1
|
De Moor D, Skelton M, MacaqueNet, Amici F, Arlet ME, Balasubramaniam KN, Ballesta S, Berghänel A, Berman CM, Bernstein SK, Bhattacharjee D, Bliss‐Moreau E, Brotcorne F, Butovskaya M, Campbell LAD, Carosi M, Chatterjee M, Cooper MA, Cowl VB, De la O C, De Marco A, Dettmer AM, Dhawale AK, Erinjery JJ, Evans CL, Fischer J, García‐Nisa I, Giraud G, Hammer R, Hansen MF, Holzner A, Kaburu S, Konečná M, Kumara HN, Larrivaz M, Leca J, Legrand M, Lehmann J, Li J, Lezé A, MacIntosh A, Majolo B, Maréchal L, Marty PR, Massen JJM, Maulany RI, McCowan B, McFarland R, Merieau P, Meunier H, Micheletta J, Mishra PS, Sah SAM, Molesti S, Morrow KS, Müller‐Klein N, Ngakan PO, Palagi E, Petit O, Pflüger LS, di Sorrentino EP, Raghaven R, Raimbault G, Ram S, Reichard UH, Riley EP, Rincon AV, Ruppert N, Sadoughi B, Santhosh K, Schino G, Sheeran LK, Silk JB, Singh M, Sinha A, Sosa S, Stribos MS, Sueur C, Tiddi B, Tkaczynski PJ, Trebouet F, Widdig A, Whitehouse J, Wooddell LJ, Xia D, von Fersen L, Young C, Schülke O, Ostner J, Neumann C, Duboscq J, Brent LJN. MacaqueNet: Advancing comparative behavioural research through large-scale collaboration. J Anim Ecol 2025; 94:519-534. [PMID: 39934999 PMCID: PMC11962231 DOI: 10.1111/1365-2656.14223] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/31/2024] [Accepted: 10/29/2024] [Indexed: 02/13/2025]
Abstract
There is a vast and ever-accumulating amount of behavioural data on individually recognised animals, an incredible resource to shed light on the ecological and evolutionary drivers of variation in animal behaviour. Yet, the full potential of such data lies in comparative research across taxa with distinct life histories and ecologies. Substantial challenges impede systematic comparisons, one of which is the lack of persistent, accessible and standardised databases. Big-team approaches to building standardised databases offer a solution to facilitating reliable cross-species comparisons. By sharing both data and expertise among researchers, these approaches ensure that valuable data, which might otherwise go unused, become easier to discover, repurpose and synthesise. Additionally, such large-scale collaborations promote a culture of sharing within the research community, incentivising researchers to contribute their data by ensuring their interests are considered through clear sharing guidelines. Active communication with the data contributors during the standardisation process also helps avoid misinterpretation of the data, ultimately improving the reliability of comparative databases. Here, we introduce MacaqueNet, a global collaboration of over 100 researchers (https://macaquenet.github.io/) aimed at unlocking the wealth of cross-species data for research on macaque social behaviour. The MacaqueNet database encompasses data from 1981 to the present on 61 populations across 14 species and is the first publicly searchable and standardised database on affiliative and agonistic animal social behaviour. We describe the establishment of MacaqueNet, from the steps we took to start a large-scale collective, to the creation of a cross-species collaborative database and the implementation of data entry and retrieval protocols. We share MacaqueNet's component resources: an R package for data standardisation, website code, the relational database structure, a glossary and data sharing terms of use. With all these components openly accessible, MacaqueNet can act as a fully replicable template for future endeavours establishing large-scale collaborative comparative databases.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Delphine De Moor
- School of Psychology, Centre for Research in Animal BehaviourUniversity of ExeterExeterUK
| | - Macaela Skelton
- School of Psychology, Centre for Research in Animal BehaviourUniversity of ExeterExeterUK
| | | | - Federica Amici
- Research Group for Human Biology and Primate Cognition, Biology InstituteUniversity of LeipzigLeipzigGermany
- Department of Comparative Cultural PsychologyMax Planck Institute for Evolutionary AnthropologyLeipzigGermany
| | - Malgorzata E. Arlet
