1
|
Sankhe P, Haruno M. Model-free decision-making underlies motor errors in rapid sequential movements under threat. COMMUNICATIONS PSYCHOLOGY 2024; 2:81. [PMID: 39242765 PMCID: PMC11347585 DOI: 10.1038/s44271-024-00123-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/11/2023] [Accepted: 07/30/2024] [Indexed: 09/09/2024]
Abstract
Our movements, especially sequential ones, are usually goal-directed, i.e., coupled with task-level goals. Consequently, cognitive strategies for decision-making and motor performance are likely to influence each other. However, evidence linking decision-making strategies and motor performance remains elusive. Here, we designed a modified version of the two-step task, named the two-step sequential movement task, where participants had to conduct rapid sequential finger movements to obtain rewards (n = 40). In the shock session, participants received an electrical shock if they made an erroneous or slow movement, while in the no-shock session, they only received zero reward. We found that participants who prioritised model-free decision-making committed more motor errors in the presence of the shock stimulus (shock sessions) than those who prioritised model-based decision-making. Using a mediation analysis, we also revealed a strong link between the balance of the model-based and the model-free learning strategies and sequential movement performances. These results suggested that model-free decision-making produces more motor errors than model-based decision-making in rapid sequential movements under the threat of stressful stimuli.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Pranav Sankhe
- Center for Information and Neural Networks, NICT, 1-4 Yamadaoka, Suita, Osaka, 565-0871, Japan.
- Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, 17-19 Queen Square, London, WC1N 3AZ, UK.
| | - Masahiko Haruno
- Center for Information and Neural Networks, NICT, 1-4 Yamadaoka, Suita, Osaka, 565-0871, Japan.
- Graduate School of Frontier Biosciences, Osaka University, 1-3 Yamadaoka, Suita, Osaka, 565-0871, Japan.
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Yang T, Liu Y, Li J, Xu H, Li S, Xiong L, Wang T. Advances in clinical basic research: Performance, treatments, and mechanisms of Parkinson disease. IBRAIN 2021; 7:362-378. [PMID: 37786563 PMCID: PMC10529016 DOI: 10.1002/ibra.12011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/10/2021] [Revised: 12/05/2021] [Accepted: 12/05/2021] [Indexed: 02/05/2023]
Abstract
The loss of neuronal in the substantia nigra of the elderly contributes to striatal damage and plays a critical part in the common forms of neurodegenerative diseases such as Parkinson disease (PD). The deficit of dopamine is one of the most familiar neuropathological features of PD as well as α-Synuclein aggregation. The peripheral autonomic nervous system is also affected negatively during the course of the disease, although the subsistent of dyskinesias and else major motor characteristic deficits take significant role in the diagnostic methods during clinical practice, which is related to a number of non-motor symptoms that might increase aggregate risks. Multiple pathways and mechanisms are involved in the molecular pathogenesis: α-Synuclein, neuronal homeostasis, mitochondrial function, oxidative stress, as well as neuroinflammation. Investigations in the last few years for diagnostic biomarkers used neuroimaging, including single photon emission computed tomography as well as cutting-edge magnetic resonance imaging techniques, which has been presented to facilitate discrepant diagnosis. Pharmacological treatment is also important and efficient in equal measure. In addition to reliance on striatal dopamine replacement therapy, many solutions that are used for motor or nonmotor symptoms in these patients are available.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ting‐Ting Yang
- Department of AnesthesiologyZunyi Medical UniversityZunyiGuizhouChina
| | - Yu‐Cong Liu
- Department of AnesthesiologyZunyi Medical UniversityZunyiGuizhouChina
| | - Jing Li
- Department of AnesthesiologyZunyi Medical UniversityZunyiGuizhouChina
| | - Hui‐Chan Xu
- Department of AnesthesiologyZunyi Medical UniversityZunyiGuizhouChina
| | - Shun‐Lian Li
- Department of AnesthesiologyZunyi Medical UniversityZunyiGuizhouChina
| | - Liu‐Lin Xiong
- Department of AnesthesiologyAffiliated Hospital of Zunyi Medical UniversityZunyiGuizhouChina
| | - Ting‐Hua Wang
- Department of Anesthesiology, Translational Neuroscience Center, Institute of Neurological Disease, West China HospitalSichuan UniversityChengduChina
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Sutil-Martín DL, Rienda-Gómez JJ. The Influence of Unconscious Perceptual Processing on Decision-Making: A New Perspective From Cognitive Neuroscience Applied to Generation Z. Front Psychol 2020; 11:1728. [PMID: 32903621 PMCID: PMC7438726 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01728] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/15/2020] [Accepted: 06/23/2020] [Indexed: 11/26/2022] Open
Abstract
Cognitive neuroscience and its applied developments have revolutionized marketing. With advances in neuroscientific techniques, marketing has needed to refocus toward understanding issues like the area of the brain that should be stimulated to transform the consumer’s intention to purchase into a real decision, how information is processed when making a decision, and how personality traits affect the purchase decision. Neuroscience has opened the door to the consumer’s brain. For many years, scientists have investigated the role of subliminal messages in marketing, with their findings generating a significant controversy. Many have shown that making sound decisions based on intuition rather than conscious reasoning is more common than previously thought. In fact, many studies have shown that sound intuitive decision-making depends on the association of the subliminal messages of a given situation with the limbic brain structures formed. Scientists have concluded that the brain does not consciously need to know contextual information to learn the value of this information and make the necessary linkages to make productive decisions. In this study, we consider whether unconscious perceptual processing influences decision-making and explore the influence of aspects of personality that are related to unconscious processing, such as the degree of neuroticism, extroversion, and gender of the individual, applied to the demographic cohort Generation Z, distinguishing between whether the stimuli are verbal or pictorial. The backward masking visual paradigm has been used to assess unconscious perceptual processing. To test these processes, a set of ANOVA models and logistic regressions were run where the dependent variable is whether the people perceived the stimuli or not and the independent variables were gender, the form of the stimuli (pictorial or verbal), and the personality traits extroversion, introversion, and neuroticism. The results suggest that verbal stimuli work better than pictorial stimuli, although a possible explanation is that the pictures require modification to be more effective. In the case of verbal stimuli, gender and level of neuroticism are found to be important variables that influence unconscious perceptual decision-making processes. Specifically, a female with a high level of neuroticism shows greater permeability in its unconscious perceptual processes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Juan José Rienda-Gómez
