1
|
Clarke S, Qu C, Luzzi F, Brannon E. Children's Number Judgments Are Influenced by Connectedness. Dev Sci 2025; 28:e70032. [PMID: 40420823 DOI: 10.1111/desc.70032] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/10/2024] [Revised: 04/24/2025] [Accepted: 04/27/2025] [Indexed: 05/28/2025]
Abstract
Visual illusions provide a means of investigating the rules and principles through which approximate number representations are formed. Here, we investigated the developmental trajectory of an important numerical illusion-the connectedness illusion, wherein connecting pairs of items with thin lines reduces perceived number without altering continuous attributes of the collections. We found that children as young as 5 years of age showed susceptibility to the illusion and that the magnitude of the effect increased into adulthood. Moreover, individuals with greater numerical acuity exhibited stronger connectedness illusions after controlling for age. Overall, these results suggest the approximate number system expects to enumerate over bounded wholes and doing so is a signature of its optimal functioning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sam Clarke
- Department of Philosophy, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, USA
| | - Chuyan Qu
- Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, USA
| | - Francesca Luzzi
- Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, USA
| | - Elizabeth Brannon
- Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, USA
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Mazuz Y, Hadad BS, Ganel T. Intact Susceptibility to Visual Illusions in Autistic Individuals. Autism Res 2025. [PMID: 40259703 DOI: 10.1002/aur.70044] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/13/2024] [Revised: 03/20/2025] [Accepted: 04/03/2025] [Indexed: 04/23/2025]
Abstract
Altered sensory perception, a core characteristic of autism, has been attributed to attenuated use of stimuli context or prior information in perception. Reduced susceptibility to perceptual illusions was extensively used to support these accounts for autistic perception. However, empirical evidence has been inconsistent. The current study systematically investigated susceptibility to size illusions in autistic and non-autistic individuals using a standardized psychophysical battery. Eighty-one participants, 41 autistic and 40 non-autistic individuals, completed the Ben-Gurion University Test for Perceptual Illusions (BTPI), measuring susceptibility to the Ponzo, Ebbinghaus, and Height-width illusions. The results demonstrate clear evidence for susceptibility to illusions in the perception of size both in the autistic and non-autistic groups. No significant differences were found between groups in the magnitude of illusion on the perceived size, or on the perceptual resolutions of size (discrimination thresholds) in any of the illusory settings tested. The results challenge current theories suggesting reduced reliance on priors or enhanced sensory measurement in autism. Instead, using robust psychophysical methods, the study provides clear evidence for autistic people forming priors and using long-term knowledge in perception.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yarden Mazuz
- Department of Psychology, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beer-Sheva, Israel
| | - Bat-Sheva Hadad
- Department of Special Education and the Edmond J. Safra Brain Research Center, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel
| | - Tzvi Ganel
- Department of Psychology, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beer-Sheva, Israel
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Bratzke D. Ebbinghaus, Müller-Lyer, and Ponzo: Three examples of bidirectional space-time interference. Psychon Bull Rev 2024; 31:2285-2292. [PMID: 38519757 PMCID: PMC11543825 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-024-02491-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 03/08/2024] [Indexed: 03/25/2024]
Abstract
Previous studies have shown interference between illusory size and perceived duration. The present study replicated this space-time interference in three classic visual-spatial illusions, the Ebbinghaus, the Müller-Lyer, and the Ponzo illusion. The results showed bidirectional interference between illusory size and duration for all three illusions. That is, subjectively larger stimuli were judged to be presented longer, and stimuli that were presented longer were judged to be larger. Thus, cross-dimensional interference between illusory size and duration appears to be a robust phenomenon and to generalize across a wide range of visual size illusions. This space-time interference most likely arises at the memory level and supports the theoretical notion of a common representational metric for space and time.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Daniel Bratzke
- Department of Psychology, University of Bremen, Bremen, Germany.
