1
|
Delanoeije J, Engels M, Janssens M. "Pet effect" patterns: Dynamics of animal presence and caregiver affect across (tele)work and non-work contexts. PLoS One 2025; 20:e0319009. [PMID: 39977430 PMCID: PMC11841913 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0319009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/16/2024] [Accepted: 01/24/2025] [Indexed: 02/22/2025] Open
Abstract
Human-animal interactions (HAI) may relate to animal caregivers' affect, also referred to as the "pet effect". However, studies have not explored these associations in work contexts or evaluated longitudinal patterns of HAI with other activities across work and non-work contexts, and their associations with caregiver emotions. We therefore assess momentary associations between HAI during (tele)work and non-work time and positive and negative caregiver affect (PA/NA), identify person-level patterns of longitudinal HAI state trajectories, and analyze cross-level moderating effects of these patterns on momentary associations between HAI and PA/NA. First, we evaluated associations between momentary HAI and caregiver PA/NA including the moderating role of momentary work state (teleworking vs. not working). Second, using a data-driven approach, we applied sequence analysis to determine heterogeneity in state trajectories amongst caregivers using working activity and animal presence in five possible states (working at work/teleworking with animal/teleworking without animal/not working with animal/not working without animal), which we labelled as animal-work constellations. Similar trajectories of animal-work constellations across caregivers were grouped into clusters with recognizable patterns. Third, we assessed whether such patterns moderated momentary associations between HAI and caregiver PA/NA. Caregivers (Npersons = 324) completed ecological momentary survey data during five days with 10 prompts per day (Nobservations = 16,127) between 2017 and 2024. Results showed that momentary associations between HAI and affect were moderated by momentary work state and person-level animal-work constellation pattern, contextualizing the "pet effect". Our results highlight the importance of microlevel investigations of animal-work constellations and validates the novel use of sequence analysis to explore the role of context and time.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Joni Delanoeije
- Work and Organisation Studies, Faculty of Economics and Business, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
- Research Foundation Flanders (FWO), Brussels, Belgium
- International Association of Human-Animal Interaction Organizations (IAHAIO), Seattle, Washington, United States,
| | - Miriam Engels
- Faculty of Psychology, Open University, Heerlen, Netherlands
| | - Mayke Janssens
- Faculty of Psychology, Open University, Heerlen, Netherlands
- Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, School for Mental Health and Neuroscience, Maastricht University Medical Centre, Maastricht, Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Abdai J. Perception of animate motion in dogs. Front Psychol 2025; 15:1522489. [PMID: 39830849 PMCID: PMC11739167 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1522489] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/04/2024] [Accepted: 12/10/2024] [Indexed: 01/22/2025] Open
Abstract
Various motion cues can lead to the perception of animacy, including (1) simple motion characteristics such as starting to move from rest, (2) motion patterns of interactions like chasing, or (3) the motion of point-lights representing the joints of a moving biological agent. Due to the relevance of dogs in comparative research and considering the large variability within the species, studying animacy perception in dogs can provide unique information about how selection for specific traits and individual-level (social) differences may shape social perception. Despite these advantages, only a few studies have investigated the phenomenon in dogs. In this mini-review, we discuss the current findings about how specific motion dynamics associated with animacy drive dogs' visual attention.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Judit Abdai
- Center for Mind/Brain Sciences, University of Trento, Rovereto, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Lõoke M, Guérineau C, Broseghini A, Mongillo P, Marinelli L. Visual continuum in non-human animals: serial dependence revealed in dogs. Proc Biol Sci 2024; 291:20240051. [PMID: 39045690 PMCID: PMC11267470 DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2024.0051] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/08/2024] [Revised: 04/18/2024] [Accepted: 06/04/2024] [Indexed: 07/25/2024] Open
Abstract
Serial dependence is a recently described phenomenon by which the perceptual evaluation of a stimulus is biased by a previously attended one. By integrating stimuli over time, serial dependence is believed to ensure a stable conscious experience. Despite increasing studies in humans, it is unknown if the process occurs also in other species. Here, we assessed whether serial dependence occurs in dogs. To this aim, dogs were trained on a quantity discrimination task before being presented with a discrimination where one of the discriminanda was preceded by a task-irrelevant stimulus. If dogs are susceptible to serial dependence, the task-irrelevant stimulus was hypothesized to influence the perception of the subsequently presented quantity. Our results revealed that dogs perceived the currently presented quantity to be closer to the one presented briefly before, in accordance with serial dependence. The direction and strength of the effect were comparable to those observed in humans. Data regarding dogs' attention during the task suggest that dogs used two different quantity estimation mechanisms, an indication of a higher cognitive mechanism involved in the process. The present results are the first empirical evidence that serial dependence extends beyond humans, suggesting that the mechanism is shared by phylogenetically distant mammals.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Miina Lõoke