- Faculty of Biology, Institute of Human Biology and EvolutionAdam Mickiewicz UniversityPoznanPoland
| | - Krishna N. Balasubramaniam
- Faculty of Science & Engineering, School of Life SciencesAnglia Ruskin UniversityCambridgeUK
- Department of Population Health & Reproduction, School of Veterinary MedicineUniversity of California at DavisDavisCaliforniaUSA
| | - Sébastien Ballesta
- Laboratoire de Neurosciences Cognitives et AdaptativesStrasbourgFrance
- Centre de Primatologie de l'Université de StrasbourgNiederhausbergenFrance
| | - Andreas Berghänel
- Domestication Lab, Department of Interdisciplinary Life Sciences, Konrad Lorenz Institute of EthologyUniversity of Veterinary Medicine ViennaViennaAustria
| | - Carol M. Berman
- Program in Evolution, Ecology and Behavior, Department of AnthropologyUniversity at BuffaloBuffaloNew YorkUSA
| | - Sofia K. Bernstein
- Division of Natural Sciences, Engineering and Mathematics, Department of BiologyUniversity of St. ThomasHoustonTexasUSA
| | - Debottam Bhattacharjee
- Animal Behavior & Cognition, Department of BiologyUtrecht UniversityUtrechtThe Netherlands
| | - Eliza Bliss‐Moreau
- Department of Psychology and the California National Primate Research CenterUniversity of California at DavisDavisCaliforniaUSA
| | - Fany Brotcorne
- Primatology Research Group, Research Unit SPHERESThe University of LiègeLiègeBelgium
| | - Marina Butovskaya
- Institute of Ethnology and AnthropologyRussian Academy of SciencesMoscowRussia
| | - Liz A. D. Campbell
- Wildlife Conservation Research Unit (WildCRU)University of OxfordOxfordUK
| | | | - Mayukh Chatterjee
- Conservation Science and OutreachNorth of England Zoological SocietyCheshireUK
- Animal Behaviour and Cognition Programme, National Institute of Advanced StudiesIndian Institute of Science CampusBengaluruIndia
| | - Matthew A. Cooper
- Department of PsychologyUniversity of Tennessee KnoxvilleKnoxvilleTennesseeUSA
| | | | - Claudio De la O
- FES ZaragozaNational Autonomous University of MexicoMexico CityMexico
- National Institute of Psychiatry Ramón de la Fuente MuñizMexico CityMexico
- School of PsychologyMexico CityMexico
| | | | - Amanda M. Dettmer
- Yale Child Study CenterYale School of MedicineNew HavenConnecticutUSA
| | - Ashni K. Dhawale
- Animal Behaviour and Cognition ProgrammeNational Institute of Advanced StudiesBengaluruIndia
| | | | - Cara L. Evans
- Department of Anthropology, Durham Cultural Evolution Research CentreDurham UniversityDurhamUK
| | - Julia Fischer
- Cognitive Ethology LabGerman Primate CenterGöttingenGermany
- Department for Primate CognitionGeorg‐August‐University GöttingenGöttingenGermany
- Primate CognitionGöttingenGermany
| | | | - Gwennan Giraud
- Primatology Research Group, Research Unit SPHERESThe University of LiègeLiègeBelgium
| | - Roy Hammer
- Department of Behavioral and Cognitive BiologyUniversity of ViennaViennaAustria
| | - Malene F. Hansen
- Department of AnthropologyPrinceton UniversityPrincetonNew JerseyUSA
- The Long‐Tailed Macaque ProjectSorøDenmark
- Behavioural Ecology Group, Department of BiologyUniveristy of CopenhagenCopenhagenDenmark
- Oxford Wildlife Trade Research Trade GroupOxford Brookes UniversityOxfordUK
| | - Anna Holzner
- Behavioural Ecology Research Group, Institute of BiologyUniversity of LeipzigLeipzigGermany
- Department of Human Behavior, Ecology and CultureMax Planck Institute for Evolutionary AnthropologyLeipzigGermany
- School of Biological SciencesUniversiti Sains MalaysiaGelugorMalaysia
| | - Stefano Kaburu
- School of Animal, Rural and Environmental SciencesNottingham Trent UniversitySouthwellUK
| | - Martina Konečná
- Department of Zoology, Faculty of ScienceUniversity of South BohemiaČeské BudějoviceCzech Republic
| | | | - Marine Larrivaz
- Départment d'Anthropologie, Faculté des Arts et des SciencesUdemMontréalQuebecCanada
| | | | - Mathieu Legrand
- Centre de Primatologie de l'Université de StrasbourgNiederhausbergenFrance
| | | | - Jin‐Hua Li
- International Collaborative Research Center for Huangshan Biodiversity and Tibetan Macaque Behavioral Ecology, School of Resource and Environmental EngineeringAnhui UniversityHefeiChina