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Caparos S, Boissin E. The relationships between urbanicity, general cognitive ability, and susceptibility to the Ebbinghaus illusion. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2024; 88:1540-1549. [PMID: 38581438 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-01959-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/12/2023] [Accepted: 03/22/2024] [Indexed: 04/08/2024]
Abstract
Previous studies have shown that, in samples of non-Western observers, susceptibility to the Ebbinghaus illusion is stronger in urban than rural dwellers. While such relationship between illusion strength and urbanicity has often been ascribed to external factors (such as the visual impact of the environment), the present study explored the possibility that it is instead mediated by general cognitive ability, an internal factor. We recruited a sample of remote Namibians who varied in their level of urbanicity, and measured their susceptibility to the Ebbinghaus illusion, their levels of education and literacy, and their general cognitive ability. The results showed that urbanicity was related to Ebbinghaus susceptibility, and that general cognitive ability, literacy and education did not mediate this effect, which is reassuring with regard to the findings of previous studies that did not control for these variables. However, we found robust relationships between urbanicity, on the one hand, and cognitive ability, education and literacy, on the other, which advocates for careful consideration of the impact of the latter variables in studies about the cognitive effects of urban environments.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Serge Caparos
- Laboratoire DysCo, Université Paris 8, Serge Caparos, 2 Rue de la Liberté, Saint-Denis, 93200, France.
- Institut Universitaire de France, Paris, France.
| | | |
Collapse
|
5
|
Kramer P. Iconic logic: the visual art of drawing the right conclusion. Front Psychol 2024; 15:1368989. [PMID: 38911953 PMCID: PMC11190960 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1368989] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/11/2024] [Accepted: 05/09/2024] [Indexed: 06/25/2024] Open
Abstract
Most people, evidence suggests, have a hard time thinking straight. Symbolic logic is a tool that can help remedy this problem. Unfortunately, it is highly abstract and uses symbols whose meanings rely on unintuitive arbitrary conventions. Without sacrificing rigor, iconic logic is more concrete and uses icons that resemble what they stand for and whose meanings are thus easier to picture, process, and remember. Here I review and critique iconic existential graphs and concept diagrams-the former link iconic logic to iconic mathematics; the latter expand popular Euler or Venn diagrams and have, to some degree, been empirically investigated for user-friendliness. I lay out how expertise in perception, cognition, and genetics can inform and improve such empirical research to help make iconic logic more ergonomic. After all, logic is a tool, and tools should not only suit their use but also their user.
Collapse
|
6
|
Wincza R, Hartley C, Readman M, Linkenauger S, Crawford T. Susceptibility to geometrical visual illusions in Parkinson's disorder. Front Psychol 2024; 14:1289160. [PMID: 38259525 PMCID: PMC10800652 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1289160] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/05/2023] [Accepted: 12/15/2023] [Indexed: 01/24/2024] Open
Abstract
Parkinson's disorder (PD) is a common neurodegenerative disorder affecting approximately 1-3% of the population aged 60 years and older. In addition to motor difficulties, PD is also marked by visual disturbances, including depth perception, abnormalities in basal ganglia functioning, and dopamine deficiency. Reduced ability to perceive depth has been linked to an increased risk of falling in this population. The purpose of this paper was to determine whether disturbances in PD patients' visual processing manifest through atypical performance on visual illusion (VI) tasks. This insight will advance understanding of high-level perception in PD, as well as indicate the role of dopamine deficiency and basal ganglia pathophysiology in VIs susceptibility. Groups of 28 PD patients (Mage = 63.46, SD = 7.55) and 28 neurotypical controls (Mage = 63.18, SD = 9.39) matched on age, general cognitive abilities (memory, numeracy, attention, language), and mood responded to Ebbinghaus, Ponzo, and Müller-Lyer illusions in a computer-based task. Our results revealed no reliable differences in VI susceptibility between PD and neurotypical groups. In the early- to mid-stage of PD, abnormalities of the basal ganglia and dopamine deficiency are unlikely to be involved in top-down processing or depth perception, which are both thought to be related to VI susceptibility. Furthermore, depth-related issues experienced by PD patients (e.g., increased risk for falling) may not be subserved by the same cognitive mechanisms as VIs. Further research is needed to investigate if more explicit presentations of illusory depth are affected in PD, which might help to understand the depth processing deficits in PD.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Radoslaw Wincza
- Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, Lancaster, United Kingdom
| | - Calum Hartley
- Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, Lancaster, United Kingdom
| | - Megan Readman
- Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, Lancaster, United Kingdom
- University of Liverpool, Liverpool, United Kingdom
| | - Sally Linkenauger
- Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, Lancaster, United Kingdom
| | - Trevor Crawford
- Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, Lancaster, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Mazuz Y, Kessler Y, Ganel T. The BTPI: An online battery for measuring susceptibility to visual illusions. J Vis 2023; 23:2. [PMID: 37669069 PMCID: PMC10484025 DOI: 10.1167/jov.23.10.2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/16/2023] [Accepted: 07/31/2023] [Indexed: 09/06/2023] Open
Abstract
Visual illusions provide a powerful tool for probing the mechanisms that underlie perception. While most previous studies of visual illusions focused on average group-level performance, less attention has been devoted to individual differences in susceptibility to illusions. Unlike in other perceptual domains, in which there are established, validated tools to measure individual differences, such tools are not yet available in the domain of visual illusions. Here, we describe the development and validation of the BTPI (Ben-Gurion University Test for Perceptual Illusions), a new online battery designed to measure susceptibility to the influence of three prominent size illusions: the Ebbinghaus, the Ponzo, and the height-width illusions. The BTPI also measures perceptual resolution, reflected by the just noticeable difference (JND), to detect size differences in the context of each illusion. In Experiment 1 (N = 143), we examined performance in typical self-paced tasks, whereas in Experiment 2 (N = 69), we employed a fixed presentation duration paradigm. High test-retest reliability scores were found for all illusions, with little evidence for intercorrelations between different illusions. In addition, lower perceptual resolution (larger JND) was associated with a larger susceptibility to the illusory effect. The computerized task battery and analysis codes are freely available online.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yarden Mazuz
- Department of Psychology and School of Brain Sciences and Cognition, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beer-Sheva, Israel
| | - Yoav Kessler
- Department of Psychology and School of Brain Sciences and Cognition, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beer-Sheva, Israel
| | - Tzvi Ganel
- Department of Psychology and School of Brain Sciences and Cognition, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beer-Sheva, Israel
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Makowski D, Te AS, Kirk S, Liang NZ, Chen SHA. A novel visual illusion paradigm provides evidence for a general factor of illusion sensitivity and personality correlates. Sci Rep 2023; 13:6594. [PMID: 37087480 PMCID: PMC10122668 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-023-33148-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/26/2022] [Accepted: 04/07/2023] [Indexed: 04/24/2023] Open
Abstract
Visual illusions are a gateway to understand how we construct our experience of reality. Unfortunately, important questions remain open, such as the hypothesis of a common factor underlying the sensitivity to different types of illusions, as well as of personality correlates of illusion sensitivity. In this study, we used a novel parametric framework for visual illusions to generate 10 different classic illusions (Delboeuf, Ebbinghaus, Rod and Frame, Vertical-Horizontal, Zöllner, White, Müller-Lyer, Ponzo, Poggendorff, Contrast) varying in strength, embedded in a perceptual discrimination task. We tested the objective effect of the illusions on errors and response times, and extracted participant-level performance scores (n=250) for each illusion. Our results provide evidence in favour of a general factor underlying the sensitivity to different illusions (labelled Factor i). Moreover, we report a positive link between illusion sensitivity and personality traits such as Agreeableness, Honesty-Humility, and negative relationships with Psychoticism, Antagonism, Disinhibition, and Negative Affect.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dominique Makowski
- School of Social Sciences, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore, Singapore.