- Dipartimento di Biomedicina Comparata e Alimentazione, University of Padua, Legnaro, PD35020, Italy
| | - Cécile Guérineau
- Dipartimento di Biomedicina Comparata e Alimentazione, University of Padua, Legnaro, PD35020, Italy
| | - Anna Broseghini
- Dipartimento di Biomedicina Comparata e Alimentazione, University of Padua, Legnaro, PD35020, Italy
| | - Paolo Mongillo
- Dipartimento di Biomedicina Comparata e Alimentazione, University of Padua, Legnaro, PD35020, Italy
| | - Lieta Marinelli
- Dipartimento di Biomedicina Comparata e Alimentazione, University of Padua, Legnaro, PD35020, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Correia-Caeiro C, Guo K, Mills DS. Visual perception of emotion cues in dogs: a critical review of methodologies. Anim Cogn 2023; 26:727-754. [PMID: 36870003 PMCID: PMC10066124 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-023-01762-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/22/2022] [Revised: 02/07/2023] [Accepted: 02/20/2023] [Indexed: 03/05/2023]
Abstract
Comparative studies of human-dog cognition have grown exponentially since the 2000's, but the focus on how dogs look at us (as well as other dogs) as social partners is a more recent phenomenon despite its importance to human-dog interactions. Here, we briefly summarise the current state of research in visual perception of emotion cues in dogs and why this area is important; we then critically review its most commonly used methods, by discussing conceptual and methodological challenges and associated limitations in depth; finally, we suggest some possible solutions and recommend best practice for future research. Typically, most studies in this field have concentrated on facial emotional cues, with full body information rarely considered. There are many challenges in the way studies are conceptually designed (e.g., use of non-naturalistic stimuli) and the way researchers incorporate biases (e.g., anthropomorphism) into experimental designs, which may lead to problematic conclusions. However, technological and scientific advances offer the opportunity to gather much more valid, objective, and systematic data in this rapidly expanding field of study. Solving conceptual and methodological challenges in the field of emotion perception research in dogs will not only be beneficial in improving research in dog-human interactions, but also within the comparative psychology area, in which dogs are an important model species to study evolutionary processes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Catia Correia-Caeiro
- School of Psychology, University of Lincoln, Brayford Pool, Lincoln, LN6 7TS, UK.
- Department of Life Sciences, University of Lincoln, Lincoln, LN6 7DL, UK.
- Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University, Inuyama, 484-8506, Japan.
- Center for the Evolutionary Origins of Human Behavior, Kyoto University, Inuyama, 484-8506, Japan.
| | - Kun Guo
- School of Psychology, University of Lincoln, Brayford Pool, Lincoln, LN6 7TS, UK
| | - Daniel S Mills
- Department of Life Sciences, University of Lincoln, Lincoln, LN6 7DL, UK
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Lõoke M, Kanizsar O, Guérineau C, Mongillo P, Marinelli L. Dogs' ability to follow temporarily invisible moving objects: the ability to track and expect is shaped by experience. Anim Cogn 2023; 26:523-535. [PMID: 36167877 PMCID: PMC9950174 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-022-01695-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/03/2022] [Revised: 09/19/2022] [Accepted: 09/20/2022] [Indexed: 11/01/2022]
Abstract
Visually tracking a moving object, even if it becomes temporarily invisible, is an important skill for animals living in complex environments. However, this ability has not been widely explored in dogs. To address this gap of knowledge and understand how experience contributes to such ability, we conducted two experiments using a violation of expectation paradigm. Dogs were shown an animation of a ball moving horizontally across a screen, passing behind an occluder, and reappearing with a timing that was faster, slower or congruent with its initial speed. In the first experiment, dogs (N = 15) were exposed to the incongruent conditions without prior experience; while in the second experiment, dogs (N = 37) were preliminarily exposed to the congruent stimulus. Dogs of the first experiment did not exhibit a surprise effect, as measured by latency to look away from the expected stimulus presentation area, in response to the incongruent conditions, suggesting they had not formed an expectation about the timing of reappearance. However, their latency to orient towards the reappearing ball depended on the condition, suggesting they were able, to some extent, to visually keep track of the stimulus' trajectory. Dogs of the second experiment were surprised when the ball stayed behind the occluder longer than expected, but showed no difference in latency to orient across conditions. This suggests they had overcome the visual tracking mechanism and had formed expectations about the timing of reappearance. In conclusion, dogs seem to use a low-level mechanism to keep visual track of a temporarily disappearing moving object, but experience is required to make expectation about its trajectory.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Miina Lõoke
- grid.5608.b0000 0004 1757 3470Department of Comparative Biomedicine and Food Science, University of Padua, Viale dell’Università 16, 35020 Legnaro, PD Italy
| | - Orsolya Kanizsar
- grid.5608.b0000 0004 1757 3470Department of Comparative Biomedicine and Food Science, University of Padua, Viale dell’Università 16, 35020 Legnaro, PD Italy
| | - Cécile Guérineau
- grid.5608.b0000 0004 1757 3470Department of Comparative Biomedicine and Food Science, University of Padua, Viale dell’Università 16, 35020 Legnaro, PD Italy
| | - Paolo Mongillo
- Department of Comparative Biomedicine and Food Science, University of Padua, Viale dell'Università 16, 35020, Legnaro, PD, Italy.
