1
|
Cade AE, Turnbull PRK. Cervical Spine Vibration Modifies Oculomotor Function in Young Adults with Traumatic Brain Injury. J Manipulative Physiol Ther 2024; 47:96-105. [PMID: 39412450 DOI: 10.1016/j.jmpt.2024.08.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/29/2023] [Revised: 03/21/2024] [Accepted: 08/14/2024] [Indexed: 10/29/2024]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE The purpose of this study was to investigate if vibrational interference of spinal proprioception affects oculomotor function, visual attention and processing, and selective attention in individuals with mild traumatic brain injury (mTBI) compared to healthy age-matched controls. METHODS This study was a parallel design, single-session intervention with 20 young adults with mTBI and 20 age-matched controls. Each completed a battery of computerized eye-tracking assessments (CEAs), including egocentric localization, fixational stability, smooth pursuit, saccades, Stroop, and the vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR), and then had their cervical spine function (flexion-relaxation ratio) recorded at baseline. Spinal vibration (100 Hz) was applied to the cervical spine and the CEA battery was repeated. CEA outcomes were compared to baseline and between mTBI and control groups. RESULTS Following cervical vibration, significant pre to post-differences were seen in both the mTBI and control group for egocentric localization, fixation stability, pursuit, saccades, Stroop, and VOR. At baseline, there was a significant difference between the mTBI and control groups across many CEA measures, with the mTBI group performing more poorly in egocentric localization, pursuit, saccades, Stroop, and VOR. The mTBI group also had a poorer flexion-relaxation ratio than the control group. CONCLUSION Cervical spine vibration improved cognitive and oculomotor performance in the mTBI group for VOR, Stroop, and pursuit, but had mixed effects on the control group. These findings suggest that some optometric mTBI symptoms may result from spinal or proprioceptive dysfunction, as altering proprioceptive input appears to positively impact visual outcomes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alice E Cade
- Optometry & Vision Science, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand.
| | | |
Collapse
|
2
|
Krasovskaya S, Kristjánsson Á, MacInnes WJ. Microsaccade rate activity during the preparation of pro- and antisaccades. Atten Percept Psychophys 2023; 85:2257-2276. [PMID: 37258896 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-023-02731-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 05/11/2023] [Indexed: 06/02/2023]
Abstract
Microsaccades belong to the category of fixational micromovements and may be crucial for image stability on the retina. Eye movement paradigms typically require fixational control, but this does not eliminate all oculomotor activity. The antisaccade task requires a planned eye movement in the direction opposite of an onset, allowing separation of planning and execution. We build on previous studies of microsaccades in the antisaccade task using a combination of fixed and mixed pro- and antisaccade blocks. We hypothesized that microsaccade rates may be reduced prior to the execution of antisaccades as compared with regular saccades (prosaccades). In two experiments, we measured microsaccades in four conditions across three trial blocks: one block each of fixed prosaccade and antisaccade trials, and a mixed block where both saccade types were randomized. We anticipated that microsaccade rates would be higher prior to antisaccades than prosaccades due to the need to preemptively suppress reflexive saccades during antisaccade generation. In Experiment 1, with monocular eye tracking, there was an interaction between the effects of saccade and block type on microsaccade rates, suggesting lower rates on antisaccade trials, but only within mixed blocks. In Experiment 2, eye tracking was binocular, revealing suppressed microsaccade rates on antisaccade trials. A cluster permutation analysis of the microsaccade rate over the course of a trial did not reveal any particular critical time for this difference in microsaccade rates. Our findings suggest that microsaccade rates reflect the degree of suppression of the oculomotor system during the antisaccade task.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sofia Krasovskaya
- Faculty of Psychology, University of Iceland, Reykjavik, Iceland.
- Icelandic Vision Lab, Faculty of Psychology, University of Iceland, Nýi Garður, Sæmundargata 12, 102, Reykjavik, Iceland.
- Vision Modelling Lab, HSE University, Moscow, Russia.
| | - Árni Kristjánsson
- Faculty of Psychology, University of Iceland, Reykjavik, Iceland
- Icelandic Vision Lab, Faculty of Psychology, University of Iceland, Nýi Garður, Sæmundargata 12, 102, Reykjavik, Iceland
| | - W Joseph MacInnes
- Vision Modelling Lab, HSE University, Moscow, Russia
- Department of Computer Science, Swansea University, Swansea, UK
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Ouerfelli-Ethier J, Fournet R, Khan AZ, Pisella L. Spatial bias in anti-saccade endpoints following bilateral dorsal posterior parietal lesions. Eur J Neurosci 2023; 58:3488-3502. [PMID: 37501610 DOI: 10.1111/ejn.16102] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/03/2023] [Revised: 06/21/2023] [Accepted: 07/07/2023] [Indexed: 07/29/2023]
Abstract
Anti-saccades are eye movements in which the saccade is executed in the opposite direction of a visual target and are often hypometric. Because the visual target and saccade goal are decoupled, it has been suggested that competition between the two locations occurs and needs to be resolved. It has been hypothesized that the hypometria of anti-saccades reflects this spatial competition by revealing a bias towards the visual target. To confirm that this hypometria is not simply due to reduced gain, we tested 10 healthy subjects on three different anti-saccade spatial configuration tasks: 90° away across hemifields, 90° away within the same hemifield and 180° away (classic, diagonally opposite). Specifically, we examined whether saccade endpoints showed evidence for the visual target location's interference with anti-saccade programming and execution processes. Among other neural substrates involved in anti-saccades production, the dorsal posterior parietal cortex (PPC) has been implicated in the spatial inhibition of contralateral visual target. To gain insight into the neural processes involved in spatial competition during anti-saccades, we also tested one patient with a bilateral dorsal PPC lesion. In all spatial configurations, we observed that anti-saccade endpoints demonstrated a spatial bias towards the visual target for all participants, likely due to an incomplete inhibition of the visual target location. This spatial bias was exacerbated in our patient, which suggests that the dorsal PPC contributes to the amalgamation of the two competing spatial representations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Julie Ouerfelli-Ethier
- Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, CNRS, INSERM, Centre de Recherche en Neurosciences de Lyon CRNL, Trajectoires, France
- School of Optometry, University of Montreal, Montreal, Quebec, Canada
| | - Romain Fournet
- School of Optometry, University of Montreal, Montreal, Quebec, Canada
| | - Aarlenne Z Khan
- School of Optometry, University of Montreal, Montreal, Quebec, Canada
| | - Laure Pisella
- Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, CNRS, INSERM, Centre de Recherche en Neurosciences de Lyon CRNL, Trajectoires, France
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Yang C, He L, Liu Y, Lin Z, Luo L, Gao S. Anti-saccades reveal impaired attention control over negative social evaluation in individuals with depressive symptoms. J Psychiatr Res 2023; 165:64-69. [PMID: 37463539 DOI: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2023.07.016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/08/2023] [Revised: 07/10/2023] [Accepted: 07/12/2023] [Indexed: 07/20/2023]
Abstract
Depressed individuals are excessively sensitive to negative information but blunt to positive information, which has been considered as vulnerability to depression. Here, we focused on inhibitory control over attentional bias on social evaluation in individuals with depression. We engaged individuals with and without depressive symptoms (categorized by Beck Depression Inventory-II) in a novel attention control task using positive and negative evaluative adjectives as self-referential feedback given by social others. Participants were instructed to look at sudden onset feedback targets (pro-saccade) or the mirror location of the targets (anti-saccade) when correct saccade latencies and saccade errors were collected. The two indices showed that while both groups displayed longer latencies and more errors for anti-saccade relative to pro-saccade responses depressed individuals spent more time reacting correctly and made more errors than non-depressed individuals in the anti-saccade trials and such group differences were not observed in the pro-saccade trials. Although group differences in correct anti-saccade latencies were found for both positive and negative stimuli, depressed individuals spent more time making correct anti-saccade responses to negative social feedback than to positive ones whereas non-depressed individuals featured longer correct anti-saccade latencies for positive relative to negative evaluations. Our results suggest that depressed individuals feature an impaired ability in attention control for self-referential evaluations, notably those of negative valence, shedding new light on depression-distorted self-schema and corresponding social dysfunctions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chaoqing Yang
- School of Foreign Languages, University of Electronic Science and Technology of China, Chengdu, China
| | - Linlin He
- School of Law, Southwest University of Science and Technology, Mianyang, China
| | - Yucheng Liu
- School of Foreign Languages, University of Electronic Science and Technology of China, Chengdu, China
| | - Ziyang Lin
- Pittsburgh Institute, Sichuan University, Chengdu, China
| | - Lizhu Luo
- Brian-Body Initiative, A*STAR Research Entities (ARES), Singapore; Singapore Institute for Clinical Sciences, A*STAR Research Entities, Singapore.
| | - Shan Gao
- School of Foreign Languages, University of Electronic Science and Technology of China, Chengdu, China; The Clinical Hospital of Chengdu Brain Science Institute, MOE Key Laboratory for Neuroinformation, University of Electronic Science and Technology of China, Chengdu, China.