- Department of Financial Economics and Accounting and Modern Language, Rey Juan Carlos University, Móstoles, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
'Cognitive freezing': A newly recognized episodic phenomenon in Parkinson's disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 2019; 65:49-54. [PMID: 31178334 DOI: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2019.06.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/13/2019] [Revised: 06/01/2019] [Accepted: 06/04/2019] [Indexed: 11/21/2022]
|
5
|
Beversdorf DQ. Neuropsychopharmacological regulation of performance on creativity-related tasks. Curr Opin Behav Sci 2019; 27:55-63. [PMID: 31106256 PMCID: PMC6519931 DOI: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.09.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/20/2022]
Abstract
A number of factors affect performance on tasks associated with creativity. Two pharmacological systems in particularly been identified as important for their impact on creativity, the noradrenergic system and the dopaminergic systems. Furthermore, stress is also established as an important factor impacting performance, most likely mediated by its effects on these neurotransmitter systems. Herein, we review the current literature on the relationships between stress, the noradrenergic system, the dopaminergic system, and other pharmacological factors and their effects on performance on tasks associated with creativity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- David Q Beversdorf
- William and Nancy Thompson Endowed Chair in Radiology, University of Missouri
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Isaacs ML, McMahon KL, Angwin AJ, Copland DA. The Suppression of Irrelevant Semantic Representations in Parkinson's Disease. Front Hum Neurosci 2019; 12:511. [PMID: 30723399 PMCID: PMC6349768 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00511] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/16/2018] [Accepted: 12/05/2018] [Indexed: 11/17/2022] Open
Abstract
The impairment of lexical-semantic inhibition mechanisms in Parkinson's disease (PD) remains a source of contention. In order to observe whether people with PD are able to suppress irrelevant semantic information during picture naming, the present study employed an object-based negative priming paradigm with 16 participants with PD and 13 healthy controls. The task required participants to name a red target image while ignoring a superimposed, green distractor image. The semantic relationship between the distractor image and the target image of the subsequent trial was manipulated, such that the distractor image was identical, semantically related, or semantically unrelated to said target image. The PD group and the control group were slower in naming a target image that had previously served as a distractor image, relative to naming a target image that was unrelated to the previous distractor image. Thus, a negative priming effect was present in both groups. Furthermore, no significant difference in the magnitude of this effect was observed between the control and PD groups. When considered in the context of existing literature surrounding negative priming in PD, these results suggest that inhibition is subserved by multiple, domain-specific mechanisms and that the inhibitory processing of visual-semantic stimuli is intact in PD.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Megan L. Isaacs
- Centre for Clinical Research, University of Queensland, Herston, QLD, Australia
- School of Health and Rehabilitation Sciences, University of Queensland, Saint Lucia, QLD, Australia
| | - Katie L. McMahon
- School of Clinical Sciences and Institute of Health and Biomedical Innovation, Queensland University of Technology, Brisbane, QLD, Australia
| | - Anthony J. Angwin
- School of Health and Rehabilitation Sciences, University of Queensland, Saint Lucia, QLD, Australia
| | - David A. Copland
- Centre for Clinical Research, University of Queensland, Herston, QLD, Australia
- School of Health and Rehabilitation Sciences, University of Queensland, Saint Lucia, QLD, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Sengupta S, Medendorp WP, Praamstra P, Selen LPJ. Uncertainty modulated exploration in the trade-off between sensing and acting. PLoS One 2018; 13:e0199544. [PMID: 29979698 PMCID: PMC6034831 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0199544] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/05/2017] [Accepted: 06/08/2018] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Many sensorimotor activities have a time constraint for successful completion. In this case, any time devoted to sensory processing is at the expense of time available for motor execution. Earlier studies have explored how this competition between sensory processing and motor execution is resolved by using experimental designs that segregate the sensing and acting phase of the task. It was found that participants switch from the sensing to the acting stage such that the overall (sensorimotor) uncertainty in the outcome of the task is minimized. An unexplained observation in these studies is the substantial variability in switching times. We investigated the variability in switching time by correlating it with the underlying sensorimotor uncertainty. To this end, we used a modified version of the virtual ball catching paradigm proposed by Faisal & Wolpert (2009). Subjects were instructed to catch a ball, but as soon as they initiated their movement the ball disappeared. We extended the range of horizontal velocities and used two different start positions of the ball to quantify the dependence of sensory uncertainty on ball velocity. Velocity dependence of sensory uncertainty allowed us to manipulate sensory uncertainty and hence the sensorimotor uncertainty. We found that the variability in switching times is correlated with two factors. Firstly, variability in switching times is greater when variation in switching time has only minimal effects on sensorimotor uncertainty. Secondly, variability in switching times is greater when the sensorimotor uncertainty is larger. Our results suggest that the variability in switching time reflects an uncertainty-driven exploratory process.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sonal Sengupta
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition, and Behaviour, Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
