department of EcologySwedish University of Agricultural SciencesUppsalaSweden
| | | | - Dale R. Seip
- British Columbia Ministry of EnvironmentPrince GeorgeBritish ColumbiaCanada
| | - Mathew S. Sorum
- Yukon‐Charley Rivers National Preserve, National Park ServiceFairbanksAlaskaUSA
| | | | - Robin Steenweg
- Canadian Wildlife Service, Environment and Climate Change CanadaKelownaBritish ColumbiaCanada
| | - Michael W. Strohbach
- Technische Universität Braunschweig, Landscape Ecology and Environmental Systems AnalysisInstitute of GeoecologyBraunschweigGermany
| | - Jack Tatler
- School of Biological Sciences, The University of AdelaideNorth TerraceSouth AustraliaAustralia
| | - Maria Thaker
- Centre for Ecological Sciences, Indian Institute of ScienceBengaluruIndia
| | - Connor A. Thompson
- Environmental and Life Sciences Graduate ProgramTrent UniversityPeterboroughOntarioCanada
| | - Julie W. Turner
- Department of BiologyMemorial University of Newfoundland and LabradorSt. John'sNewfoundland and LabradorCanada
| | - Abi T. Vanak
- Wellcome Trust/Department of Biotechnology (DBT) India Alliance Program, Kaushik SocietyHyderabadIndia
- School of Life Sciences, University of KwaZulu‐NatalDurbanSouth Africa
| | - Eric Vander Wal
- Department of BiologyMemorial University of Newfoundland and LabradorSt. John'sNewfoundland and LabradorCanada
| | - Petter Wabakken
- Faculty of Applied Ecology, Agricultural Sciences and BiotechnologyInland Norway University of Applied SciencesKoppangNorway
| | | | - Sarah C. Webster
- University of Georgia Savannah River Ecology LaboratoryWarnell School of Forestry and Natural ResourcesAikenSouth CarolinaUSA
| | - Tyler J. Wheeldon
- Wildlife Research and Monitoring Section, Ministry of Natural Resources and ForestryPeterboroughOntarioCanada
| | - Camilla Wikenros
- Grimsö Wildlife Research Station, Department of EcologySwedish University of Agricultural SciencesUppsalaSweden
| | - Steve K. Windels
- Voyageurs National Park, US National Park ServiceInternational FallsMinnesotaUSA
| | - Julie K. Young
- Department of Wildland Resources and Ecology CenterUtah State UniversityLoganUtahUSA
| | - Sana Zabihi‐Seissan
- Department of BiologyMemorial University of Newfoundland and LabradorSt. John'sNewfoundland and LabradorCanada
| | - Barbara Zimmermann
- Faculty of Applied Ecology, Agricultural Sciences and BiotechnologyInland Norway University of Applied SciencesKoppangNorway
| | - Brent R. Patterson
- Wildlife Research and Monitoring Section, Ministry of Natural Resources and ForestryPeterboroughOntarioCanada
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Fürtbauer I, Shergold C, Christensen C, Bracken AM, Heistermann M, Papadopoulou M, O'Riain MJ, King AJ. Linking energy availability, movement and sociality in a wild primate ( Papio ursinus). Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2024; 379:20220466. [PMID: 39463242 PMCID: PMC11513646 DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2022.0466] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/01/2024] [Revised: 05/23/2024] [Accepted: 08/20/2024] [Indexed: 10/29/2024] Open
Abstract
Proximate mechanisms of 'social ageing', i.e. shifts in social activity and narrowing of social networks, are understudied. It is proposed that energetic deficiencies (which are often seen in older individuals) may restrict movement and, in turn, sociality, but empirical tests of these intermediary mechanisms are lacking. Here, we study wild chacma baboons (Papio ursinus), combining measures of faecal triiodothyronine (fT3), a non-invasive proxy for energy availability, high-resolution GPS data (movement and social proximity) and accelerometry (social grooming durations). Higher (individual mean-centred) fT3 was associated with increased residency time (i.e. remaining in the same area longer), which, in turn, was positively related to social opportunities (i.e. close physical proximity). Individuals with more frequent social opportunities received more grooming, whereas for grooming given, fT3 moderated this effect, suggesting an energetic cost of giving grooming. While our results support the spirit of the energetic deficiencies hypothesis, the directionality of the relationship between energy availability and movement is unexpected and suggests that lower-energy individuals may use strategies to reduce the costs of intermittent locomotion. Thus, future work should consider whether age-related declines in sociality may be a by-product of a strategy to conserve energy.This article is part of the discussion meeting issue 'Understanding age and society using natural populations'.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ines Fürtbauer
- Biosciences, Faculty of Science and Engineering, Swansea University, SwanseaSA2 8PP, UK
| | - Chloe Shergold
- Biosciences, Faculty of Science and Engineering, Swansea University, SwanseaSA2 8PP, UK