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Navalón P, Sahuquillo-Leal R, Moreno-Giménez A, Salmerón L, Benavent P, Sierra P, Cañada Y, Cañada-Martínez A, Berk M, García-Blanco A. Attentional engagement and inhibitory control according to positive and negative symptoms in schizophrenia: An emotional antisaccade task. Schizophr Res 2022; 239:142-150. [PMID: 34891078 DOI: 10.1016/j.schres.2021.11.044] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/10/2020] [Revised: 10/18/2021] [Accepted: 11/27/2021] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
Abstract
Despite schizophrenia (SZ) is characterized by a high psychopathological heterogeneity, the underlying psychological mechanisms that result in different clinical profiles are unclear. This study examined the cognitive processing of emotional faces (angry, happy, neutral, and sad) by means of assessing inhibitory control (antisaccade task) and attentional engagement (prosaccade task) with the eye-tracking paradigm. Firstly, two clinical SZ subgroups classified according to the predominance of positive (PSZ; n = 20) or negative symptoms (NSZ; n = 34) and a control group of 32 individuals were compared. Secondly, the association between prosaccade and antisaccade measurements and the severity of positive and negative symptoms were analyzed. The PSZ group showed slower antisaccades when angry faces were displayed, and higher positive symptoms were associated with slower prosaccade latencies to ones. Conversely, the NSZ group made overall slower prosaccades with an emotional advantage for angry faces, and higher negative symptoms were associated with faster antisaccade latencies to ones. Hence, whereas positive SZ profile is related to a lack of attentional engagement and an impaired inhibitory control to threatening information; negative SZ profile is linked to a lack of attentional engagement to faces, mainly with non-threat ones, and with an advantage to ignore distracting threatening stimuli. These findings support affective information-processing theories suggesting a hypersensitivity to threat for positive SZ profiles, and a desensitization to socio-emotional information for negative ones. Consequently, characterizing psychological mechanisms of SZ may allow improving current treatments to threat management when positive symptoms are predominant, or emotion sensitization when negative symptoms prevail.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Pablo Navalón
- Department of Psychiatry and Clinical Psychology, La Fe University and Polytechnic Hospital, Valencia, Spain; Neonatal Research Group, La Fe Health Research Institute, Valencia, Spain
| | - Rosa Sahuquillo-Leal
- Department of Personality, Evaluation, and Psychological Treatment, University of Valencia, Valencia, Spain
| | | | | | - Pilar Benavent
- Department of Psychiatry and Clinical Psychology, La Fe University and Polytechnic Hospital, Valencia, Spain; Mental Health Research Group, La Fe Health Research Institute, Valencia, Spain
| | - Pilar Sierra
- Department of Psychiatry and Clinical Psychology, La Fe University and Polytechnic Hospital, Valencia, Spain; Mental Health Research Group, La Fe Health Research Institute, Valencia, Spain; Department of Medicine, University of Valencia, Valencia, Spain
| | - Yolanda Cañada
- Department of Psychiatry and Clinical Psychology, La Fe University and Polytechnic Hospital, Valencia, Spain; Mental Health Research Group, La Fe Health Research Institute, Valencia, Spain
| | - Antonio Cañada-Martínez
- Data Science, Biostatistics, and Bioinformatics, La Fe Health Research Institute, Valencia, Spain
| | - Michael Berk
- Deakin University, IMPACT Strategic Research Centre, School of Medicine, Geelong, Australia; Orygen, The National Centre of Excellence in Youth Mental Health, Centre for Youth Mental Health, Florey Institute for Neuroscience and Mental Health, Department of Psychiatry, The University of Melbourne, Melbourne, Australia
| | - Ana García-Blanco
- Department of Psychiatry and Clinical Psychology, La Fe University and Polytechnic Hospital, Valencia, Spain; Neonatal Research Group, La Fe Health Research Institute, Valencia, Spain; Department of Personality, Evaluation, and Psychological Treatment, University of Valencia, Valencia, Spain.
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Barsznica Y, Noiret N, Lambert B, Monnin J, De Pinho C, Hickel J, Masse C, Richard-Devantoy S, Morgny C, Bennabi D, Haffen E, Laurent E, Vandel P, Chopard G. Saccadic Eye Movements in Elderly Depressed Patients With Suicidal Behaviors: An Exploratory Eye-Tracking Study. Front Psychol 2021; 12:712347. [PMID: 34858252 PMCID: PMC8631956 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.712347] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/20/2021] [Accepted: 10/11/2021] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Suicidal behaviors (SBs) are often associated with impaired performance on neuropsychological executive functioning (EF) measures that encourage the development of more specific and reliable tools. Recent evidence could suggest that saccadic movement using eye tracking can provide reliable information on EF in depressive elderly. The aim of this study was to describe oculomotor performances in elderly depressed patients with SB. To achieve this aim, we compared saccadic eye movement (SEM) performances in elderly depressed patients (N = 24) with SB and with no SB in prosaccade (PS) and antisaccade (AS) tasks under the gap, step, and overlap conditions. All participants also underwent a complete neuropsychological battery. Performances were impaired in patients with SB who exhibited less corrected AS errors and longer time to correct them than patients with no SB. Moreover, both groups had a similar performance for PS latencies and correct AS. These preliminary results suggested higher cognitive inflexibility in suicidal patients compared to non-suicidal. This inflexibility may explain the difficulty of the depressed elderly in generating solutions to the resurgence of suicidal ideation (SI) to respond adequately to stressful environments. The assessment of eye movement parameters in depressed elderly patients may be a first step in identifying high-risk patients for suicide.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yoan Barsznica
- Department of Clinical Psychiatry, Besançon University Hospital, Besançon, France
- Department of Neurology, Besançon University Hospital, Besançon, France
- Laboratory of Neurosciences and Cognitive Psychology, University of Bourgogne Franche-Comté, Besançon, France
- Memory Center of Research and Resources (CMRR), Besançon University Hospital, Besançon, France
| | - Nicolas Noiret
- UMR CNRS 7295 “Research Centre on Cognition and Learning,” University of Poitiers, Poitiers, France
| | | | - Julie Monnin
- Department of Clinical Psychiatry, Besançon University Hospital, Besançon, France
- Laboratory of Neurosciences and Cognitive Psychology, University of Bourgogne Franche-Comté, Besançon, France
- Memory Center of Research and Resources (CMRR), Besançon University Hospital, Besançon, France
| | - Claire De Pinho
- Department of Old Age Psychiatry, Association Hospitalière de Bourgogne Franche-Comté Hospital, Bavilliers, France
| | - Julia Hickel
- Department of Old Age Psychiatry, Association Hospitalière de Bourgogne Franche-Comté Hospital, Bavilliers, France
| | - Caroline Masse
- Department of Clinical Psychiatry, Besançon University Hospital, Besançon, France
- Laboratory of Neurosciences and Cognitive Psychology, University of Bourgogne Franche-Comté, Besançon, France
| | - Stephane Richard-Devantoy
- McGill Group for Suicide Studies, Douglas Mental Health University Institute, Department of Psychiatry, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada
| | - Cynthia Morgny
- Regional Health Observatory, Bourgogne-Franche-Comté, France
| | - Djamila Bennabi
- Department of Clinical Psychiatry, Besançon University Hospital, Besançon, France
- Laboratory of Neurosciences and Cognitive Psychology, University of Bourgogne Franche-Comté, Besançon, France
| | - Emmanuel Haffen
- Department of Clinical Psychiatry, Besançon University Hospital, Besançon, France
- Laboratory of Neurosciences and Cognitive Psychology, University of Bourgogne Franche-Comté, Besançon, France
| | - Eric Laurent
- Laboratory of Neurosciences and Cognitive Psychology, University of Bourgogne Franche-Comté, Besançon, France
| | - Pierre Vandel
- Department of Clinical Psychiatry, Besançon University Hospital, Besançon, France
- Laboratory of Neurosciences and Cognitive Psychology, University of Bourgogne Franche-Comté, Besançon, France
- Memory Center of Research and Resources (CMRR), Besançon University Hospital, Besançon, France
- Regional Health Observatory, Bourgogne-Franche-Comté, France
| | - Gilles Chopard
- Department of Clinical Psychiatry, Besançon University Hospital, Besançon, France
- Department of Neurology, Besançon University Hospital, Besançon, France
- Laboratory of Neurosciences and Cognitive Psychology, University of Bourgogne Franche-Comté, Besançon, France
- Memory Center of Research and Resources (CMRR), Besançon University Hospital, Besançon, France
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Gorina E, Kulikova AA, MacInnes WJ. Comparing saccadic and manual responses in the attention network test. Cortex 2021; 144:29-42. [PMID: 34597874 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2021.07.014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/28/2020] [Revised: 02/28/2021] [Accepted: 07/26/2021] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
Attention is proposed to be a system of multiple functional networks, including alertness, orienting and executive control. A popular experimental paradigm for testing these networks and their interactions within a single design is the Attentional Networks Test (ANT) (Fan et al., 2002). The role of the oculomotor system in these various networks, however, has not been tested despite the strong link between attention and eye movements. We modified the executive control component of the manual response ANT version (ANTm) that allows testing the networks' involvement with oculomotor responses. Specifically, we used a central target to signal pro or anti-saccades that allows us to match the saccadic response compatibility of the original ANTm. We conducted three experiments to compare interactions of the networks between the traditional ANTm that used a flanker task response, our new ANTs with saccadic responses signalled with a fixation arrow, and a manual response version with the response arrow at fixation (ANTf). Results for all three experiments showed typical main effects of all three attention networks, but we observed differences in their interactions. The ANTm showed only an interaction of alerting enhancing the orienting; ANTs showed a congruency by orienting interaction with the orienting effect only observed for pro-saccades. The ANTf showed both alerting by orienting, and orienting by congruency. Although the saccadic response did differ from the original ANTm, key differences were also highlighted by the switch from peripheral to central target. Overall the proposed ANTf is a valid tool to test main effects of attentional networks. Further investigation of interaction differences between manual and oculomotor systems is required.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elena Gorina
- Vision Modelling Lab, HSE University, Moscow, Russia.
| | | | - W Joseph MacInnes
- Vision Modelling Lab, HSE University, Moscow, Russia; Department of Psychology, HSE University, Moscow, Russia.