| | - Lieta Marinelli
- grid.5608.b0000 0004 1757 3470Department of Comparative Biomedicine and Food Science, University of Padua, Viale dell’Università 16, 35020 Legnaro, PD Italy
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Reeve C, Jacques S. Responses to spoken words by domestic dogs: A new instrument for use with dog owners. Appl Anim Behav Sci 2022. [DOI: 10.1016/j.applanim.2021.105513] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/29/2022]
|
7
|
Eatherington CJ, Mongillo P, Lõoke M, Marinelli L. Dogs fail to recognize a human pointing gesture in two-dimensional depictions of motion cues. Behav Processes 2021; 189:104425. [PMID: 34010674 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2021.104425] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/03/2021] [Revised: 05/11/2021] [Accepted: 05/13/2021] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
Abstract
Few studies have investigated biological motion perception in dogs and it remains unknown whether dogs recognise the biological identity of two-dimensional animations of human motion cues. To test this, we assessed the dogs' (N = 32) responses to point-light displays of a human performing a pointing gesture towards one of two pots. At the start of the experiment the demonstrator was a real-life person, but over the course of the test dogs were presented with two-dimensional figurative representations of pointing gestures in which visual information was progressively removed until only the isolated motion cues remained. Dogs' accuracy was above chance level only with real-life and black-and-white videos, but not with the silhouette or the point-light figure. Dogs' accuracy during these conditions was significantly lower than in the real-life condition. This result could not be explained by trial order since dogs' performance was still not higher than chance when only the point-light figure condition was presented after the initial demonstration. The results imply that dogs are unable to recognise humans in two-dimensional depictions of human motion cues only. In spite of extensive exposure to human movement, dogs need more perceptual cues to detect equivalence between human two-dimensional animations and the represented living entity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Carla J Eatherington
- Laboratory of Applied Ethology, Department of Comparative Biomedicine and Food Science, University of Padua, Viale dell'Università 16, 35020, Legnaro, Italy
| | - Paolo Mongillo
- Laboratory of Applied Ethology, Department of Comparative Biomedicine and Food Science, University of Padua, Viale dell'Università 16, 35020, Legnaro, Italy.
| | - Miina Lõoke
- Laboratory of Applied Ethology, Department of Comparative Biomedicine and Food Science, University of Padua, Viale dell'Università 16, 35020, Legnaro, Italy
| | - Lieta Marinelli
- Laboratory of Applied Ethology, Department of Comparative Biomedicine and Food Science, University of Padua, Viale dell'Università 16, 35020, Legnaro, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Lõoke M, Kanizsàr O, Battaglini L, Guerineau C, Mongillo P, Marinelli L. Are dogs good at spotting movement? Velocity thresholds of motion detection in Canis familiaris. Curr Zool 2021; 66:699-701. [PMID: 33391371 PMCID: PMC7769578 DOI: 10.1093/cz/zoaa044] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/28/2020] [Accepted: 08/18/2020] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Miina Lõoke
- Laboratory of Applied Ethology, Department of Comparative Biomedicine and Food Science, University of Padua, Viale dell'Università 16, Legnaro (PD), 30202, Italy
| | - Orsolya Kanizsàr
- Laboratory of Applied Ethology, Department of Comparative Biomedicine and Food Science, University of Padua, Viale dell'Università 16, Legnaro (PD), 30202, Italy
| | - Luca Battaglini
- Department of General Psychology, University of Padua, Via Venezia 8, Padua, 35131, Italy
| | - Cécile Guerineau
- Laboratory of Applied Ethology, Department of Comparative Biomedicine and Food Science, University of Padua, Viale dell'Università 16, Legnaro (PD), 30202, Italy
| | - Paolo Mongillo
- Laboratory of Applied Ethology, Department of Comparative Biomedicine and Food Science, University of Padua, Viale dell'Università 16, Legnaro (PD), 30202, Italy
| | - Lieta Marinelli
- Laboratory of Applied Ethology, Department of Comparative Biomedicine and Food Science, University of Padua, Viale dell'Università 16, Legnaro (PD), 30202, Italy
| |
Collapse
|