- School of Resource and Environmental EngineeringAnhui UniversityHefeiChina
| | | | | | | | | | | | - Jorg J. M. Massen
- Animal Behavior & Cognition, Department of BiologyUtrecht UniversityUtrechtThe Netherlands
- Austrian Research Center for PrimatologyOssiachAustria
| | | | - Brenda McCowan
- Department of Population Health & Reproduction, School of Veterinary MedicineUniversity of California at DavisDavisCaliforniaUSA
| | - Richard McFarland
- NTU PsychologyNottingham Trent UniversityNottinghamUK
- Applied Behavioural Ecology and Ecosystems Research UnitUniversity of South AfricaPretoriaSouth Africa
| | | | - Hélène Meunier
- Laboratoire de Neurosciences Cognitives et AdaptativesStrasbourgFrance
- Centre de Primatologie de l'Université de StrasbourgNiederhausbergenFrance
| | - Jérôme Micheletta
- Department of Psychology, Centre for Comparative and Evolutionary PsychologyUniversity of PortsmouthPortsmouthUK
| | - Partha S. Mishra
- Sálim Ali Centre for Ornithology and Natural HistoryCoimbatoreIndia
- Srishti Manipal Institute of Arts Design and TechnologyBengaluruIndia
| | - Shahrul A. M. Sah
- School of Biological SciencesUniversiti Sains MalaysiaGelugorMalaysia
| | | | | | - Nadine Müller‐Klein
- Institute for Evolutionary Ecology and Conservation GenomicsUlm UniversityUlmGermany
| | - Putu Oka Ngakan
- Forest Conservation DepartmentHasanuddin UniversityMakassarIndonesia
| | | | - Odile Petit
- Laboratoire de Psychologie Sociale et CognitiveCentre National de la Recherche Scientifique et Université Clermont‐AuvergneClermont‐FerrandFrance
| | - Lena S. Pflüger
- Department of Behavioral and Cognitive BiologyUniversity of ViennaViennaAustria
- Austrian Research Center for PrimatologyOssiachAustria
| | | | - Roopali Raghaven
- Animal Behaviour and Cognition Programme, National Institute of Advanced StudiesIndian Institute of Science CampusBengaluruIndia
| | - Gaël Raimbault
- Centre de Primatologie de l'Université de StrasbourgNiederhausbergenFrance
| | - Sunita Ram
- Animal Behaviour and Cognition Programme, National Institute of Advanced StudiesIndian Institute of Science CampusBengaluruIndia
- Foundation for Ecological ResearchAdvocacy and Learning MorattandiVillupuramIndia
| | - Ulrich H. Reichard
- Anthropology Program, School of Anthropology, Political Science and SociologySouthern Illinois University CarbondaleCarbondaleUSA
| | - Erin P. Riley
- Department of AnthropologySan Diego State UniversitySan DiegoCaliforniaUSA
| | - Alan V. Rincon
- Department of Psychology, Centre for Comparative and Evolutionary PsychologyUniversity of PortsmouthPortsmouthUK
| | - Nadine Ruppert
- School of Biological SciencesUniversiti Sains MalaysiaGelugorMalaysia
| | - Baptiste Sadoughi
- Laboratoire de Neurosciences Cognitives et AdaptativesStrasbourgFrance
- Department of Life SciencesUniversity of RoehamptonLondonUK
- Primate Social Evolution Group, German Primate CenterLeibniz Institute for Primate ResearchGöttingenGermany
- Behavioral Ecology DepartmentUniversity of GöttingenGöttingenGermany
| | - Kumar Santhosh
- Sálim Ali Centre for Ornithology and Natural HistoryCoimbatoreIndia
| | - Gabriele Schino
- Istituto di Scienze e Tecnologie della CognizioneConsiglio Nazionale delle RicercheRomeItaly
| | - Lori K. Sheeran
- AnthropologyCentral Washington UniversityEllensburgWashingtonUSA
| | - Joan B. Silk
- School of Human Evolution and Social Change and Institute of Human OriginsArizona State UniversityTempeArizonaUSA
| | - Mewa Singh
- Biopsychology Laboratory, Institution of ExcellenceUniversity of MysoreMysuruIndia
| | - Anindya Sinha
- Animal Behaviour and Cognition Programme, National Institute of Advanced StudiesIndian Institute of Science CampusBengaluruIndia
| | - Sebastian Sosa
- Department of Human Behavior, Ecology and CultureMax Planck Institute for Evolutionary AnthropologyLeipzigGermany
| | - Mathieu S. Stribos
- Animal Behavior & Cognition, Department of BiologyUtrecht UniversityUtrechtThe Netherlands
| | - Cédric Sueur
- Institut pluridisciplinaire Hubert Curien (UMR 7178), Centre national de la recherche scientifiqueUniversité de StrasbourgStrasbourgFrance