| | - Charlotte Christensen
- Department of Evolutionary Biology and Environmental Science, University of Zurich, Zurich8057, Switzerland
| | - Anna M. Bracken
- School of Biodiversity, One Health and Veterinary Medicine, University of Glasgow, GlasgowG12 8QQ, UK
| | | | - Marina Papadopoulou
- Biosciences, Faculty of Science and Engineering, Swansea University, SwanseaSA2 8PP, UK
| | - M. Justin O'Riain
- Institute for Communities and Wildlife in Africa, Biological Sciences Department, University of Cape Town, Cape Town, Rondebosch7701, South Africa
| | - Andrew J. King
- Biosciences, Faculty of Science and Engineering, Swansea University, SwanseaSA2 8PP, UK
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Gaynor KM, Abrahms B, Manlove KR, Oestreich WK, Smith JA. Anthropogenic impacts at the interface of animal spatial and social behaviour. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2024; 379:20220527. [PMID: 39230457 PMCID: PMC11449167 DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2022.0527] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/16/2023] [Revised: 04/15/2024] [Accepted: 04/25/2024] [Indexed: 09/05/2024] Open
Abstract
Human disturbance is contributing to widespread, global changes in the distributions and densities of wild animals. These anthropogenic impacts on wildlife arise from multiple bottom-up and top-down pathways, including habitat loss, resource provisioning, climate change, pollution, infrastructure development, hunting and our direct presence. Animal behaviour is an important mechanism linking these disturbances to population outcomes, although these behavioural pathways are often complex and can remain obscured when different aspects of behaviour are studied in isolation from one another. The spatial-social interface provides a lens for understanding how an animal's spatial and social environments interact to determine its spatial and social phenotype (i.e. measurable characteristics of an individual), and how these phenotypes interact and feed back to reshape environments. Here, we review studies of animal behaviour at the spatial-social interface to understand and predict how human disturbance affects animal movement, distribution and intraspecific interactions, with consequences for the conservation of populations and ecosystems. By understanding the spatial-social mechanisms linking human disturbance to conservation outcomes, we can better design management interventions to mitigate undesired consequences of disturbance.This article is part of the theme issue 'The spatial-social interface: a theoretical and empirical integration'.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kaitlyn M Gaynor
- Departments of Zoology and Botany, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia V6T 1Z4, Canada
| | - Briana Abrahms
- Center for Ecosystem Sentinels, Department of Biology, University of Washington, Seattle, WA 98195, USA
| | - Kezia R Manlove
- Department of Wildland Resources, Utah State University, Logan, UT 84322, USA
| | | | - Justine A Smith
- Department of Wildlife Fish, and Conservation Biology, University of California Davis, Davis, CA 95616, USA
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Shelton DS, Suriyampola PS, Dinges ZM, Glaholt SP, Shaw JR, Martins EP. Plants buffer some of the effects of a pair of cadmium-exposed zebrafish on the un-exposed majority. ENVIRONMENTAL TOXICOLOGY AND PHARMACOLOGY 2024; 107:104419. [PMID: 38508506 PMCID: PMC11042042 DOI: 10.1016/j.etap.2024.104419] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/22/2023] [Revised: 03/10/2024] [Accepted: 03/14/2024] [Indexed: 03/22/2024]
Abstract
Certain individuals have a disproportionate effect on group responses. Characteristics may include susceptibility to pollutants, such as cadmium (Cd), a potent trace metal. Here, we show how a pair of Cd-exposed individuals can impact the behavior of unexposed groups. We used behavioral assessments to characterize the extent of the effects of the Cd-exposed individuals on group boldness, cohesion, foraging, activity, and responses to plants. We found that groups with a pair of Cd-exposed fish remained closer to novel stimuli and plants than did groups with untreated (control) fish. The presence of plants reduced Cd-induced differences in shoal cohesion and delays feeding in male shoals. Shoals with Cd- and water-treated fish were equally active. The results suggest that fish acutely exposed to environmentally relevant Cd concentrations can have profound effects on the un-exposed majority. However, the presence of plants may mitigate the effects of contaminants on some aspects of social behavior.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Delia S Shelton
- Department of Biology, University of Miami, 1301 Memorial Dr, Coral Gables, FL 33134, USA.