- Department of Neurology, Radboud University Nijmegen Medical Centre, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | - W. Pieter Medendorp
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition, and Behaviour, Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | - Peter Praamstra
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition, and Behaviour, Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
- Department of Neurology, Radboud University Nijmegen Medical Centre, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | - Luc P. J. Selen
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition, and Behaviour, Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
- * E-mail:
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Yang XQ, Lauzon B, Seergobin KN, MacDonald PA. Dopaminergic Therapy Increases Go Timeouts in the Go/No-Go Task in Patients with Parkinson's Disease. Front Hum Neurosci 2018; 11:642. [PMID: 29354045 PMCID: PMC5758505 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00642] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/03/2017] [Accepted: 12/18/2017] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Parkinson's disease (PD) is characterized by resting tremor, rigidity and bradykinesia. Dopaminergic medications such as L-dopa treat these motor symptoms, but can have complex effects on cognition. Impulse control is an essential cognitive function. Impulsivity is multifaceted in nature. Motor impulsivity involves the inability to withhold pre-potent, automatic, erroneous responses. In contrast, cognitive impulsivity refers to improper risk-reward assessment guiding behavior. Informed by our previous research, we anticipated that dopaminergic therapy would decrease motor impulsivity though it is well known to enhance cognitive impulsivity. We employed the Go/No-go paradigm to assess motor impulsivity in PD. Patients with PD were tested using a Go/No-go task on and off their normal dopaminergic medication. Participants completed cognitive, mood, and physiological measures. PD patients on medication had a significantly higher proportion of Go trial Timeouts (i.e., trials in which Go responses were not completed prior to a deadline of 750 ms) compared to off medication (p = 0.01). No significant ON-OFF differences were found for Go trial or No-go trial response times (RTs), or for number of No-go errors. We interpret that dopaminergic therapy induces a more conservative response set, reflected in Go trial Timeouts in PD patients. In this way, dopaminergic therapy decreased motor impulsivity in PD patients. This is in contrast to the widely recognized effects of dopaminergic therapy on cognitive impulsivity leading in some patients to impulse control disorders. Understanding the nuanced effects of dopaminergic treatment in PD on cognitive functions such as impulse control will clarify therapeutic decisions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Xue Q. Yang
- MacDonald Lab, Brain and Mind Institute, University of Western Ontario, London, ON, Canada
| | - Brian Lauzon
- MacDonald Lab, Brain and Mind Institute, University of Western Ontario, London, ON, Canada
| | - Ken N. Seergobin
- MacDonald Lab, Brain and Mind Institute, University of Western Ontario, London, ON, Canada
| | - Penny A. MacDonald
- MacDonald Lab, Brain and Mind Institute, University of Western Ontario, London, ON, Canada
- Clinical Neurological Sciences, Schulich School of Medicine and Dentistry, University of Western Ontario, London, ON, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Nieuwhof F, Helmich RC. Entangled cerebral networks in Parkinson’s disease. Brain 2017; 140:2767-2769. [DOI: 10.1093/brain/awx267] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
|
10
|
Viejo G, Girard B, Procyk E, Khamassi M. Adaptive coordination of working-memory and reinforcement learning in non-human primates performing a trial-and-error problem solving task. Behav Brain Res 2017; 355:76-89. [PMID: 29061387 DOI: 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.09.030] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/15/2017] [Revised: 08/08/2017] [Accepted: 09/18/2017] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
Abstract
Accumulating evidence suggest that human behavior in trial-and-error learning tasks based on decisions between discrete actions may involve a combination of reinforcement learning (RL) and working-memory (WM). While the understanding of brain activity at stake in this type of tasks often involve the comparison with non-human primate neurophysiological results, it is not clear whether monkeys use similar combined RL and WM processes to solve these tasks. Here we analyzed the behavior of five monkeys with computational models combining RL and WM. Our model-based analysis approach enables to not only fit trial-by-trial choices but also transient slowdowns in reaction times, indicative of WM use. We found that the behavior of the five monkeys was better explained in terms of a combination of RL and WM despite inter-individual differences. The same coordination dynamics we used in a previous study in humans best explained the behavior of some monkeys while the behavior of others showed the opposite pattern, revealing a possible different dynamics of WM process. We further analyzed different variants of the tested models to open a discussion on how the long pretraining in these tasks may have favored particular coordination dynamics between RL and WM. This points towards either inter-species differences or protocol differences which could be further tested in humans.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Guillaume Viejo
- Sorbonne Universités, UPMC Univ Paris 06, CNRS, Institute of Intelligent Systems and Robotics (ISIR), F-75005 Paris, France; Montreal Neurological Institute and Hospital, 3801 University Street, Montreal, Quebec, Canada.
| | - Benoît Girard
- Sorbonne Universités, UPMC Univ Paris 06, CNRS, Institute of Intelligent Systems and Robotics (ISIR), F-75005 Paris, France
| | - Emmanuel Procyk
- University of Lyon, Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, INSERM, Stem Cell and Brain Research Institute, U1208 Lyon, France
| | - Mehdi Khamassi
- Sorbonne Universités, UPMC Univ Paris 06, CNRS, Institute of Intelligent Systems and Robotics (ISIR), F-75005 Paris, France
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Tremblay L, Worbe Y, Thobois S, Sgambato-Faure V, Féger J. Selective dysfunction of basal ganglia subterritories: From movement to behavioral disorders. Mov Disord 2015; 30:1155-70. [DOI: 10.1002/mds.26199] [Citation(s) in RCA: 144] [Impact Index Per Article: 14.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/09/2014] [Revised: 01/14/2015] [Accepted: 02/06/2015] [Indexed: 12/14/2022] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Léon Tremblay
- Centre de Neurosciences Cognitives-UMR 5229; CNRS-Université de Lyon 1; Bron France
| | - Yulia Worbe