| | - Barbara Tiddi
- Natural Resources InstituteUniversity of GreenwichKentUK
| | - Patrick J. Tkaczynski
- Research Centre for Evolutionary Anthropology & Palaeoecology, School of Biological and Environmental SciencesLiverpool John Moores UniversityLiverpoolUK
| | - Florian Trebouet
- Department of AnthropologyNorthern Arizona UniversityFlagstaffArizonaUSA
| | - Anja Widdig
- Behavioural Ecology Research Group, Institute of BiologyUniversity of LeipzigLeipzigGermany
- Department of Human Behavior, Ecology and CultureMax Planck Institute for Evolutionary AnthropologyLeipzigGermany
- Department of Primate Behavior and EvolutionMax‐Planck Institute for Evolutionary AnthropologyLeipzigGermany
| | | | | | - Dong‐Po Xia
- International Collaborative Research Center for Huangshan Biodiversity and Tibetan Macaque Behavioral Ecology, School of Resource and Environmental EngineeringAnhui UniversityHefeiChina
- School of Life SciencesAnhui UniversityHefeiAnhuiChina
| | | | | | - Oliver Schülke
- Primate CognitionGöttingenGermany
- Primate Social Evolution Group, German Primate CenterLeibniz Institute for Primate ResearchGöttingenGermany
- Behavioral Ecology DepartmentUniversity of GöttingenGöttingenGermany
| | - Julia Ostner
- Primate CognitionGöttingenGermany
- Primate Social Evolution Group, German Primate CenterLeibniz Institute for Primate ResearchGöttingenGermany
- Behavioral Ecology DepartmentUniversity of GöttingenGöttingenGermany
| | - Christof Neumann
- Cognitive Ethology LabGerman Primate CenterGöttingenGermany
- Primate CognitionGöttingenGermany
| | - Julie Duboscq
- Behavioral Ecology DepartmentUniversity of GöttingenGöttingenGermany
- UMR7206 Eco‐AnthropologyCNRS‐MNHN‐Université Paris CitéParisFrance
| | - Lauren J. N. Brent
- School of Psychology, Centre for Research in Animal BehaviourUniversity of ExeterExeterUK
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Smit N. Hierarchies inferred from different agonistic behaviours are not always comparable. J Anim Ecol 2024; 93:1947-1959. [PMID: 39435973 PMCID: PMC11615268 DOI: 10.1111/1365-2656.14203] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/17/2024] [Accepted: 09/24/2024] [Indexed: 10/23/2024]
Abstract
Social hierarchies are widely used to predict life-history patterns and priority of access to resources. Yet, behavioural ecology and social sciences lack a consistent relationship between specific behaviours and social rank across studies. I used published data sets from 42 groups of 25 species representing several taxa to determine whether hierarchies inferred from different behaviours are similar or (in)consistently different at both individual and group levels. Ranks inferred from yielding interactions in the absence of aggression ('ritualized') were often comparable to ranks inferred from decided aggression (unambiguous outcome) but not to ranks inferred from undecided aggression. Accordingly, hierarchies inferred from data sets including only decided interactions were steeper than those inferred from data sets including undecided aggression. These results support the hypothesis that aggression can be context-dependent and might reflect less stable or mutually recognized relationships than (ritualized) yielding interactions. I discuss the consequences of choosing different behaviours to infer social hierarchies and the difficulty of making generalizations from one species or taxon to another. Finally, I recommend that the use of ritualized yielding and certainly the use of decided over undecided interactions to infer social hierarchies should be preferred, especially in comparative studies which go beyond taxon-specific idiosyncrasies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nikolaos Smit
- Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary AnthropologyLeipzigGermany
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Jerdee M, Newman MEJ. Luck, skill, and depth of competition in games and social hierarchies. SCIENCE ADVANCES 2024; 10:eadn2654. [PMID: 39504380 PMCID: PMC11540035 DOI: 10.1126/sciadv.adn2654] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/25/2024] [Accepted: 09/30/2024] [Indexed: 11/08/2024]