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Ouerfelli-Ethier J, Salemme R, Fournet R, Urquizar C, Pisella L, Khan AZ. Impaired Spatial Inhibition Processes for Interhemispheric Anti-saccades following Dorsal Posterior Parietal Lesions. Cereb Cortex Commun 2021; 2:tgab054. [PMID: 34604753 PMCID: PMC8481671 DOI: 10.1093/texcom/tgab054] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/26/2021] [Revised: 08/25/2021] [Accepted: 08/25/2021] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Anti-saccades are eye movements that require inhibition to stop the automatic saccade to the visual target and to perform instead a saccade in the opposite direction. The inhibitory processes underlying anti-saccades have been primarily associated with frontal cortex areas for their role in executive control. Impaired performance in anti-saccades has also been associated with the parietal cortex, but its role in inhibitory processes remains unclear. Here, we tested the assumption that the dorsal parietal cortex contributes to spatial inhibition processes of contralateral visual target. We measured anti-saccade performance in 2 unilateral optic ataxia patients and 15 age-matched controls. Participants performed 90 degree (across and within visual fields) and 180 degree inversion anti-saccades, as well as pro-saccades. The main result was that our patients took longer to inhibit visually guided saccades when the visual target was presented in the ataxic hemifield and the task required a saccade across hemifields. This was observed through anti-saccades latencies and error rates. These deficits show the crucial role of the dorsal posterior parietal cortex in spatial inhibition of contralateral visual target representations to plan an accurate anti-saccade toward the ipsilesional side.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Julie Ouerfelli-Ethier
- School of Optometry, University of Montreal, Montreal H3T 1P1, Canada
- Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, Trajectoires Team, INSERM 1028, CNRS UMR 5292, University of Lyon I Claude-Bernard, Lyon 69500, France
| | - Romeo Salemme
- Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, Trajectoires Team, INSERM 1028, CNRS UMR 5292, University of Lyon I Claude-Bernard, Lyon 69500, France
| | - Romain Fournet
- School of Optometry, University of Montreal, Montreal H3T 1P1, Canada
| | - Christian Urquizar
- Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, Trajectoires Team, INSERM 1028, CNRS UMR 5292, University of Lyon I Claude-Bernard, Lyon 69500, France
| | - Laure Pisella
- Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, Trajectoires Team, INSERM 1028, CNRS UMR 5292, University of Lyon I Claude-Bernard, Lyon 69500, France
| | - Aarlenne Z Khan
- School of Optometry, University of Montreal, Montreal H3T 1P1, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Boutsen L, Pearson NA, Jüttner M. Do facially disfiguring features influence attention and perception of faces? Evidence from an antisaccade task. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2021; 75:830-840. [PMID: 34388951 PMCID: PMC8958561 DOI: 10.1177/17470218211041621] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
Facial disfigurements can influence how observers attend to and interact with the person, leading to disease-avoidance behaviour and emotions (disgust, threat, fear for contagion). However, it is unclear whether this behaviour is reflected in the effect of the facial stigma on attention and perceptual encoding of facial information. We addressed this question by measuring, in a mixed antisaccade task, observers' speed and accuracy of orienting of visual attention towards or away from peripherally presented upright and inverted unfamiliar faces that had either a realistic looking disease-signalling feature (a skin discolouration), a non-disease-signalling control feature, or no added feature. The presence of a disfiguring or control feature did not influence the orienting of attention (in terms of saccadic latency) towards upright faces, suggesting that avoidance responses towards facial stigma do not occur during covert attention. However, disfiguring and control features significantly reduced the effect of face inversion on saccadic latency, thus suggesting an impact on the holistic processing of facial information. The implications of these findings for the encoding and appraisal of facial disfigurements are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Luc Boutsen
- School of Psychology, Aston University, Birmingham, UK
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
10
|
Zeligman L, Zivotofsky AZ. A novel variation of the Stroop task reveals reflexive supremacy of peripheral over gaze stimuli in pro and anti saccades. Conscious Cogn 2020; 85:103020. [PMID: 32932098 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2020.103020] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/07/2020] [Revised: 06/28/2020] [Accepted: 09/02/2020] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
Abstract
A long-standing controversy in social attention debates whether gaze-of-another induces reflexive shifts of one's own attention. In attempting to resolve this controversy, we utilized a novel Stroop task, the PAT Stroop, in which pro- and anti-saccade (PAT) responses are made to competing gaze and peripheral stimuli. The first experiment demonstrated a "Stroop effect" for peripheral stimuli, i.e. peripheral distractors interfered with gaze triggers, but gaze distractors did not interfere with peripheral triggers. These results were replicated in the second experiment, which also negated the possibility that the mere display and practice of the "clean PAT" influenced the results. Thus, the use a new PAT Stroop task demonstrated reflexive supremacy of peripheral stimuli over gaze stimuli. This novel variant of the Stroop task demonstrated similar characteristics to the classic color naming Stroop - i.e. an asymmetrical pattern, and again showed the utility and versatility of stoop-like tasks in probing mental tasks.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Liran Zeligman
- Department of Psychology, Bar-Ilan University, and School of Behavioral Sciences, Peres Academic Center, Israel
| | - Ari Z Zivotofsky
- The Leslie and Susan Gonda (Goldschmied) Multidisciplinary Brain Research Center, Bar-Ilan University, Israel.
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Moran RN, Wallace J, Murray NG, Covassin T. Effects of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and learning disability on vestibular and ocular baseline concussion assessment in pediatric athletes. APPLIED NEUROPSYCHOLOGY-CHILD 2019; 10:276-282. [PMID: 31650856 DOI: 10.1080/21622965.2019.1683453] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
Abstract
Vestibular and ocular motor assessment has gained popularity as a tool for sport-related concussion among healthcare professionals. With awareness of premorbid risk factors, such as attentional problems, it is imperative to understand their effects at baseline. The purpose of this study was to examine the effects of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and diagnosed learning disability (LD) on vestibular and oculomotor baseline concussion assessment in pediatrics. Pediatric athletes between the ages of 8 and 14 years with ADHD/LD (n = 30) and match controls without ADHD/LD (n = 30) completed baseline concussion testing, consisting of symptom provocation on the Vestibular/Ocular Motor Screening (VOMS) and oculomotor performance on near point of convergence (NPC) and King-Devick (K-D) tools. Those diagnosed with ADHD/LD performed worse on baseline saccades (p range = .02-.10), convergence (p = .04), vestibular ocular reflex (VOR) (p = .03) and visual motion sensitivity (VMS) (p = .04) of the VOMS. Base rate analyses revealed that 26% of athletes in the ADHD/LD group had ≥1 and 13.3% had ≥2 VOMS domains over clinical cutoff levels, compared to 3.3% (≥1 domain) and 0 (≥2 domains) of controls. Individuals with ADHD/LD also performed worse on K-D assessment (p = .005). However, no differences were reported on NPC distance (p = .19). These findings suggest worse baseline concussion assessment scores on vestibular/ocular motor assessment domains in pediatric athletes diagnosed with ADHD/LD. Additional research is needed on assessment outcomes to determine if special consideration to those diagnosed with ADHD/LD is warranted.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ryan N Moran
- Athletic Training Research Laboratory, Department of Health Science, The University of Alabama, Tuscaloosa, AL, USA
| | - Jessica Wallace
- Athletic Training Research Laboratory, Department of Health Science, The University of Alabama, Tuscaloosa, AL, USA
| | - Nicholas G Murray
- School of Community Health Sciences, University of Nevada, Reno, NV, USA
| | - Tracey Covassin
- Sport Injury Research Laboratory, Department of Kinesiology, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI, USA
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Abstract
The antisaccade (AS) task is considered a prominent measure of inhibitory control, but it is still unclear which cognitive processes are used for successful performance of the task. Previous results have provided evidence for the involvement of several processes, including working memory (WM), inhibition and attention. Thus, the aim of this study was to explore, using a range of neuropsychological tests, which cognitive factors predict individual differences in AS performance. To do so, 143 healthy participants underwent a battery including tests measuring inhibition, working memory, cognitive flexibility, sustained attention, IQ and fluency. Hierarchical stepwise regression analyses were conducted to assess the association with AS performance. Performance on the Trail-Making-Test, version B (TMT-B), a test measuring flexibility, divided attention and WM, was found to significantly predict AS latency. Rapid Visual Information Processing (RVIP), used to assess sustained attention and WM, significantly predicted AS error rate. Other cognitive measures, however, did not significantly predict AS performance. Bayesian Model Averaging supported these conclusions and showed that non-significant predictors are unlikely to be associated with AS outcomes. Several explanations are provided for the associations of TMT-B and RVIP with AS performance; as the tests measure a range of different cognitive processes, interpretation of these results remains less clear. For a better understanding of the cognitive mechanisms underlying AS performance, future research should make use of a wider range of attention and WM tests.