- UPMC Université Paris 6, UMR-S975, CRICM-Centre de Recherche de l'Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle épinière; Paris France
| | - Stéphane Thobois
- Centre de Neurosciences Cognitives-UMR 5229; CNRS-Université de Lyon 1; Bron France
- Hospices Civils de Lyon, Hopital Neurologique Pierre Wertheimer, Neurologie C; Lyon France
- Université de Lyon, Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, Faculté de Médecine Lyon Sud Charles Mérieux; Lyon France
| | | | - Jean Féger
- UPMC Université Paris 6, UMR-S975, CRICM-Centre de Recherche de l'Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle épinière; Paris France
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Helmich RC, Thaler A, van Nuenen BFL, Gurevich T, Mirelman A, Marder KS, Bressman S, Orr-Urtreger A, Giladi N, Bloem BR, Toni I. Reorganization of corticostriatal circuits in healthy G2019S LRRK2 carriers. Neurology 2015; 84:399-406. [PMID: 25540317 PMCID: PMC4336002 DOI: 10.1212/wnl.0000000000001189] [Citation(s) in RCA: 59] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/28/2014] [Accepted: 09/30/2014] [Indexed: 11/15/2022] Open
Abstract
OBJECTIVE We investigated system-level corticostriatal changes in a human model of premotor Parkinson disease (PD), i.e., healthy carriers of the G2019S LRRK2 mutation that is associated with a markedly increased, age-dependent risk of developing PD. METHODS We compared 37 asymptomatic LRRK2 G2019S mutation carriers (age range 30-78 years) with 32 matched, asymptomatic nonmutation carriers (age range 30-74 years). Using fMRI, we tested the hypothesis that corticostriatal connectivity in premotor PD shifts from severely affected to less affected striatal subregions, as shown previously in symptomatic PD. Specifically, we predicted that in premotor PD, the shift in corticostriatal connectivity would follow the same gradient of striatal dopamine depletion known from overt PD, with the dorsoposterior putamen being more affected than the ventroanterior putamen. RESULTS The known parallel topology of corticostriatal loops was preserved in each group, but the topography of putamen connectivity shifted. In LRRK2 G2019S mutation carriers, the right inferior parietal cortex had reduced functional connectivity with the dorsoposterior putamen but increased connectivity with the ventroanterior putamen, as compared with noncarriers. This shift in functional connectivity increased with age in LRRK2 G2019S mutation carriers. CONCLUSIONS Asymptomatic LRRK2 G2019S mutation carriers show a reorganization of corticostriatal circuits that mirrors findings in idiopathic PD. These changes may reflect premotor basal ganglia dysfunction or circuit-level compensatory changes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rick C Helmich
- From the Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging (R.C.H., I.T.), Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Nijmegen; Department of Neurology (R.C.H., B.R.B.), Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical Center, Nijmegen, the Netherlands; Movement Disorders Unit, Department of Neurology (A.T., T.G., A.M., N.G.), and Genetic Institute (A.O.-U.), Tel Aviv Sourasky Medical Center; Sackler School of Medicine (A.T., T.G., A.O.-U., N.G.), Tel Aviv University, Israel; Department of Neurology (B.F.L.v.N.), Catharina Hospital, Eindhoven, the Netherlands; Columbia University (K.S.M.), Columbia University Medical Center, New York; and Beth Israel Medical Center (S.B.), New York, NY.
| | - Avner Thaler
- From the Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging (R.C.H., I.T.), Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Nijmegen; Department of Neurology (R.C.H., B.R.B.), Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical Center, Nijmegen, the Netherlands; Movement Disorders Unit, Department of Neurology (A.T., T.G., A.M., N.G.), and Genetic Institute (A.O.-U.), Tel Aviv Sourasky Medical Center; Sackler School of Medicine (A.T., T.G., A.O.-U., N.G.), Tel Aviv University, Israel; Department of Neurology (B.F.L.v.N.), Catharina Hospital, Eindhoven, the Netherlands; Columbia University (K.S.M.), Columbia University Medical Center, New York; and Beth Israel Medical Center (S.B.), New York, NY
| | - Bart F L van Nuenen
- From the Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging (R.C.H., I.T.), Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Nijmegen; Department of Neurology (R.C.H., B.R.B.), Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical Center, Nijmegen, the Netherlands; Movement Disorders Unit, Department of Neurology (A.T., T.G., A.M., N.G.), and Genetic Institute (A.O.-U.), Tel Aviv Sourasky Medical Center; Sackler School of Medicine (A.T., T.G., A.O.-U., N.G.), Tel Aviv University, Israel; Department of Neurology (B.F.L.v.N.), Catharina Hospital, Eindhoven, the Netherlands; Columbia University (K.S.M.), Columbia University Medical Center, New York; and Beth Israel Medical Center (S.B.), New York, NY
| | - Tanya Gurevich
- From the Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging (R.C.H., I.T.), Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Nijmegen; Department of Neurology (R.C.H., B.R.B.), Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical Center, Nijmegen, the Netherlands; Movement Disorders Unit, Department of Neurology (A.T., T.G., A.M., N.G.), and Genetic Institute (A.O.-U.), Tel Aviv Sourasky Medical Center; Sackler School of Medicine (A.T., T.G., A.O.-U., N.G.), Tel Aviv University, Israel; Department of Neurology (B.F.L.v.N.), Catharina Hospital, Eindhoven, the Netherlands; Columbia University (K.S.M.), Columbia University Medical Center, New York; and Beth Israel Medical Center (S.B.), New York, NY
| | - Anat Mirelman
- From the Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging (R.C.H., I.T.), Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Nijmegen; Department of Neurology (R.C.H., B.R.B.), Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical Center, Nijmegen, the Netherlands; Movement Disorders Unit, Department of Neurology (A.T., T.G., A.M., N.G.), and Genetic Institute (A.O.-U.), Tel Aviv Sourasky Medical Center; Sackler School of Medicine (A.T., T.G., A.O.-U., N.G.), Tel Aviv University, Israel; Department of Neurology (B.F.L.v.N.), Catharina Hospital, Eindhoven, the Netherlands; Columbia University (K.S.M.), Columbia University Medical Center, New York; and Beth Israel Medical Center (S.B.), New York, NY
| | - Karen S Marder
- From the Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging (R.C.H., I.T.), Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Nijmegen; Department of Neurology (R.C.H., B.R.B.), Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical Center, Nijmegen, the Netherlands; Movement Disorders Unit, Department of Neurology (A.T., T.G., A.M., N.G.), and Genetic Institute (A.O.-U.), Tel Aviv Sourasky Medical Center; Sackler School of Medicine (A.T., T.G., A.O.-U., N.G.), Tel Aviv University, Israel; Department of Neurology (B.F.L.v.N.), Catharina Hospital, Eindhoven, the Netherlands; Columbia University (K.S.M.), Columbia University Medical Center, New York; and Beth Israel Medical Center (S.B.), New York, NY