Abstract
Patterns of wins and lo sses in pairwise contests, such as occur in sports and games, consumer research and paired comparison studies, and human and animal social hierarchies, are commonly analyzed using probabilistic models that allow one to quantify the strength of competitors or predict the outcome of future contests. Here, we generalize this approach to incorporate two additional features: an element of randomness or luck that leads to upset wins, and a "depth of competition" variable that measures the complexity of a game or hierarchy. Fitting the resulting model, we estimate depth and luck in a range of games, sports, and social situations. In general, we find that social competition tends to be "deep," meaning it has a pronounced hierarchy with many distinct levels, but also that there is often a nonzero chance of an upset victory. Competition in sports and games, by contrast, tends to be shallow, and in most cases, there is little evidence of upset wins.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Maximilian Jerdee
- Department of Physics, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI 48109, USA
| | - M. E. J. Newman
- Department of Physics, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI 48109, USA
- Center for the Study of Complex Systems, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI 48109, USA
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Nagy M, Davidson JD, Vásárhelyi G, Ábel D, Kubinyi E, El Hady A, Vicsek T. Long-term tracking of social structure in groups of rats. Sci Rep 2024; 14:22857. [PMID: 39353967 PMCID: PMC11445254 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-024-72437-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/03/2024] [Accepted: 09/06/2024] [Indexed: 10/03/2024] Open
Abstract
Rodents serve as an important model for examining both individual and collective behavior. Dominance within rodent social structures can determine access to critical resources, such as food and mating opportunities. Yet, many aspects of the intricate interplay between individual behaviors and the resulting group social hierarchy, especially its evolution over time, remain unexplored. In this study, we utilized an automated tracking system that continuously monitored groups of male rats for over 250 days to enable an in-depth analysis of individual behavior and the overarching group dynamic. We describe the evolution of social structures within a group and additionally investigate how past behaviors influence the emergence of new social hierarchies when group composition and experimental area changes. Notably, we find that conventional individual and pairwise tests exhibit a weak correlation with group behavior, highlighting their limited accuracy in predicting behavioral outcomes in a collective context. These results emphasize the context-dependence of social behavior as an emergent property of interactions within a group and highlight the need to measure and quantify social behavior in more naturalistic environments.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Máté Nagy
- Department of Biological Physics, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Hungary.
- MTA-ELTE 'Lendület' Collective Behaviour Research Group, Hungarian Academy of Sciences, Budapest, Hungary.
- MTA-ELTE Statistical and Biological Physics Research Group, Hungarian Academy of Sciences, Budapest, Hungary.
- Department of Collective Behaviour, Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior, Constance, Germany.
- Department of Biology, University of Konstanz, Constance, Germany.
- Centre for the Advanced Study of Collective Behaviour, University of Konstanz, Constance, Germany.
| | - Jacob D Davidson
- Department of Collective Behaviour, Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior, Constance, Germany.
- Department of Biology, University of Konstanz, Constance, Germany.
- Centre for the Advanced Study of Collective Behaviour, University of Konstanz, Constance, Germany.