Collapse
|
13
|
Ferreira MB, Pereira PA, Parreira M, Sousa I, Figueiredo J, Cerqueira JJ, Macedo AF. Relationships between neuropsychological and antisaccade measures in multiple sclerosis patients. PeerJ 2018; 6:e5737. [PMID: 30310755 PMCID: PMC6174870 DOI: 10.7717/peerj.5737] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/03/2018] [Accepted: 09/12/2018] [Indexed: 11/20/2022] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND The Stroop test is frequently used to assess deficits in inhibitory control in people with multiple sclerosis (MS). This test has limitations and antisaccade eye movements, that also measure inhibitory control, may be an alternative to Stroop. OBJECTIVES The aim of this study was twofold: (i) to investigate if the performance in the antisaccade task is altered in patients with MS and (ii) to investigate the correlation between performances in neuropsychological tests, the Stroop test and the antisaccade task. METHODS We measured antisaccades (AS) parameters with an infrared eye tracker (SMIRED 250 Hz) using a standard AS paradigm. A total of 38 subjects diagnosed with MS and 38 age and gender matched controls participated in this study. Neuropsychological measures were obtained from the MS group. RESULTS Patients with MS have higher error rates and prolonged latency than controls in the antisaccade task. There was a consistent association between the Stroop performance and AS latency. Stroop performance but not AS latency was associated with other neuropsychological measures in which the MS group showed deficits. CONCLUSIONS Our findings suggest that AS may be a selective and independent measure to investigate inhibitory control in patients with MS. More studies are necessary to confirm our results and to describe brain correlates associated with impaired performance in the antisaccade task in people diagnosed with MS.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marisa Borges Ferreira
- Low Vision and Visual Rehabilitation Lab, Department and Center of Physics—Optometry and Vision Science, University of Minho, Braga, Portugal
- Association “Todos com a Esclerose Multipla (TEM)”, Braga, Portugal
| | - Paulo Alexandre Pereira
- Association “Todos com a Esclerose Multipla (TEM)”, Braga, Portugal
- Centre of Mathematics and Department of Mathematics and Applications, University of Minho, Braga, Portugal
| | - Marta Parreira
- Association “Todos com a Esclerose Multipla (TEM)”, Braga, Portugal
| | - Ines Sousa
- Centre of Molecular and Environmental Biology (CBMA), and Department of Mathematics and Applications, Universidade do Minho, Braga, Portugal
| | - José Figueiredo
- Association “Todos com a Esclerose Multipla (TEM)”, Braga, Portugal
| | - João José Cerqueira
- Neurosciences Domain; Life and Health Sciences Research Institute, School of Health Sciences and ICVS/3B’s Associate Laboratory, University of Minho, Braga, Portugal
- Clinical Academic Centre (CCA), Hospital de Braga, Braga, Portugal
| | - Antonio Filipe Macedo
- Low Vision and Visual Rehabilitation Lab, Department and Center of Physics—Optometry and Vision Science, University of Minho, Braga, Portugal
- Department of Medicine and Optometry, Linnaeus University, Kalmar, Sweden
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Van Humbeeck N, Meghanathan RN, Wagemans J, van Leeuwen C, Nikolaev AR. Presaccadic EEG activity predicts visual saliency in free-viewing contour integration. Psychophysiology 2018; 55:e13267. [PMID: 30069911 DOI: 10.1111/psyp.13267] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/14/2016] [Revised: 04/26/2018] [Accepted: 06/11/2018] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
While viewing a scene, the eyes are attracted to salient stimuli. We set out to identify the brain signals controlling this process. In a contour integration task, in which participants searched for a collinear contour in a field of randomly oriented Gabor elements, a previously established model was applied to calculate a visual saliency value for each fixation location. We studied brain activity related to the modeled saliency values, using coregistered eye tracking and EEG. To disentangle EEG signals reflecting salience in free viewing from overlapping EEG responses to sequential eye movements, we adopted generalized additive mixed modeling (GAMM) to single epochs of saccade-related EEG. We found that, when saliency at the next fixation location was high, amplitude of the presaccadic EEG activity was low. Since presaccadic activity reflects covert attention to the saccade target, our results indicate that larger attentional effort is needed for selecting less salient saccade targets than more salient ones. This effect was prominent in contour-present conditions (half of the trials), but ambiguous in the contour-absent condition. Presaccadic EEG activity may thus be indicative of bottom-up factors in saccade guidance. The results underscore the utility of GAMM for EEG-eye movement coregistration research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Johan Wagemans
- Brain & Cognition Research Unit, KU Leuven-University of Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
| | - Cees van Leeuwen
- Brain & Cognition Research Unit, KU Leuven-University of Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
| | - Andrey R Nikolaev
- Brain & Cognition Research Unit, KU Leuven-University of Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Tseng P, Wang MC, Lo YH, Juan CH. Anodal and Cathodal tDCS Over the Right Frontal Eye Fields Impacts Spatial Probability Processing Differently in Pro- and Anti-saccades. Front Neurosci 2018; 12:421. [PMID: 29997471 PMCID: PMC6030360 DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00421] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/02/2018] [Accepted: 06/04/2018] [Indexed: 11/17/2022] Open
Abstract
Learning regularities that exist in the environment can help the visual system achieve optimal efficiency while reducing computational burden. Using a pro- and anti-saccade task, studies have shown that probabilistic information regarding spatial locations can be a strong modulator of frontal eye fields (FEF) activities and consequently alter saccadic behavior. One recent study has also shown that FEF activities can be modulated by transcranial direct current stimulation, where anodal tDCS facilitated prosaccades but cathodal tDCS prolonged antisaccades. These studies together suggest that location probability and tDCS can both alter FEF activities and oculomotor performance, yet how these two modulators interact with each other remains unclear. In this study, we applied anodal or cathodal tDCS over right FEF, and participants performed an interleaved pro- and anti-saccade task. Location probability was manipulated in prosaccade trials but not antisaccade trials. We observed that anodal tDCS over rFEF facilitated prosaccdes toward low-probability locations but not to high-probability locations; whereas cathodal tDCS facilitated antisaccades away from the high-probability location (i.e., same location as the low-probability locations in prosaccades). These observed effects were specific to rFEF as tDCS over the SEF in a separate control experiment did not yield similar patterns. These effects were also more pronounced in low-performers who had slower saccade reaction time. Together, we conclude that (1) the overlapping spatial endpoint between prosaccades (i.e., toward low-probability location) and antisaccades (i.e., away from high-probability location) possibly suggest an endpoint-selective mechanism within right FEF, (2) anodal tDCS and location probability cannot be combined to produce a bigger facilitative effect, and (3) anodal rFEF tDCS works best on low-performers who had slower saccade reaction time. These observations are consistent with the homeostasis account of tDCS effect and FEF functioning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Philip Tseng
- Graduate Institute of Humanities in Medicine, Taipei Medical University, Taipei, Taiwan.,Research Center of Brain and Consciousness, Taipei Medical University, Taipei, Taiwan.,Shuang-Ho Hospital, Taipei Medical University, Taipei, Taiwan
| | - Mu-Chen Wang
- Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, National Central University, Taoyuan, Taiwan
| | - Yu-Hui Lo
- Research Center of Brain and Consciousness, Taipei Medical University, Taipei, Taiwan
| | - Chi-Hung Juan
- Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, National Central University, Taoyuan, Taiwan.,Brain Research Center, National Central University, Taoyuan, Taiwan
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Clough M, Foletta P, Frohman AN, Sears D, Ternes A, White OB, Fielding J. Multiple sclerosis: Executive dysfunction, task switching and the role of attention. Mult Scler J Exp Transl Clin 2018; 4:2055217318771781. [PMID: 29707228 PMCID: PMC5912274 DOI: 10.1177/2055217318771781] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/07/2018] [Revised: 02/27/2018] [Accepted: 03/20/2018] [Indexed: 11/25/2022] Open
Abstract
Background It has been suggested that switching ability might not be affected in multiple sclerosis (MS) as previously thought; however, whether this is true under more ‘real-world’ conditions when asymmetry in task difficulty is present has not been ascertained. Objective The objective of this paper is to examine the impact of task difficulty asymmetry on task switching ability in MS. Method An ocular motor (OM) paradigm that interleaves the simple task of looking towards a target (prosaccade, PS) with the cognitively more difficult task of looking away from a target (antisaccade, PS) was used. Two switching conditions: (1) PS switch cost, switching to a simple task from a difficult task (PS switch), relative to performing two simple tasks concurrently (PS repeat); (2) AS switch cost, switching to a difficult task from a simple task (AS switch) relative to performing two difficult tasks concurrently (AS repeat). Forty-five relapsing–remitting MS patients and 30 control individuals were compared. Results Controls and patients produced a similar magnitude PS switch cost, suggesting that task difficulty asymmetry does not detrimentally impact MS patients when transitioning from a more difficult task to a simpler task. However, MS patients alone found switching from the simpler PS trial to the more difficult AS trial easier (shorter latency and reduced error) than performing two AS trials consecutively (AS switch benefit). Further, MS patients performed significantly more errors than controls when required to repeat the same trial consecutively. Conclusion MS patients appear to find the maintenance of task-relevant processes difficult not switching per se, with deficits exacerbated under increased attentional demands.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- M Clough
- Department of Neurosciences, Central Clinical School, Monash University, Alfred Hospital, Australia.,School of Psychological Sciences and Monash Institute of Cognitive and Clinical Neurosciences, Monash University, Australia
| | - P Foletta
- Department of Neurosciences, Central Clinical School, Monash University, Alfred Hospital, Australia.,School of Psychological Sciences and Monash Institute of Cognitive and Clinical Neurosciences, Monash University, Australia
| | - A N Frohman
- Departments of Neurology and Neurotherapeutics, University of Texas Southwestern School of Medicine, USA
| | - D Sears
- Departments of Neurology and Neurotherapeutics, University of Texas Southwestern School of Medicine, USA