| | - Piyumika S Suriyampola
- School of Life Sciences, Arizona State University, 427 East Tyler Mall, Tempe, AZ 85287, USA
| | - Zoe M Dinges
- Department of Biology, Indiana University, 1001 E 3rd St, Bloomington, IN 47405, USA
| | - Stephen P Glaholt
- O'Neill School of Public and Environmental Affairs, Indiana University, 1315 E 10th St, Bloomington, IN 47405, USA
| | - Joseph R Shaw
- O'Neill School of Public and Environmental Affairs, Indiana University, 1315 E 10th St, Bloomington, IN 47405, USA
| | - Emília P Martins
- School of Life Sciences, Arizona State University, 427 East Tyler Mall, Tempe, AZ 85287, USA
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Papageorgiou D, Nyaguthii B, Farine DR. Compromise or choose: shared movement decisions in wild vulturine guineafowl. Commun Biol 2024; 7:95. [PMID: 38218910 PMCID: PMC10787764 DOI: 10.1038/s42003-024-05782-w] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/31/2023] [Accepted: 01/04/2024] [Indexed: 01/15/2024] Open
Abstract
Shared-decision making is beneficial for the maintenance of group-living. However, little is known about whether consensus decision-making follows similar processes across different species. Addressing this question requires robust quantification of how individuals move relative to each other. Here we use high-resolution GPS-tracking of two vulturine guineafowl (Acryllium vulturinum) groups to test the predictions from a classic theoretical model of collective motion. We show that, in both groups, all individuals can successfully initiate directional movements, although males are more likely to be followed than females. When multiple group members initiate simultaneously, follower decisions depend on directional agreement, with followers compromising directions if the difference between them is small or choosing the majority direction if the difference is large. By aligning with model predictions and replicating the findings of a previous field study on olive baboons (Papio anubis), our results suggest that a common process governs collective decision-making in moving animal groups.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Danai Papageorgiou
- University of Zurich, Department of Evolutionary Biology and Environmental Studies, Winterthurerstrasse 190, 8057, Zurich, Switzerland.
- Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior, Department of Collective Behavior, Universitätsstraße 10, Konstanz, 78457, Germany.
- University of Konstanz, Department of Biology, Universitätsstraße 10, Konstanz, 78457, Germany.
- Kenya Wildlife Service, P.O. Box 40241-001000, Nairobi, Kenya.
- Wissenschaftskolleg zu Berlin, College for Life Sciences, Wallotstrasse 19, Berlin, 14193, Germany.
| | - Brendah Nyaguthii
- University of Eldoret, School of Natural Resource Management, Department of Wildlife, 1125-30100, Eldoret, Kenya
- Mpala Research Centre, P.O. Box 92, Nanyuki, 10400, Kenya
- National Museums of Kenya, Department of Ornithology, P.O. Box 40658-001000, Nairobi, Kenya
| | - Damien R Farine
- University of Zurich, Department of Evolutionary Biology and Environmental Studies, Winterthurerstrasse 190, 8057, Zurich, Switzerland.
- Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior, Department of Collective Behavior, Universitätsstraße 10, Konstanz, 78457, Germany.
- National Museums of Kenya, Department of Ornithology, P.O. Box 40658-001000, Nairobi, Kenya.
- Australian National University, Division of Ecology and Evolution, Research School of Biology, 46 Sullivans Creek Road, Canberra, ACT, 2600, Australia.