| | - Gábor Vásárhelyi
- Department of Biological Physics, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Hungary
- MTA-ELTE Statistical and Biological Physics Research Group, Hungarian Academy of Sciences, Budapest, Hungary
| | - Dániel Ábel
- Department of Biological Physics, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Hungary
| | - Enikő Kubinyi
- Department of Ethology, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Hungary
- ELTE NAP Canine Brain Research Group, Budapest, Hungary
- MTA-ELTE Lendület 'Momentum' Companion Animal Research Group, Budapest, Hungary
| | - Ahmed El Hady
- Department of Collective Behaviour, Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior, Constance, Germany
- Department of Biology, University of Konstanz, Constance, Germany
- Centre for the Advanced Study of Collective Behaviour, University of Konstanz, Constance, Germany
| | - Tamás Vicsek
- Department of Biological Physics, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Hungary
- MTA-ELTE Statistical and Biological Physics Research Group, Hungarian Academy of Sciences, Budapest, Hungary
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Albery GF, Bansal S, Silk MJ. Comparative approaches in social network ecology. Ecol Lett 2024; 27:e14345. [PMID: 38069575 DOI: 10.1111/ele.14345] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/03/2023] [Revised: 10/10/2023] [Accepted: 10/16/2023] [Indexed: 01/31/2024]
Abstract
Social systems vary enormously across the animal kingdom, with important implications for ecological and evolutionary processes such as infectious disease dynamics, anti-predator defence, and the evolution of cooperation. Comparing social network structures between species offers a promising route to help disentangle the ecological and evolutionary processes that shape this diversity. Comparative analyses of networks like these are challenging and have been used relatively little in ecology, but are becoming increasingly feasible as the number of empirical datasets expands. Here, we provide an overview of multispecies comparative social network studies in ecology and evolution. We identify a range of advancements that these studies have made and key challenges that they face, and we use these to guide methodological and empirical suggestions for future research. Overall, we hope to motivate wider publication and analysis of open social network datasets in animal ecology.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gregory F Albery
- Department of Biology, Georgetown University, Washington, District of Columbia, USA
- Leibniz Institute of Freshwater Ecology and Inland Fisheries, Berlin, Germany
| | - Shweta Bansal
- Department of Biology, Georgetown University, Washington, District of Columbia, USA
| | - Matthew J Silk
- CEFE, Univ Montpellier, CNRS, EPHE, IRD, Montpellier, France
- Institute of Ecology and Evolution, School of Biological Sciences, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Smith JE, Natterson-Horowitz B, Mueller MM, Alfaro ME. Mechanisms of equality and inequality in mammalian societies. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2023; 378:20220307. [PMID: 37381860 PMCID: PMC10291435 DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2022.0307] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/20/2023] [Accepted: 05/09/2023] [Indexed: 06/30/2023] Open
Abstract
The extent of (in)equality is highly diverse across species of social mammals, but we have a poor understanding of the factors that produce or inhibit equitable social organizations. Here, we adopt a comparative evolutionary perspective to test whether the evolution of social dominance hierarchies, a measure of social inequality in animals, exhibits phylogenetic conservatism and whether interspecific variation in these traits can be explained by sex, age or captivity. We find that hierarchy steepness and directional consistency evolve rapidly without any apparent constraint from evolutionary history. Given this extraordinary variability, we next consider multiple factors that have evolved to mitigate social inequality. Social networks, coalitionary support and knowledge transfer advantage to privilege some individuals over others. Nutritional access and prenatal stressors can impact the development of offspring, generating health disparities with intergenerational consequences. Intergenerational transfer of material resources (e.g. stone tools, food stashes, territories) advantage those who receive. Nonetheless, many of the same social species that experience unequal access to food (survival) and mates (reproduction) engage in levelling mechanisms such as food sharing, adoption, revolutionary coalitions, forgiveness and inequity aversion. Taken together, mammals rely upon a suite of mechanisms of (in)equality to balance the costs and benefits of group living. This article is part of the theme issue 'Evolutionary ecology of inequality'.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jennifer E. Smith
- Biology Department, University of Wisconsin Eau Claire, 105 Garfield Avenue, Eau Claire, WI 54702, USA
| | - Barbara Natterson-Horowitz
- School of Medicine, Division of Cardiology, University of California, 650 Charles Young Drive South, A2-237 CHS, Los Angeles, CA 90095, USA
| | - Maddison M. Mueller
- Biology Department, University of Wisconsin Eau Claire, 105 Garfield Avenue, Eau Claire, WI 54702, USA
| | - Michael E. Alfaro
- Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, University of California, 2149 Terasaki Life Sciences Building, 612 Charles E. Young Drive South, Box 957246, Los Angeles, CA 90095-7246, USA
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Mascaro O, Goupil N, Pantecouteau H, Depierreux A, Van der Henst JB, Claidière N. Human and animal dominance hierarchies show a pyramidal structure guiding adult and infant social inferences. Nat Hum Behav 2023; 7:1294-1306. [PMID: 37386104 DOI: 10.1038/s41562-023-01634-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/22/2022] [Accepted: 05/17/2023] [Indexed: 07/01/2023]
Abstract
This study investigates the structure of social hierarchies. We hypothesized that if social dominance relations serve to regulate conflicts over resources, then hierarchies should converge towards pyramidal shapes. Structural analyses and simulations confirmed this hypothesis, revealing a triadic-pyramidal motif across human and non-human hierarchies (114 species). Phylogenetic analyses showed that this pyramidal motif is widespread, with little influence of group size or phylogeny. Furthermore, nine experiments conducted in France found that human adults (N = 120) and infants (N = 120) draw inferences about dominance relations that are consistent with hierarchies' pyramidal motif. By contrast, human participants do not draw equivalent inferences based on a tree-shaped pattern with a similar complexity to pyramids. In short, social hierarchies exhibit a pyramidal motif across a wide range of species and environments. From infancy, humans exploit this regularity to draw systematic inferences about unobserved dominance relations, using processes akin to formal reasoning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Olivier Mascaro
- Université Paris Cité, CNRS, Integrative Neuroscience and Cognition Center, Paris, France.