| | - Susan Bressman
- From the Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging (R.C.H., I.T.), Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Nijmegen; Department of Neurology (R.C.H., B.R.B.), Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical Center, Nijmegen, the Netherlands; Movement Disorders Unit, Department of Neurology (A.T., T.G., A.M., N.G.), and Genetic Institute (A.O.-U.), Tel Aviv Sourasky Medical Center; Sackler School of Medicine (A.T., T.G., A.O.-U., N.G.), Tel Aviv University, Israel; Department of Neurology (B.F.L.v.N.), Catharina Hospital, Eindhoven, the Netherlands; Columbia University (K.S.M.), Columbia University Medical Center, New York; and Beth Israel Medical Center (S.B.), New York, NY
| | - Avi Orr-Urtreger
- From the Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging (R.C.H., I.T.), Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Nijmegen; Department of Neurology (R.C.H., B.R.B.), Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical Center, Nijmegen, the Netherlands; Movement Disorders Unit, Department of Neurology (A.T., T.G., A.M., N.G.), and Genetic Institute (A.O.-U.), Tel Aviv Sourasky Medical Center; Sackler School of Medicine (A.T., T.G., A.O.-U., N.G.), Tel Aviv University, Israel; Department of Neurology (B.F.L.v.N.), Catharina Hospital, Eindhoven, the Netherlands; Columbia University (K.S.M.), Columbia University Medical Center, New York; and Beth Israel Medical Center (S.B.), New York, NY
| | - Nir Giladi
- From the Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging (R.C.H., I.T.), Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Nijmegen; Department of Neurology (R.C.H., B.R.B.), Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical Center, Nijmegen, the Netherlands; Movement Disorders Unit, Department of Neurology (A.T., T.G., A.M., N.G.), and Genetic Institute (A.O.-U.), Tel Aviv Sourasky Medical Center; Sackler School of Medicine (A.T., T.G., A.O.-U., N.G.), Tel Aviv University, Israel; Department of Neurology (B.F.L.v.N.), Catharina Hospital, Eindhoven, the Netherlands; Columbia University (K.S.M.), Columbia University Medical Center, New York; and Beth Israel Medical Center (S.B.), New York, NY
| | - Bastiaan R Bloem
- From the Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging (R.C.H., I.T.), Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Nijmegen; Department of Neurology (R.C.H., B.R.B.), Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical Center, Nijmegen, the Netherlands; Movement Disorders Unit, Department of Neurology (A.T., T.G., A.M., N.G.), and Genetic Institute (A.O.-U.), Tel Aviv Sourasky Medical Center; Sackler School of Medicine (A.T., T.G., A.O.-U., N.G.), Tel Aviv University, Israel; Department of Neurology (B.F.L.v.N.), Catharina Hospital, Eindhoven, the Netherlands; Columbia University (K.S.M.), Columbia University Medical Center, New York; and Beth Israel Medical Center (S.B.), New York, NY
| | - Ivan Toni
- From the Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging (R.C.H., I.T.), Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Nijmegen; Department of Neurology (R.C.H., B.R.B.), Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical Center, Nijmegen, the Netherlands; Movement Disorders Unit, Department of Neurology (A.T., T.G., A.M., N.G.), and Genetic Institute (A.O.-U.), Tel Aviv Sourasky Medical Center; Sackler School of Medicine (A.T., T.G., A.O.-U., N.G.), Tel Aviv University, Israel; Department of Neurology (B.F.L.v.N.), Catharina Hospital, Eindhoven, the Netherlands; Columbia University (K.S.M.), Columbia University Medical Center, New York; and Beth Israel Medical Center (S.B.), New York, NY
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Leventhal DK, Stoetzner C, Abraham R, Pettibone J, DeMarco K, Berke JD. Dissociable effects of dopamine on learning and performance within sensorimotor striatum. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2014; 4:43-54. [PMID: 24949283 DOI: 10.1016/j.baga.2013.11.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/20/2022]
Abstract
Striatal dopamine is an important modulator of current behavior, as seen in the rapid and dramatic effects of dopamine replacement therapy in Parkinson Disease (PD). Yet there is also extensive evidence that dopamine acts as a learning signal, modulating synaptic plasticity within striatum to affect future behavior. Disentangling these "performance" and "learning" functions is important for designing effective, long-term PD treatments. We conducted a series of unilateral drug manipulations and dopamine terminal lesions in the dorsolateral striatum of rats highly-trained to perform brief instructed head/neck movements (two-alternative forced choice task). Reaction times and accuracy were measured longitudinally to determine if task behavior changed immediately, progressed over time, and/or persisted after drug withdrawal. Enhanced dopamine signaling with amphetamine caused an immediate, nonprogressive, and bilateral decrease in reaction times (RT). The altered RT distributions were consistent with reduced distance to threshold in the linear approach to threshold with ergodic rate (LATER) model of decision-making. Conversely, the dopamine antagonist flupenthixol caused experience-dependent, persistent changes in RT and accuracy indicative of a "learning" effect. These RT distributions were consistent with a slowed rate of approach to decision threshold. Our results show that dopaminergic signaling makes dissociable contributions to current and future behavior even within a single striatal subregion, and provide important clues for both models of normal decision-making and the design of novel drug therapies in PD.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Daniel K Leventhal
- Department of Neurology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan 48109 ; Movement Disorders Program, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan 48109 ; Neuroscience Program, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan 48109
| | - Colin Stoetzner
- Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan 48109
| | - Rohit Abraham
- Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan 48109
| | - Jeff Pettibone
- Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan 48109
| | - Kayla DeMarco
- Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan 48109
| | - Joshua D Berke
- Movement Disorders Program, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan 48109 ; Neuroscience Program, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan 48109 ; Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan 48109