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Papadopoulou M, Fürtbauer I, O'Bryan LR, Garnier S, Georgopoulou DG, Bracken AM, Christensen C, King AJ. Dynamics of collective motion across time and species. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2023; 378:20220068. [PMID: 36802781 PMCID: PMC9939269 DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2022.0068] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/31/2022] [Accepted: 11/17/2022] [Indexed: 02/21/2023] Open
Abstract
Most studies of collective animal behaviour rely on short-term observations, and comparisons of collective behaviour across different species and contexts are rare. We therefore have a limited understanding of intra- and interspecific variation in collective behaviour over time, which is crucial if we are to understand the ecological and evolutionary processes that shape collective behaviour. Here, we study the collective motion of four species: shoals of stickleback fish (Gasterosteus aculeatus), flocks of homing pigeons (Columba livia), a herd of goats (Capra aegagrus hircus) and a troop of chacma baboons (Papio ursinus). First, we describe how local patterns (inter-neighbour distances and positions), and group patterns (group shape, speed and polarization) during collective motion differ across each system. Based on these, we place data from each species within a 'swarm space', affording comparisons and generating predictions about the collective motion across species and contexts. We encourage researchers to add their own data to update the 'swarm space' for future comparative work. Second, we investigate intraspecific variation in collective motion over time and provide guidance for researchers on when observations made over different time scales can result in confident inferences regarding species collective motion. This article is part of a discussion meeting issue 'Collective behaviour through time'.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marina Papadopoulou
- Biosciences, School of Biosciences, Geography and Physics, Faculty of Science and Engineering, Swansea University, SA2 8PP Swansea, UK
| | - Ines Fürtbauer
- Biosciences, School of Biosciences, Geography and Physics, Faculty of Science and Engineering, Swansea University, SA2 8PP Swansea, UK
| | - Lisa R. O'Bryan
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Rice University, Houston, TX 77005, USA
| | - Simon Garnier
- Department of Biological Sciences, New Jersey Institute of Technology, Newark, NJ 07102, USA
| | - Dimitra G. Georgopoulou
- Biosciences, School of Biosciences, Geography and Physics, Faculty of Science and Engineering, Swansea University, SA2 8PP Swansea, UK
- Institute of Marine Biology, Biotechnology & Aquaculture, HCMR, 71500 Hersonissos, Crete, Greece
| | - Anna M. Bracken
- Biosciences, School of Biosciences, Geography and Physics, Faculty of Science and Engineering, Swansea University, SA2 8PP Swansea, UK
- School of Biodiversity, One Health and Veterinary Medicine, Graham Kerr Building, Glasgow G12 8QQ, UK
| | - Charlotte Christensen
- Biosciences, School of Biosciences, Geography and Physics, Faculty of Science and Engineering, Swansea University, SA2 8PP Swansea, UK
- Department of Evolutionary Biology and Environmental Studies, University of Zurich, 8057 Zürich, Switzerland
| | - Andrew J. King
- Biosciences, School of Biosciences, Geography and Physics, Faculty of Science and Engineering, Swansea University, SA2 8PP Swansea, UK
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Ogino M, Strauss ED, Farine DR. Challenges of mismatching timescales in longitudinal studies of collective behaviour. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2023; 378:20220064. [PMID: 36802775 PMCID: PMC9939264 DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2022.0064] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/01/2022] [Accepted: 11/11/2022] [Indexed: 02/21/2023] Open
Abstract
How individuals' prior experience and population evolutionary history shape emergent patterns in animal collectives remains a major gap in the study of collective behaviour. One reason for this is that the processes that can shape individual contributions to collective actions can happen over very different timescales from each other and from the collective actions themselves, resulting in mismatched timescales. For example, a preference to move towards a specific patch might arise from phenotype, memory or physiological state. Although providing critical context to collective actions, bridging different timescales remains conceptually and methodologically challenging. Here, we briefly outline some of these challenges, and discuss existing approaches that have already generated insights into the factors shaping individual contributions in animal collectives. We then explore a case study of mismatching timescales-defining relevant group membership-by combining fine-scaled GPS tracking data and daily field census data from a wild population of vulturine guineafowl (Acryllium vulturinum). We show that applying different temporal definitions can produce different assignments of individuals into groups. These assignments can then have consequences when determining individuals' social history, and thus the conclusions we might draw on the impacts of the social environment on collective actions. This article is part of a discussion meeting issue 'Collective behaviour through time'.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mina Ogino