| | - A Ternes
- School of Psychological Sciences and Monash Institute of Cognitive and Clinical Neurosciences, Monash University, Australia
| | - O B White
- Department of Neurosciences, Central Clinical School, Monash University, Alfred Hospital, Australia
| | - J Fielding
- Department of Neurosciences, Central Clinical School, Monash University, Alfred Hospital, Australia.,School of Psychological Sciences and Monash Institute of Cognitive and Clinical Neurosciences, Monash University, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Jóhannesson ÓI, Edelman JA, Sigurþórsson BD, Kristjánsson Á. Effects of saccade training on express saccade proportions, saccade latencies, and peak velocities: an investigation of nasal/temporal differences. Exp Brain Res 2018; 236:1251-1262. [PMID: 29480354 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-018-5213-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/29/2017] [Accepted: 02/20/2018] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
Abstract
Express saccades have very short latencies and are often considered a special population of saccadic eye movements. Recent evidence suggests that express saccade generation in humans increases with training, and that this training is independent of the actual saccade vector being trained. We assessed the time course of these training-induced increases in express saccade generation and how they differ between the nasal and temporal hemifields, and second whether they transfer from the trained to the untrained eye. We also measured the effects of training on saccade latencies more generally, and upon peak velocities. The training effect transferred between the nasal and temporal hemifields and between the trained and untrained eyes. More surprisingly, we found an asymmetric effect of training on express saccade proportions: Before training, express saccade proportions were higher for saccades made into the nasal hemifield but with training this reversed. This training-induced asymmetry was also observed in overall saccade latencies, showing how training can unmask nasal/temporal asymmetries in saccade latencies. Finally, we report for the first time that saccadic peak velocities increased with training, independently of changes in amplitude.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ómar I Jóhannesson
- Department of Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, School of Health Sciences, University of Iceland, Sæmundargötu 2, 101, Reykjavík, Iceland.
| | - Jay A Edelman
- Department of Biology, The City College of New York, New York, NY, USA
| | - Bjarki Dalsgaard Sigurþórsson
- Department of Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, School of Health Sciences, University of Iceland, Sæmundargötu 2, 101, Reykjavík, Iceland
| | - Árni Kristjánsson
- Department of Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, School of Health Sciences, University of Iceland, Sæmundargötu 2, 101, Reykjavík, Iceland
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Face stimulus eliminates antisaccade-cost: gaze following is a different kind of arrow. Exp Brain Res 2018; 236:1041-1052. [DOI: 10.1007/s00221-018-5198-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/18/2017] [Accepted: 02/05/2018] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
|
19
|
Commodari E. Novice Readers: The Role of Focused, Selective, Distributed and Alternating Attention at the First Year of the Academic Curriculum. Iperception 2017; 8:2041669517718557. [PMID: 28835811 PMCID: PMC5528191 DOI: 10.1177/2041669517718557] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022] Open
Abstract
The ability to read depends on different cognitive skills. This study investigated the role of the main components of attention (selective attention, focused attention, distributed attention, and alternating attention) on the different dimensions of reading skills in novice readers. Participants were 288 Italian children, who attended the first year of primary school. Attention and reading skills (reading "comprehension," "accuracy," and "speed") were measured. Different components of attention influence each dimension of reading. Moreover, both the correctness and rapidity at which attention operates play a pivotal role in learning to read. Interestingly, selective attention is involved in all dimensions of reading. These findings may have educational and practical relevance. The early assessment of attention might favor the development of new strategies of intervention in dyslexic children and in children at risk of developing learning difficulties.
Collapse
|
20
|
Zeligman L, Zivotofsky AZ. Back to basics: The effects of block vs. interleaved trial administration on pro- and anti-saccade performance. PLoS One 2017; 12:e0172485. [PMID: 28222173 PMCID: PMC5319747 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0172485] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/20/2016] [Accepted: 02/06/2017] [Indexed: 12/14/2022] Open
Abstract
The pro and anti-saccade task (PAT) is a widely used tool in the study of overt and covert attention with promising potential role in neurocognitive and psychiatric assessment. However, specific PAT protocols can vary significantly between labs, potentially resulting in large variations in findings across studies. In light of recent calls towards a standardization of PAT the current study's objective was to systematically and purposely evaluate the effects of block vs. interleaved administration—a fundamental consideration—on PAT measures in a within subject design. Additionally, this study evaluated whether measures of a Posner-type cueing paradigm parallels measures of the PAT paradigm. As hypothesized, results indicate that PAT performance is highly susceptible to administration mode. Interleaved mode resulted in larger error rates not only for anti (blocks: M = 22%; interleaved: M = 42%) but also for pro-saccades (blocks: M = 5%; interleaved: M = 12%). This difference between block and interleaved administration was significantly larger in anti-saccades compared to pro-saccades and cannot be attributed to a 'speed/accuracy tradeoff'. Interleaved mode produced larger pro and anti-saccade differences in error rates while block administration produced larger latency differences. Results question the reflexive nature of pro-saccades, suggesting they are not purely reflexive. These results were further discussed and compared to previous studies that included within subject data of blocks and interleaved trials.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Liran Zeligman
- Dept. of Psychology, Bar Ilan University, Ramat Gan, Israel
| | - Ari Z. Zivotofsky
- The Leslie and Susan Gonda (Goldschmied) Multidisciplinary Brain Research Center, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat Gan, Israel
- * E-mail:
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Commodari E. Voluntary Attentional Orienting in Schoolchildren. Percept Mot Skills 2017. [DOI: 10.1177/0033294116652034] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
Voluntary attentional orienting is the foundation of the capacity to highlight relevant stimuli and suppress competing distractions. It plays a role in the scholastic skills. This study investigated the changes in the orienting skills in 630 schoolchildren (age range: 5.7–10.2 years old), who attended the first ( N = 205, M = 6.1, SD = 0.4), third (212 children, M = 8.2, SD = 0.6), and fifth grade (213 children, M = 10, SD = 0.4) of four primary schools in Italy. Simple attentional orienting, choice attentional orienting on written stimuli, double attentional orienting (auditory and visual), and re-orienting were investigated. The tasks required pressing a key in response to some target stimuli. The stimuli were digits, images of objects, written words, letters, geometric forms, and auditory verbal stimuli. Results showed that attentional orienting improved with age. The children's performances depended on the type of stimuli and activities required for the task. The accuracy of re-orienting with written verbal stimuli did not improve during primary school. With increasing age, children became faster but not more able to focus on written verbal stimuli when shifting of attention was required.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elena Commodari
- Department of Educational Processes, University of Catania, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Noiret N, Vigneron B, Diogo M, Vandel P, Laurent É. Saccadic eye movements: what do they tell us about aging cognition? AGING NEUROPSYCHOLOGY AND COGNITION 2016; 24:575-599. [DOI: 10.1080/13825585.2016.1237613] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Nicolas Noiret
- Laboratoire de Psychologie EA-3188, Université Bourgogne Franche-Comté, Besançon, France
- Centre Mémoire de Ressources et de Recherche (CMRR), Centre Hospitalier Régional Universitaire (CHRU) Jean Minjoz, Besançon, France
| | - Blanche Vigneron
- Laboratoire de Psychologie EA-3188, Université Bourgogne Franche-Comté, Besançon, France
| | - Marine Diogo
- Laboratoire de Psychologie EA-3188, Université Bourgogne Franche-Comté, Besançon, France
| | - Pierre Vandel
- Centre Mémoire de Ressources et de Recherche (CMRR), Centre Hospitalier Régional Universitaire (CHRU) Jean Minjoz, Besançon, France
- Laboratoire de Neurosciences EA-481, Université Bourgogne Franche-Comté, Besançon, France
| | - Éric Laurent
- Laboratoire de Psychologie EA-3188, Université Bourgogne Franche-Comté, Besançon, France
- Maison des Sciences de l’Homme et de l’Environnement (UMSR-3124), CNRS & Université Bourgogne Franche-Comté, Besançon, France
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Facing competition: Neural mechanisms underlying parallel programming of antisaccades and prosaccades. Brain Cogn 2016; 107:37-47. [DOI: 10.1016/j.bandc.2016.05.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/27/2016] [Accepted: 05/31/2016] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
|
24
|
Commodari E. Voluntary Attentional Orienting in Schoolchildren: How Visual Orienting Skills Change During Primary School. Percept Mot Skills 2016; 122:855-70. [PMID: 27226225 DOI: 10.1177/0031512516652034] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
Voluntary attentional orienting is the foundation of the capacity to highlight relevant stimuli and suppress competing distractions. It plays a role in the scholastic skills. This study investigated the changes in the orienting skills in 630 schoolchildren (age range: 5.7-10.2 years old), who attended the first (N = 205, M = 6.1, SD = 0.4), third (212 children, M = 8.2, SD = 0.6), and fifth grade (213 children, M = 10, SD = 0.4) of four primary schools in Italy. Simple attentional orienting, choice attentional orienting on written stimuli, double attentional orienting (auditory and visual), and re-orienting were investigated. The tasks required pressing a key in response to some target stimuli. The stimuli were digits, images of objects, written words, letters, geometric forms, and auditory verbal stimuli. Results showed that attentional orienting improved with age. The children's performances depended on the type of stimuli and activities required for the task. The accuracy of re-orienting with written verbal stimuli did not improve during primary school. With increasing age, children became faster but not more able to focus on written verbal stimuli when shifting of attention was required.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elena Commodari