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Praamstra P, Loing AF, de Lange FP. Leakage of decision uncertainty into movement execution in Parkinson's disease? Exp Brain Res 2013; 232:21-30. [PMID: 24091773 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-013-3715-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/04/2013] [Accepted: 09/18/2013] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
Abstract
The concept of segregated basal ganglia-cortical loops entails that functional disturbances may result from abnormal processing within loops, but also from abnormal communication between loops. Cognitive and motor processes subserved by different basal ganglia-frontal loops may interfere with one another as a result of such abnormal communication, leakage, between loops. In Parkinson's disease, movement execution has been found susceptible to decision uncertainty, attributed to this mechanism. Here, we evaluate whether this mechanism of abnormal coupling or leakage extends to perceptual decision-making with trial-by-trial control of decision uncertainty. We examined 10 Parkinson's disease (PD) patients and healthy control subjects in a random-dot motion direction discrimination task with concurrent EEG recording. Random-dot motion was manipulated to make direction discrimination easy or difficult. Reaction times (RT) and movement times (MT) were recorded, and EEG was analysed to extract movement-related potentials. Easy versus difficult direction discrimination produced robust, equally large RT differences in patients and controls (>400 ms), along with a marked difference in error rates, confirming the efficacy of the task. Effects of easy versus difficult discrimination on MT were comparatively small (<50 ms) and did not differ between groups, despite robustly slower MT in patients. Lateralised movement-related EEG potentials reproduced the MT difference between patients and controls. Together, the results do not demonstrate an enhanced effect of decision uncertainty onto movement execution in PD. We surmise that leakage of decision uncertainty into movement execution is probably task-dependent, consistent with the view that the degree to which partial information is allowed to influence the motor system is under strategic control.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Peter Praamstra
- Department of Neurology, Radboud University Medical Centre, Radboud University Nijmegen, PO Box 9101, 6500 HB, Nijmegen, The Netherlands,
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
15
|
Solari N, Bonito-Oliva A, Fisone G, Brambilla R. Understanding cognitive deficits in Parkinson's disease: lessons from preclinical animal models. Learn Mem 2013; 20:592-600. [PMID: 24049188 DOI: 10.1101/lm.032029.113] [Citation(s) in RCA: 41] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
Abstract
Parkinson's disease (PD) has been, until recently, mainly defined by the presence of characteristic motor symptoms, such as rigidity, tremor, bradykinesia/akinesia, and postural instability. Accordingly, pharmacological and surgical treatments have so far addressed these motor disturbances, leaving nonmotor, cognitive deficits an unmet clinical condition. At the preclinical level, the large majority of studies aiming at defining mechanisms and testing novel therapies have similarly focused on the motor aspects of PD. Unfortunately, deterioration of the executive functions, such as attention, recognition, working memory, and problem solving, often appear in an early, premotor phase of the disease and progressively increase in intensity, negatively affecting the quality of life of ∼50%-60% of PD patients. At present, the cellular mechanisms underlying cognitive impairments in PD patients are largely unknown and an adequate treatment is still missing. The preclinical research has recently developed new animal models that may open new perspectives for a more integrated approach to the treatment of both motor and cognitive symptoms of the disease. This review will provide an overview on the cognitive symptoms occurring in early PD patients and then focus on the rodent and nonhuman primate models so far available for the study of discriminative and spatial memory attention and learning abilities related to this pathological condition.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nicola Solari
- Division of Neuroscience, Institute of Experimental Neurology, San Raffaele Scientific Institute and University, 20132 Milano, Italy
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
16
|
Buelow MT, Frakey LL, Grace J, Friedman JH. The Contribution of Apathy and Increased Learning Trials to Risky Decision-Making in Parkinson's Disease. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2013; 29:100-9. [PMID: 23969088 DOI: 10.1093/arclin/act065] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/14/2022] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Melissa T Buelow
- Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University Newark, Newark, OH, USA
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
17
|
Lindgren HS, Dunnett SB. Cognitive dysfunction and depression in Parkinson's disease: what can be learned from rodent models? Eur J Neurosci 2012; 35:1894-907. [PMID: 22708601 DOI: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.08162.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 39] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/02/2023]
Abstract
Parkinson's disease (PD) has for decades been considered a pure motor disorder and its cardinal motor symptoms have been attributed to the loss of dopaminergic (DAergic) neurons in the substantia nigra pars compacta and to nigral Lewy body pathology. However, there has more recently been a shift in the conceptualization of the disease, and its pathological features have now been recognized as involving several other areas of the brain and indeed even outside the central nervous system. There are a corresponding variety of intrinsic non-motor symptoms such as autonomic dysfunction, cognitive impairment, sleep disturbances and neuropsychiatric problems, which cannot be explained exclusively by nigral pathology. In this review, we will focus on cognitive impairment and affective symptoms in PD, and we will consider whether, and how, these deficits can best be modelled in rodent models of the disorder. As only a few of the non-motor symptoms respond to standard DA replacement therapies, the quest for a broader therapeutic approach remains a major research effort, and success in this area in particular will be strongly dependent on appropriate rodent models. In addition, better understanding of the different models, as well as the advantages and disadvantages of the available behavioural tasks, will result in better tools for evaluating new treatment strategies for PD patients suffering from these neuropsychological symptoms.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hanna S Lindgren
- Brain Repair Group, School of Biosciences, Cardiff University, Life Sciences Building, Museum Avenue, Cardiff, Wales, CF10 3AX, UK.