- Department of Evolutionary and Environmental Studies, University of Zurich, Winterthurerstrasse 190, 8057 Zurich, Switzerland
- Department of Collective Behaviour, Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior, Am Obstberg 1, 78315 Radolfzell, Germany
| | - Eli D. Strauss
- Department of Collective Behaviour, Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior, Am Obstberg 1, 78315 Radolfzell, Germany
- Centre for the Advanced Study of Collective Behaviour, University of Konstanz, Universitatsstrasse 10, 78464 Konstanz, Germany
- Department of Integrative Biology, Michigan State University, 104 Natural Science Building, East Lansing, MI 48824-1115, East Lansing, MI 48824, USA
| | - Damien R. Farine
- Department of Evolutionary and Environmental Studies, University of Zurich, Winterthurerstrasse 190, 8057 Zurich, Switzerland
- Department of Collective Behaviour, Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior, Am Obstberg 1, 78315 Radolfzell, Germany
- Division of Ecology and Evolution, Research School of Biology, Australian National University, 46 Sullivans Creek Road, Canberra, ACT 2600, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Averly B, Sridhar VH, Demartsev V, Gall G, Manser M, Strandburg-Peshkin A. Disentangling influence over group speed and direction reveals multiple patterns of influence in moving meerkat groups. Sci Rep 2022; 12:13844. [PMID: 35974046 PMCID: PMC9381760 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-022-17259-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/10/2022] [Accepted: 07/22/2022] [Indexed: 11/25/2022] Open
Abstract
Animals that travel together in groups must constantly come to consensus about both the direction and speed of movement, often simultaneously. Contributions to collective decisions may vary among group members, yet inferring who has influence over group decisions is challenging, largely due to the multifaceted nature of influence. Here we collected high-resolution GPS data from five habituated meerkat groups in their natural habitat during foraging and developed a method to quantify individual influence over both group direction and speed. We find that individual influence over direction and speed are correlated, but also exhibit substantial variation. Comparing patterns across social statuses reveals that dominant females have higher influence than other individuals over both group direction and speed. Individuals with high influence also tend to spend more time in the front of the group. We discuss our results in light of meerkat life-history and current literature on influence during group movement. Our method provides a general approach which can be applied to disentangle individual influence over group direction and speed in a wide range of species with cohesive movement, emphasizing the importance of integrating multiple lines of inquiry when inferring influence in moving animal groups.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Baptiste Averly
- Department for the Ecology of Animal Societies, Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior, Constance, Germany.,Department of Biology, University of Konstanz, Constance, Germany.,Centre for the Advanced Study of Collective Behaviour, University of Konstanz, Constance, Germany.,Kalahari Meerkat Project, Kuruman River Reserve, Northern Cape, South Africa
| | - Vivek H Sridhar
- Department for the Ecology of Animal Societies, Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior, Constance, Germany.,Department of Biology, University of Konstanz, Constance, Germany.,Centre for the Advanced Study of Collective Behaviour, University of Konstanz, Constance, Germany
| | - Vlad Demartsev
- Department for the Ecology of Animal Societies, Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior, Constance, Germany.,Department of Biology, University of Konstanz, Constance, Germany.,Centre for the Advanced Study of Collective Behaviour, University of Konstanz, Constance, Germany.,Kalahari Meerkat Project, Kuruman River Reserve, Northern Cape, South Africa.,Department of Evolutionary Biology and Environmental Studies, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
| | - Gabriella Gall
- Department for the Ecology of Animal Societies, Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior, Constance, Germany.,Department of Biology, University of Konstanz, Constance, Germany.,Centre for the Advanced Study of Collective Behaviour, University of Konstanz, Constance, Germany.,Zukunftskolleg, University of Konstanz, Constance, Germany
| | - Marta Manser
- Kalahari Meerkat Project, Kuruman River Reserve, Northern Cape, South Africa.,Department of Evolutionary Biology and Environmental Studies, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland.,Mammal Research Institute, University of Pretoria, Pretoria, South Africa
| | - Ariana Strandburg-Peshkin
- Department for the Ecology of Animal Societies, Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior, Constance, Germany. .,Department of Biology, University of Konstanz, Constance, Germany. .,Centre for the Advanced Study of Collective Behaviour, University of Konstanz, Constance, Germany. .,Kalahari Meerkat Project, Kuruman River Reserve, Northern Cape, South Africa. .,Department of Evolutionary Biology and Environmental Studies, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland.
| |
Collapse
|