- Department of Educational Processes, University of Catania, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Jóhannesson ÓI, Thornton IM, Smith IJ, Chetverikov A, Kristjánsson Á. Visual Foraging With Fingers and Eye Gaze. Iperception 2016; 7:2041669516637279. [PMID: 27433323 PMCID: PMC4934673 DOI: 10.1177/2041669516637279] [Citation(s) in RCA: 32] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
A popular model of the function of selective visual attention involves search where a single target is to be found among distractors. For many scenarios, a more realistic model involves search for multiple targets of various types, since natural tasks typically do not involve a single target. Here we present results from a novel multiple-target foraging paradigm. We compare finger foraging where observers cancel a set of predesignated targets by tapping them, to gaze foraging where observers cancel items by fixating them for 100 ms. During finger foraging, for most observers, there was a large difference between foraging based on a single feature, where observers switch easily between target types, and foraging based on a conjunction of features where observers tended to stick to one target type. The pattern was notably different during gaze foraging where these condition differences were smaller. Two conclusions follow: (a) The fact that a sizeable number of observers (in particular during gaze foraging) had little trouble switching between different target types raises challenges for many prominent theoretical accounts of visual attention and working memory. (b) While caveats must be noted for the comparison of gaze and finger foraging, the results suggest that selection mechanisms for gaze and pointing have different operational constraints.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Ian M Thornton
- Department of Cognitive Science, University of Malta, Malta
| | - Irene J Smith
- Faculty of Psychology, University of Iceland, Iceland
| | - Andrey Chetverikov
- Faculty of Psychology, University of Iceland, Iceland; Department of Psychology, Saint Petersburg State University, Saint Petersburg, Russia; Cognitive Research Lab, Russian Academy of National Economy and Public Administration, Moscow, Russia
| | - Árni Kristjánsson
- Faculty of Psychology, University of Iceland, Iceland; Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, UK
| |
Collapse
|
26
|
Pierce JE, McDowell JE. Effects of preparation time and trial type probability on performance of anti- and pro-saccades. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2016; 164:188-94. [PMID: 26829023 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2016.01.013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/29/2015] [Revised: 01/15/2016] [Accepted: 01/23/2016] [Indexed: 10/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Cognitive control optimizes responses to relevant task conditions by balancing bottom-up stimulus processing with top-down goal pursuit. It can be investigated using the ocular motor system by contrasting basic prosaccades (look toward a stimulus) with complex antisaccades (look away from a stimulus). Furthermore, the amount of time allotted between trials, the need to switch task sets, and the time allowed to prepare for an upcoming saccade all impact performance. In this study the relative probabilities of anti- and pro-saccades were manipulated across five blocks of interleaved trials, while the inter-trial interval and trial type cue duration were varied across subjects. Results indicated that inter-trial interval had no significant effect on error rates or reaction times (RTs), while a shorter trial type cue led to more antisaccade errors and faster overall RTs. Responses following a shorter cue duration also showed a stronger effect of trial type probability, with more antisaccade errors in blocks with a low antisaccade probability and slower RTs for each saccade task when its trial type was unlikely. A longer cue duration yielded fewer errors and slower RTs, with a larger switch cost for errors compared to a short cue duration. Findings demonstrated that when the trial type cue duration was shorter, visual motor responsiveness was faster and subjects relied upon the implicit trial probability context to improve performance. When the cue duration was longer, increased fixation-related activity may have delayed saccade motor preparation and slowed responses, guiding subjects to respond in a controlled manner regardless of trial type probability.
Collapse
|
27
|
Chetverikov A, Jóhannesson ÓI, Kristjánsson Á. Blaming the victims of your own mistakes: How visual search accuracy influences evaluation of stimuli. Cogn Emot 2014; 29:1091-106. [DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2014.968097] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Andrey Chetverikov
- Department of Psychology, Saint Petersburg State University, Saint Petersburg, Russia
| | - Ómar I. Jóhannesson
- Laboratory for Visual Perception and Visuomotor Control, Faculty of Psychology, School of Health Sciences, University of Iceland, Reykjavik, Iceland
| | - Árni Kristjánsson
- Laboratory for Visual Perception and Visuomotor Control, Faculty of Psychology, School of Health Sciences, University of Iceland, Reykjavik, Iceland
- Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London, UK
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
Commodari E, Di Blasi M. The role of the different components of attention on calculation skill. LEARNING AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES 2014. [DOI: 10.1016/j.lindif.2014.03.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
|
29
|
Phillipou A, Douglas J, Krieser D, Ayton L, Abel L. Changes in saccadic eye movement and memory function after mild closed head injury in children. Dev Med Child Neurol 2014; 56:337-45. [PMID: 24350895 DOI: 10.1111/dmcn.12345] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 10/11/2013] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Abstract
AIM The aim of this study was to determine whether volitional saccadic impairments are present in children with mild closed head injury (mCHI) and whether these deficits are predictive of ongoing cognitive impairment. METHOD We analysed a sample of 26 children with mCHI (20 males, 6 females; mean age 13y 1mo, SD 2y) and 29 age-matched comparison children (20 males, 9 females; mean age 12y 2mo, SD 2y). Participants completed a battery of saccadic eye movement tasks and a set of computer-based cognitive tasks at three time points: within 2 weeks of mCHI, and at 3 months and 6 months. RESULTS The group with mCHI made fewer errors on the antisaccade task at the first time point and showed increased latencies on prosaccades, correct antisaccades, and corrected antisaccade errors at the third time point (6mo). The group with mCHI also showed poorer performance on the cognitive tasks assessing memory. INTERPRETATION Even very mild, uncomplicated mCHI in children may persistently affect aspects of executive control and visual processing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Andrea Phillipou
- Department of Optometry & Vision Sciences, The University of Melbourne, Melbourne, Vic., Australia
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
30
|
Jóhannesson ÓI, Haraldsson HM, Kristjánsson Á. Modulation of antisaccade costs through manipulation of target-location probability: only under decisional uncertainty. Vision Res 2013; 93:62-73. [PMID: 24148874 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2013.10.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/20/2013] [Revised: 09/04/2013] [Accepted: 10/09/2013] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
Abstract
Latencies of antisaccades made in the direction opposite to a peripheral target are typically slower longer than of prosaccades towards such a target by 50-100 ms. Antisaccades have proved to be an important tool for diagnostic purposes in neurology, psychology and psychiatry, providing invaluable insights into attentional function, decision making and the functionality of eye movement control. Recent findings have suggested, however, that latency differences between pro- and antisaccades can be eliminated by manipulating target-location probabilities. Pro- and antisaccades were equally fast to locations where a target rarely appeared, a finding that may be of promise for more elaborate diagnoses of neurological and psychiatric illness and further understanding of the eye movement system. Here, we tested probability manipulations for a number of different pro- and antisaccade tasks of varied difficulty. Probability only modulated antisaccade costs in a difficult antisaccade task involving decisional uncertainty with low target saliency. For other tasks including standard ones from the literature, target-location probability asymmetries had minimal effects. Probability modulation of antisaccade costs may therefore reflect effects upon decision making rather than saccade generation. This may limit the usefulness of probability manipulations of antisaccades for diagnostic purposes in neurology, psychology and related disciplines.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ómar I Jóhannesson
- Laboratory for Visual Perception and Visuomotor Control, Faculty of Psychology, School of Health Sciences, University of Iceland, Reykjavik, Iceland.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
31
|
Noorani I, Carpenter RHS. Re-starting a neural race: anti-saccade correction. Eur J Neurosci 2013; 39:159-64. [DOI: 10.1111/ejn.12396] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/26/2013] [Revised: 09/17/2013] [Accepted: 09/19/2013] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Imran Noorani
- Department of Physiology, Development and Neuroscience; University of Cambridge; Cambridge UK
- Division of Neurosurgery; Wessex Neurological Centre; University Hospital Southampton; Southampton UK
| | - R. H. S. Carpenter
- Department of Physiology, Development and Neuroscience; University of Cambridge; Cambridge UK
| |
Collapse
|
32
|
Tseng P, Chang CF, Chiau HY, Liang WK, Liu CL, Hsu TY, Hung DL, Tzeng OJL, Juan CH. The dorsal attentional system in oculomotor learning of predictive information. Front Hum Neurosci 2013; 7:404. [PMID: 23935573 PMCID: PMC3731626 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00404] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/24/2013] [Accepted: 07/09/2013] [Indexed: 11/15/2022] Open
Abstract
The dorsal attentional network is known for its role in directing top-down visual attention toward task-relevant stimuli. This goal-directed nature of the dorsal network makes it a suitable candidate for processing and extracting predictive information from the visual environment. In this review we briefly summarize some of the findings that delineate the neural substrates that contribute to predictive learning at both levels within the dorsal attentional system: including the frontal eye field (FEF) and posterior parietal cortex (PPC). We also discuss the similarities and differences between these two regions when it comes to learning predictive information. The current findings from the literature suggest that the FEFs may be more involved in top-down spatial attention, whereas the parietal cortex is involved in processing task-relevant attentional influences driven by stimulus salience, both contribute to the processing of predictive cues at different time points.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Philip Tseng
- Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, National Central University Jhongli, Taiwan ; Laboratories for Cognitive Neuroscience, National Yang-Ming University Taipei, Taiwan
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
33
|
Aichert DS, Derntl B, Wöstmann NM, Groß JK, Dehning S, Cerovecki A, Möller HJ, Habel U, Riedel M, Ettinger U. Intact emotion–cognition interaction in schizophrenia patients and first-degree relatives: Evidence from an emotional antisaccade task. Brain Cogn 2013; 82:329-36. [DOI: 10.1016/j.bandc.2013.05.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/18/2012] [Revised: 05/07/2013] [Accepted: 05/28/2013] [Indexed: 01/01/2023]
|
34
|
Jóhannesson ÓI, Kristjánsson Á. Violating the main sequence: asymmetries in saccadic peak velocities for saccades into the temporal versus nasal hemifields. Exp Brain Res 2013; 227:101-10. [DOI: 10.1007/s00221-013-3490-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/12/2012] [Accepted: 03/14/2013] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
|
35
|
Antoniades C, Ettinger U, Gaymard B, Gilchrist I, Kristjánsson A, Kennard C, John Leigh R, Noorani I, Pouget P, Smyrnis N, Tarnowski A, Zee DS, Carpenter RHS. An internationally standardised antisaccade protocol. Vision Res 2013; 84:1-5. [PMID: 23474300 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2013.02.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 114] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/27/2012] [Revised: 02/08/2013] [Accepted: 02/09/2013] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
Detailed measurements of saccadic latency--the time taken to make an eye movement to a suddenly-presented visual target--have proved a valuable source of detailed and quantitative information in a wide range of neurological conditions, as well as shedding light on the mechanisms of decision, currently of intense interest to cognitive neuroscientists. However, there is no doubt that more complex oculomotor tasks, and in particular the antisaccade task in which a participant must make a saccade in the opposite direction to the target, are potentially more sensitive indicators of neurological dysfunction, particularly in neurodegenerative conditions. But two obstacles currently hinder their widespread adoption for this purpose. First, that much of the potential information from antisaccade experiments, notably about latency distribution and amplitude, is typically thrown away. Second, that there is no standardised protocol for carrying out antisaccade experiments, so that results from one laboratory cannot easily be compared with those from another. This paper, the outcome of a recent international meeting of oculomotor scientists and clinicians with an unusually wide experience of such measurements, sets out a proposed protocol for clinical antisaccade trials: its adoption will greatly enhance the clinical and scientific benefits of making these kinds of measurements.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chrystalina Antoniades
- Department of Physiology, Development and Neuroscience, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, CB2 3EG, UK
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
36
|
Noorani I, Carpenter RHS. Antisaccades as decisions: LATER model predicts latency distributions and error responses. Eur J Neurosci 2012; 37:330-8. [DOI: 10.1111/ejn.12025] [Citation(s) in RCA: 56] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/30/2012] [Revised: 09/06/2012] [Accepted: 09/19/2012] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- I. Noorani
- Department of Physiology; Development and Neuroscience; University of Cambridge; CB2 3EG; Cambridge; UK
| | - R. H. S. Carpenter
- Department of Physiology; Development and Neuroscience; University of Cambridge; CB2 3EG; Cambridge; UK
| |
Collapse
|
37
|
Saccade performance in the nasal and temporal hemifields. Exp Brain Res 2012; 219:107-20. [PMID: 22453719 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-012-3071-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/21/2011] [Accepted: 03/06/2012] [Indexed: 10/28/2022]
Abstract
There are numerous asymmetries in anatomy between the nasal and temporal hemiretinae, which have been connected to various asymmetries in behavioral performance. These include asymmetries in Vernier acuity, saccade selection, and attentional function, in addition to some evidence for latency differences for saccadic eye movements. There is also evidence for stronger retinotectal neural projection from the nasal than the temporal hemiretina. There is, accordingly, good reason to predict asymmetries in saccadic performance depending on which hemifield the saccade trigger stimuli are presented in, but the evidence on this is mixed. We tested for asymmetries in both saccade latency and landing point accuracy in a variety of different saccade tasks. We found no evidence for any asymmetries in saccade latency and only modest evidence for asymmetries in landing point accuracy. While this lack of asymmetry is surprising in light of previous findings of attentional asymmetries, it may reflect that cortical input to midbrain eye control centers mitigates any retinal and retinotectal asymmetry.
Collapse
|
38
|
|
39
|
Reinholdt-Dunne ML, Mogg K, Benson V, Bradley BP, Hardin MG, Liversedge SP, Pine DS, Ernst M. Anxiety and selective attention to angry faces: An antisaccade study. JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2012. [DOI: 10.1080/20445911.2011.560111] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/31/2022]
|
40
|
Revisiting the suitability of antisaccade performance as an endophenotype in schizophrenia. Brain Cogn 2011; 77:223-30. [DOI: 10.1016/j.bandc.2011.08.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/03/2010] [Revised: 07/26/2011] [Accepted: 08/04/2011] [Indexed: 02/06/2023]
|
41
|
van Steenbergen H, Band GPH, Hommel B. Threat but not arousal narrows attention: evidence from pupil dilation and saccade control. Front Psychol 2011; 2:281. [PMID: 22059081 PMCID: PMC3204575 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00281] [Citation(s) in RCA: 69] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/27/2011] [Accepted: 10/05/2011] [Indexed: 01/22/2023] Open
Abstract
It has been shown that negative affect causes attentional narrowing. According to Easterbrook’s (1959) influential hypothesis this effect is driven by the withdrawal motivation inherent to negative emotions and might be related to increases in arousal. We investigated whether valence-unspecific increases in physiological arousal, as measured by pupil dilation, could account for attentional narrowing effects in a cognitive control task. Following the presentation of a negative, positive, or neutral picture, participants performed a saccade task with a pro-saccade versus an anti-saccade instruction. The reaction time difference between pro- and anti-saccades was used to index attentional selectivity, and while pupil diameter was used as an index of physiological arousal. Pupil dilation was observed for both negative and positive pictures, which indicates increased physiological arousal. However, increased attentional selectivity was only observed following negative pictures. Our data show that motivational intensity effects on attentional narrowing can occur independently of physiological arousal effects.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Henk van Steenbergen
- Leiden Institute for Brain and Cognition and Leiden University Institute of Psychology, Leiden University Leiden, Netherlands
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
42
|
Is the relationship of prosaccade reaction times and antisaccade errors mediated by working memory? Exp Brain Res 2010; 208:385-97. [PMID: 21107543 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-010-2488-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/07/2009] [Accepted: 11/03/2010] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
|
43
|
Chapman BB, Corneil BD. Neuromuscular recruitment related to stimulus presentation and task instruction during the anti-saccade task. Eur J Neurosci 2010; 33:349-60. [DOI: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07496.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
|
44
|
Kristjánsson A, Eyjólfsdóttir KÓ, Jónsdóttir A, Arnkelsson G. Temporal consistency is currency in shifts of transient visual attention. PLoS One 2010; 5:e13660. [PMID: 21060888 PMCID: PMC2965655 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0013660] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/28/2010] [Accepted: 10/01/2010] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
Background Observers respond more accurately to targets in visual search tasks that share properties with previously presented items, and transient attention can learn featural consistencies on a precue, irrespective of its absolute location. Methodology/Principal Findings We investigated whether such attentional benefits also apply to temporal consistencies. Would performance on a precued Vernier acuity discrimination task, followed by a mask, improve if the cue-lead times (CLTs; 50, 100, 150 or 200 ms) remained constant between trials compared to when they changed? The results showed that if CLTs remained constant for a few trials in a row, Vernier acuity performance gradually improved while changes in CLT from one trial to the next led to worse than average discrimination performance. The results show that transient attention can quickly adjust to temporal regularities, similarly to spatial and featural regularities. Further experiments show that this form of learning is not under voluntary control. Conclusions/Significance The results add to a growing literature showing how consistency in visual presentation improves visual performance, in this case temporal consistency.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Arni Kristjánsson
- Department of Psychology, University of Iceland, Reykjavík, Iceland.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
45
|
Reuter B, Kaufmann C, Bender J, Pinkpank T, Kathmann N. Distinct neural correlates for volitional generation and inhibition of saccades. J Cogn Neurosci 2010; 22:728-38. [PMID: 19366286 DOI: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21235] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
Abstract
The antisaccade task has proven highly useful in basic and clinical neuroscience, and the neural structures involved are well documented. However, the cognitive and neural mechanisms that mediate task performance are not yet understood. An event-related fMRI study was designed to dissociate the neural correlates of two putative key functions, volitional saccade generation and inhibition of reflexive saccades, and to investigate their interaction. Nineteen healthy volunteers performed a task that required (a) to initiate saccades volitionally, either with or without a simultaneous demand to inhibit a reflexive saccade; and (b) to inhibit a reflexive saccade, either with or without a simultaneous demand to initiate a saccade volitionally. Analysis of blood oxygen level-dependent signal changes confirmed a major role of the frontal eye fields and the supplementary eye fields in volitional saccade generation. Inhibition-related activation of a specific fronto-parietal network was highly consistent with previous evidence involved in inhibitory processes. Unexpectedly, there was little evidence of specific brain activation during combined generation and inhibition demands, suggesting that the neural processing of generation and inhibition in antisaccades is independent to a large extent.