| | - Nicolas Goupil
- Institut des Sciences Cognitives-Marc Jeannerod, UMR5229, CNRS and Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, Bron, France
| | | | - Adeline Depierreux
- Université Paris Cité, CNRS, Integrative Neuroscience and Cognition Center, Paris, France
| | - Jean-Baptiste Van der Henst
- Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, CNRS, INSERM, Centre de Recherche en Neurosciences de Lyon, U1028, UMR5292, Trajectoires, Bron, France
| | | |
Collapse
|
8
|
Peixoto TP. Ordered community detection in directed networks. Phys Rev E 2022; 106:024305. [PMID: 36109944 DOI: 10.1103/physreve.106.024305] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/13/2022] [Accepted: 08/02/2022] [Indexed: 06/15/2023]
Abstract
We develop a method to infer community structure in directed networks where the groups are ordered in a latent one-dimensional hierarchy that determines the preferred edge direction. Our nonparametric Bayesian approach is based on a modification of the stochastic block model (SBM), which can take advantage of rank alignment and coherence to produce parsimonious descriptions of networks that combine ordered hierarchies with arbitrary mixing patterns between groups. Since our model also includes directed degree correction, we can use it to distinguish nonlocal hierarchical structure from local in- and out-degree imbalance-thus, removing a source of conflation present in most ranking methods. We also demonstrate how we can reliably compare with the results obtained with the unordered SBM variant to determine whether a hierarchical ordering is statistically warranted in the first place. We illustrate the application of our method on a wide variety of empirical networks across several domains.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tiago P Peixoto
- Department of Network and Data Science, Central European University, 1100 Vienna, Austria
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Strauss ED, Curley JP, Shizuka D, Hobson EA. The centennial of the pecking order: current state and future prospects for the study of dominance hierarchies. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2022; 377:20200432. [PMID: 35000437 PMCID: PMC8743894 DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2020.0432] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/03/2021] [Accepted: 12/03/2021] [Indexed: 12/26/2022] Open
Abstract
A century ago, foundational work by Thorleif Schjelderup-Ebbe described a 'pecking order' in chicken societies, where individuals could be ordered according to their ability to exert their influence over their group-mates. Now known as dominance hierarchies, these structures have been shown to influence a plethora of individual characteristics and outcomes, situating dominance research as a pillar of the study of modern social ecology and evolution. Here, we first review some of the major questions that have been answered about dominance hierarchies in the last 100 years. Next, we introduce the contributions to this theme issue and summarize how they provide ongoing insight in the epistemology, physiology and neurobiology, hierarchical structure, and dynamics of dominance. These contributions employ the full range of research approaches available to modern biologists. Cross-cutting themes emerging from these contributions include a focus on cognitive underpinnings of dominance, the application of network-analytical approaches, and the utility of experimental rank manipulations for revealing causal relationships. Reflection on the last 100 years of dominance research reveals how Schjelderup-Ebbe's early ideas and the subsequent research helped drive a shift from an essentialist view of species characteristics to the modern recognition of rich inter-individual variation in social, behavioural and physiological phenotypes. This article is part of the theme issue 'The centennial of the pecking order: current state and future prospects for the study of dominance hierarchies'.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Eli D. Strauss
- Department of Collective Behavior, Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior, Konstanz, Germany
- Centre for the Advanced Study of Collective Behaviour, University of Konstanz, Konstanz, Germany
- School of Biological Sciences, University of Nebraska Lincoln, Lincoln, NE, USA
| | - James P. Curley
- Department of Psychology, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX, USA
| | - Daizaburo Shizuka
- School of Biological Sciences, University of Nebraska Lincoln, Lincoln, NE, USA
| | - Elizabeth A. Hobson
- Department of Biological Sciences, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, OH, USA
| |
Collapse
|