| | | |
Collapse
|
18
|
Knott VJ, Millar AM, McIntosh JF, Shah DK, Fisher DJ, Blais CM, Ilivitsky V, Horn E. Separate and combined effects of low dose ketamine and nicotine on behavioural and neural correlates of sustained attention. Biol Psychol 2011; 88:83-93. [PMID: 21742012 DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.06.012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/04/2010] [Revised: 06/21/2011] [Accepted: 06/24/2011] [Indexed: 01/08/2023]
Abstract
Given the cognitive-promoting properties of the nicotinic acetylcholinergic receptor (nAChR) agonist, nicotine, the increased prevalence of smoke-inhaled nicotine in schizophrenia has been interpreted as an attempt to self-correct cognitive deficits, which have been particularly pronounced in the attentional domain. As glutamatergic abnormalities have been implicated in these attentional deficiencies, this study attempted to shed light on the separate and interactive roles of the N-methyl-d-aspartate receptor (NMDAR) and nAChR systems in the modulation of attention by investigating, in healthy volunteers, the separate and combined effects of nicotine and the NMDAR antagonist ketamine on neural and behavioural responses in a sustained attention task. In a randomized, double-blind, placebo controlled study, performance and the P300 event-related brain potential (ERP) in a visual information processing (RVIP) task were examined in 20 smokers and 20 non-smokers (both male and female). Assessment involved intravenous injection of a low subperceptual bolus dose (.04mg/kg) of ketamine or placebo, which was accompanied by acute treatment with nicotine (4mg) or placebo gum. Nicotine-enhanced attentional processing was most evident in nonsmokers, with both performance accuracy and P300 amplitude measures. Ketamine's detrimental effects on these behavioural and electrophysiologic measures were negatively moderated by acute nicotine, the synergistic effects being expressed differently in smokers and nonsmokers. These findings support the view that acute alterations and individual differences in nAChR function can moderate even subtle glutamatergic-driven cognitive deficiencies in schizophrenia and can be important therapeutic targets for treating cognitive impairments in schizophrenia.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Verner J Knott
- University of Ottawa Institute of Mental Health Research, Ottawa, ON, Canada.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
19
|
Keramati M, Dezfouli A, Piray P. Speed/accuracy trade-off between the habitual and the goal-directed processes. PLoS Comput Biol 2011; 7:e1002055. [PMID: 21637741 PMCID: PMC3102758 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002055] [Citation(s) in RCA: 196] [Impact Index Per Article: 14.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/14/2010] [Accepted: 03/31/2011] [Indexed: 12/04/2022] Open
Abstract
Instrumental responses are hypothesized to be of two kinds: habitual and goal-directed, mediated by the sensorimotor and the associative cortico-basal ganglia circuits, respectively. The existence of the two heterogeneous associative learning mechanisms can be hypothesized to arise from the comparative advantages that they have at different stages of learning. In this paper, we assume that the goal-directed system is behaviourally flexible, but slow in choice selection. The habitual system, in contrast, is fast in responding, but inflexible in adapting its behavioural strategy to new conditions. Based on these assumptions and using the computational theory of reinforcement learning, we propose a normative model for arbitration between the two processes that makes an approximately optimal balance between search-time and accuracy in decision making. Behaviourally, the model can explain experimental evidence on behavioural sensitivity to outcome at the early stages of learning, but insensitivity at the later stages. It also explains that when two choices with equal incentive values are available concurrently, the behaviour remains outcome-sensitive, even after extensive training. Moreover, the model can explain choice reaction time variations during the course of learning, as well as the experimental observation that as the number of choices increases, the reaction time also increases. Neurobiologically, by assuming that phasic and tonic activities of midbrain dopamine neurons carry the reward prediction error and the average reward signals used by the model, respectively, the model predicts that whereas phasic dopamine indirectly affects behaviour through reinforcing stimulus-response associations, tonic dopamine can directly affect behaviour through manipulating the competition between the habitual and the goal-directed systems and thus, affect reaction time. When confronted with different alternatives, animals can respond either based on their pre-established habits, or by considering the short- and long-term consequences of each option. Whereas habitual decision making is fast, goal-directed thinking is a time-consuming task. Instead, habits are inflexible after being consolidated, but goal-directed decision making can rapidly adapt the animal's strategy after a change in environmental conditions. Based on these features of the two decision making systems, we suggest a computational model using the reinforcement learning framework, that makes a balance between the speed of decision making and behavioural flexibility. The behaviour of the model is consistent with the observation that at the early stages of learning, animals behave in a goal-directed way (flexible, but slow), but after extensive learning, their responses become habitual (inflexible, but fast). Moreover, the model explains that the animal's reaction time must decrease through the course of learning, as the habitual system takes control over behaviour. The model also attributes a functional role to the tonic activity of dopamine neurons in balancing the competition between the habitual and the goal-directed systems.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mehdi Keramati
- School of Management and Economics, Sharif University of Technology, Tehran, Iran.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
20
|
Neuropsychological and clinical heterogeneity of cognitive impairment and dementia in patients with Parkinson's disease. Lancet Neurol 2010; 9:1200-1213. [PMID: 20880750 DOI: 10.1016/s1474-4422(10)70212-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 636] [Impact Index Per Article: 42.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/20/2022]
|
21
|
Hanna-Pladdy B, Heilman KM. Dopaminergic modulation of the planning phase of skill acquisition in Parkinson's disease. Neurocase 2010; 16:182-90. [PMID: 20013480 PMCID: PMC4354687 DOI: 10.1080/13554790903379609] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/01/2023]
Abstract
Patients with Parkinson's disease (PD) are often impaired when performing motor acts and in the acquisition of new motor skills. However, the role of dopamine in developing plans for skill acquisition is unclear. To assess the role of dopamine on the planning of actions, we tested 12 PD and 12 matched normal participants on two skill acquisition tasks matched for motor demands, but varying in requirements for planning. The participants with PD were tested on these tasks when they were on and off dopaminergic medications. To minimize influence of movement related deficits, the subjects used a computer track-pointer that generated the required straight lines when the subjects applied a slight force and clicked the track-pointer to initiate and terminate each line segment. The amount of time the track-pointer was deflected determined the line lengths, while clicking of the mouse determined the location of the line. The simple figure replication task only required the subjects to repeatedly generate lines of two sizes, while the complex figure replication task required subjects to generate lines of different sizes. Thus, this complex task demanded more anticipatory planning. Compared to controls, the subjects with PD were slower to learn the programs needed to produce these figures and produced figures with reduced amplitudes on both the simple and complex tasks. Dopamine treatment, however, only improved the speed of figure completion on the complex task, suggesting that dopamine is important in action planning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Brenda Hanna-Pladdy