Collapse
|
46
|
Akaishi R, Morishima Y, Rajeswaren VP, Aoki S, Sakai K. Stimulation of the frontal eye field reveals persistent effective connectivity after controlled behavior. J Neurosci 2010; 30:4295-305. [PMID: 20335465 PMCID: PMC6634502 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.6198-09.2010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/15/2009] [Revised: 02/01/2010] [Accepted: 02/12/2010] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
Our ability to choose nonhabitual controlled behavior instead of habitual automatic behavior is based on a flexible control mechanism subserved by neural activity representing the behavior-guiding rule. However, it has been shown that the behavior slows down more when switching from controlled to automatic behavior than vice versa. Here we show that persistent effective connectivity of the neural network after execution of controlled behavior is responsible for the behavioral slowing on a subsequent trial. We asked normal human subjects to perform a prosaccade or antisaccade task based on a cue and examined the effective connectivity of the neural network based on the pattern of neural impulse transmission induced by stimulation of the frontal eye field (FEF). Effective connectivity during the task preparation period was dependent on the task that subjects had performed on the previous trial, regardless of the upcoming task. The strength of this persistent effective connectivity was associated with saccade slowing especially on trials after controlled antisaccade. In contrast, the pattern of regional activation changed depending on the upcoming task regardless of the previous task and the decrease in activation was associated with errors in upcoming antisaccade task. These results suggest that the effective connectivity examined by FEF stimulation reflects a residual functional state of the network involved in performance of controlled antisaccade and its persistence may account for the behavioral slowing on the subsequent trial.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | | | - Shigeki Aoki
- Radiology, Graduate School of Medicine, The University of Tokyo, Bunkyo-ku, Tokyo 113-0033, Japan
| | | |
Collapse
|
47
|
Liu CL, Chiau HY, Tseng P, Hung DL, Tzeng OJL, Muggleton NG, Juan CH. Antisaccade cost is modulated by contextual experience of location probability. J Neurophysiol 2009; 103:1438-47. [PMID: 20032240 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00815.2009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
It is well known that pro- and antisaccades may deploy different cognitive processes. However, the specific reason why antisaccades have longer latencies than prosaccades is still under debate. In three experiments, we studied the factors contributing to the antisaccade cost by taking attentional orienting and target location probabilities into account. In experiment 1, using a new antisaccade paradigm, we directly tested Olk and Kingstone's hypothesis, which attributes longer antisaccade latency to the time it takes to reorient from the visual target to the opposite saccadic target. By eliminating the reorienting component in our paradigm, we found no significant difference between the latencies of the two saccade types. In experiment 2, we varied the proportion of prosaccades made to certain locations and found that latencies in the high location-probability (75%) condition were faster than those in the low location-probability condition. Moreover, antisaccade latencies were significantly longer when location probability was high. This pattern can be explained by the notion of competing pathways for pro- and antisaccades in findings of others. In experiment 3, we further explored the degrees of modulation of location probability by decreasing the magnitude of high probability from 75 to 65%. We again observed a pattern similar to that seen in experiment 2 but with smaller modulation effects. Together, these experiments indicate that the reorienting process is a critical factor in producing the antisaccade cost. Furthermore, the antisaccade cost can be modulated by probabilistic contextual information such as location probabilities.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chia-Lun Liu
- Inst. of Cognitive Neuroscience, National Central Univ., No.300, Jhongda Rd., Jhongli City 320, Taiwan
| | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
48
|
Abstract
The saccadic eye movement system provides researchers with a powerful tool with which to explore the cognitive control of behaviour. It is a behavioural system whose limited output can be measured with exceptional precision, and whose input can be controlled and manipulated in subtle ways. A range of cognitive processes (notably those involved in working memory and attention) have been shown to influence saccade parameters. Researchers interested in the relationship between cognitive function and psychiatric disorders have made extensive use of saccadic eye movement tasks to draw inferences as to the cognitive deficits associated with particular psychopathologies. The purpose of this review is to provide researchers with an overview of the research literature documenting cognitive involvement in saccadic tasks in healthy controls. An appreciation of this literature provides a solid background against which to interpret the deficits on saccadic tasks demonstrated in patient populations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- S B Hutton
- Department of Psychology, University of Sussex, Pevensey 1 1c03, Brighton BN1 9RH, UK.
| |
Collapse
|
49
|
Franke C, Arndt D, Ploner CJ, Heinz A, Reuter B. Saccade generation and suppression in schizophrenia: effects of response switching and perseveration. Psychophysiology 2008; 45:698-704. [PMID: 18513361 DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00671.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
Abstract
Poor antisaccade performance is a reliable index of action control deficits in schizophrenia. To further elucidate the underlying cognitive impairments, the current study aimed to confirm effects of switching the response direction on saccadic performance and to investigate whether response switch effects relate to perseveration. Fourteen schizophrenia patients and 14 healthy controls performed sequences of 1 to 3 simple volitional saccades to one direction and a subsequent volitional saccade with distractor to the same or the opposite direction. Response switches increased error rates in schizophrenia if they followed 3 saccades to the opposite side, suggesting that response switching affects performance on conditions of strong persisting response programs. The increase of response switch error rates with multiple repetitions of the prior response points to a relationship between perseveration and response selection.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Cosima Franke
- Institut für Psychologie, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, 12489 Berlin, Germany.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
50
|
Kristjánsson Á, Sigurdardottir HM. On the Benefits of Transient Attention across the Visual Field. Perception 2008; 37:747-64. [DOI: 10.1068/p5922] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
Abstract
There are well-known differences in resolution and performance across the visual field with performance generally better for the lower than the upper visual hemifield. Here we attempted to assess how transient attention summoned by a peripheral precue affects performance across the visual field. Four different attentional precueing tasks were used, varying in difficulty and attentional load. When a single discrimination target was presented (experiments 1 and 2), precues that summon transient attention had very little, if any, effect upon performance. However, when the target was presented among distractors (experiments 3 and 4), the precue had a substantial effect upon discrimination performance. The results showed that asymmetries in visual resolution between the upper and lower hemifields become more pronounced with increasing eccentricity. Furthermore, when the observers performed a precued acuity task with distractors, involving the judgment of the relative position of a small disk within a larger one, there was an asymmetry in the transient attentional effect on discrimination performance; the benefits of transient attention were larger in the upper than in the lower hemifield. Areas in the visual field where visual performance is generally worse thus appear to receive the largest attentional boost when needed. Possible ecological explanations for this are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Árni Kristjánsson
- Department of Psychology, University of Iceland, Oddi v. Sturlugötu, 101 Reykjavik, Iceland
| | | |
Collapse
|