- Landon Center on Aging, University of Kansas Medical Center, Kansas City, KS, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
22
|
Albert NB, Peiris Y, Cohen G, Miall RC, Praamstra P. Interference effects from observed movement in Parkinson's disease. J Mot Behav 2010; 42:145-9. [PMID: 20207605 PMCID: PMC4962902 DOI: 10.1080/00222891003612805] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
Abstract
Previous research has demonstrated that Parkinson's disease patients have an increased susceptibility to response conflict. In the present study, the authors investigate whether Parkinson's patients have a similar sensitivity to interference from observed movements. In all, 10 patients and 10 controls performed horizontal and vertical arm movements while watching a video of either a person performing similar movements or a moving dot. Movements were performed in the same plane (congruent) and orthogonal to the observed movement (incongruent). The off-axis variance of movements was our index of interference. Although patients tended to exhibit more off-axis variability than did controls, both groups demonstrated similar congruence effects, with greater variance in incongruent conditions. These results indicated that increased susceptibility to interference in Parkinson's disease does not extend to interference from observed movements.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Neil B. Albert
- Behavioural Brain Sciences Centre, University of Birmingham, UK
- Department of Psychology, University of Chicago, USA
| | - Yasmin Peiris
- Behavioural Brain Sciences Centre, University of Birmingham, UK
| | - Georgia Cohen
- Behavioural Brain Sciences Centre, University of Birmingham, UK
| | - R. Chris Miall
- Behavioural Brain Sciences Centre, University of Birmingham, UK
| | - Peter Praamstra
- Behavioural Brain Sciences Centre, University of Birmingham, UK
- Department of Neurology, Queen Elizabeth Hospital, University of Birmingham, UK
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Pharmacological modulation of subliminal learning in Parkinson's and Tourette's syndromes. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2009; 106:19179-84. [PMID: 19850878 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.0904035106] [Citation(s) in RCA: 103] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Theories of instrumental learning aim to elucidate the mechanisms that integrate success and failure to improve future decisions. One computational solution consists of updating the value of choices in proportion to reward prediction errors, which are potentially encoded in dopamine signals. Accordingly, drugs that modulate dopamine transmission were shown to impact instrumental learning performance. However, whether these drugs act on conscious or subconscious learning processes remains unclear. To address this issue, we examined the effects of dopamine-related medications in a subliminal instrumental learning paradigm. To assess generality of dopamine implication, we tested both dopamine enhancers in Parkinson's disease (PD) and dopamine blockers in Tourette's syndrome (TS). During the task, patients had to learn from monetary outcomes the expected value of a risky choice. The different outcomes (rewards and punishments) were announced by visual cues, which were masked such that patients could not consciously perceive them. Boosting dopamine transmission in PD patients improved reward learning but worsened punishment avoidance. Conversely, blocking dopamine transmission in TS patients favored punishment avoidance but impaired reward seeking. These results thus extend previous findings in PD to subliminal situations and to another pathological condition, TS. More generally, they suggest that pharmacological manipulation of dopamine transmission can subconsciously drive us to either get more rewards or avoid more punishments.
Collapse
|
24
|
Helmich RC, Derikx LC, Bakker M, Scheeringa R, Bloem BR, Toni I. Spatial remapping of cortico-striatal connectivity in Parkinson's disease. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2009; 20:1175-86. [PMID: 19710357 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhp178] [Citation(s) in RCA: 319] [Impact Index Per Article: 19.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/18/2022]
Abstract
Parkinson's disease (PD) is characterized by striatal dopamine depletion, especially in the posterior putamen. The dense connectivity profile of the striatum suggests that these local impairments may propagate throughout the whole cortico-striatal network. Here we test the effect of striatal dopamine depletion on cortico-striatal network properties by comparing the functional connectivity profile of the posterior putamen, the anterior putamen, and the caudate nucleus between 41 PD patients and 36 matched controls. We used multiple regression analyses of resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging data to quantify functional connectivity across different networks. Each region had a distinct connectivity profile that was similarly expressed in patients and controls: the posterior putamen was uniquely coupled to cortical motor areas, the anterior putamen to the pre-supplementary motor area and anterior cingulate cortex, and the caudate nucleus to the dorsal prefrontal cortex. Differences between groups were specific to the putamen: although PD patients showed decreased coupling between the posterior putamen and the inferior parietal cortex, this region showed increased functional connectivity with the anterior putamen. We conclude that dopamine depletion in PD leads to a remapping of cerebral connectivity that reduces the spatial segregation between different cortico-striatal loops. These alterations of network properties may underlie abnormal sensorimotor integration in PD.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rick C Helmich
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging, Radboud University Nijmegen, 6500 HB Nijmegen, the Netherlands.
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
25
|
Dywan J, Mathewson KJ, Choma BL, Rosenfeld B, Segalowitz SJ. Autonomic and electrophysiological correlates of emotional intensity in older and younger adults. Psychophysiology 2008; 45:389-97. [PMID: 18221446 DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00637.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 34] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/30/2022]
Abstract
Anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) is involved in the modulation of autonomic activity, emotional responsivity, and the monitoring of goal-directed behavior. However, these functions are rarely studied together to determine how they relate or whether their pattern of relation changes with age. We recorded respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA), an index of autonomic activity, error-related event related potentials (ERN/Pe), generated in ACC, and the self-reported intensity of 5 basic emotions in older and younger adults. Emotional intensity did not differ with age. The ERN/Pe and RSA were reduced with age and related specifically to sadness intensity for both groups. When examined together, RSA accounted for the relation between ERN/Pe and sadness. This is consistent with a model of medial prefrontal function in which autonomic processes mediate the relation between cognitive control and affective regulation, a pattern that also did not differ with age.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jane Dywan
- Department of Psychology, Brock University, St. Catharines, Ontario, Canada.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
26
|
Laser literature watch. Photomed Laser Surg 2006; 24:424-53. [PMID: 16875454 DOI: 10.1089/pho.2006.24.424] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
|