- School of Psychology, University of Sussex, Brighton, UK.
| | - An Shu Te
- School of Social Sciences, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore, Singapore
| | - Stephanie Kirk
- School of Social Sciences, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore, Singapore
| | - Ngoi Zi Liang
- School of Social Sciences, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore, Singapore
| | - S H Annabel Chen
- School of Social Sciences, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore, Singapore
- LKC Medicine, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore, Singapore
- National Institute of Education, Singapore, Singapore
- Centre for Research and Development in Learning, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore, Singapore
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Langridge RW, Marotta JJ. Use of remote data collection methodology to test for an illusory effect on visually guided cursor movements. Front Psychol 2022; 13:922381. [PMID: 36118434 PMCID: PMC9478591 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.922381] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/17/2022] [Accepted: 08/01/2022] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Investigating the influence of perception on the control of visually guided action typically involves controlled experimentation within the laboratory setting. When appropriate, however, behavioral research of this nature may benefit from the use of methods that allow for remote data collection outside of the lab. This study tested the feasibility of using remote data collection methods to explore the influence of perceived target size on visually guided cursor movements using the Ebbinghaus illusion. Participants completed the experiment remotely, using the trackpad of their personal laptop computers. The task required participants to click on a single circular target presented at either the left or right side of their screen as quickly and accurately as possible (Experiment 1), or to emphasize speed (Experiment 2) or accuracy (Experiment 3). On each trial the target was either surrounded by small or large context circles, or no context circles. Participants’ judgments of the targets’ perceived size were influenced by the illusion, however, the illusion failed to produce differences in click-point accuracy or movement time. Interestingly, the illusion appeared to affect participants’ movement of the cursor toward the target; more directional changes were made when clicking the Perceived Large version of the illusion compared to the Perceived Small version. These results suggest the planning of the cursor movement may have been influenced by the illusion, while later stages of the movement were not, and cursor movements directed toward targets perceived as smaller required less correction compared to targets perceived as larger.
Collapse
|
10
|
Makowski D, Lau ZJ, Pham T, Paul Boyce W, Annabel Chen SH. A Parametric Framework to Generate Visual Illusions Using Python. Perception 2021; 50:950-965. [PMID: 34841973 DOI: 10.1177/03010066211057347] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Visual illusions are fascinating phenomena that have been used and studied by artists and scientists for centuries, leading to important discoveries about the neurocognitive underpinnings of perception, consciousness, and neuropsychiatric disorders such as schizophrenia or autism. Surprisingly, despite their historical and theoretical importance as psychological stimuli, there is no dedicated software, nor consistent approach, to generate illusions in a systematic fashion. Instead, scientists have to craft them by hand in an idiosyncratic fashion, or use pre-made images not tailored for the specific needs of their studies. This, in turn, hinders the reproducibility of illusion-based research, narrowing possibilities for scientific breakthroughs and their applications. With the aim of addressing this gap, Pyllusion is a Python-based open-source software (freely available at https://github.com/RealityBending/Pyllusion), that offers a framework to manipulate and generate illusions in a systematic way, compatible with different output formats such as image files (.png, .jpg, .tiff, etc.) or experimental software (such as PsychoPy).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dominique Makowski
- School of Social Sciences, 54761Nanyang Technological University, Singapore.,School of Social Sciences, 54761Nanyang Technological University, Singapore.,Centre for Research and Development in Learning, 54761Nanyang Technological University, Singapore.,Lee Kong Chian School of Medicine, 54761Nanyang Technological University, Singapore.,National Institute of Education, 54761Nanyang Technological University, Singapore
| | - Zen J Lau
- School of Social Sciences, 54761Nanyang Technological University, Singapore.,School of Social Sciences, 54761Nanyang Technological University, Singapore.,Centre for Research and Development in Learning, 54761Nanyang Technological University, Singapore.,Lee Kong Chian School of Medicine, 54761Nanyang Technological University, Singapore.,National Institute of Education, 54761Nanyang Technological University, Singapore
| | - Tam Pham
- School of Social Sciences, 54761Nanyang Technological University, Singapore.,School of Social Sciences, 54761Nanyang Technological University, Singapore.,Centre for Research and Development in Learning, 54761Nanyang Technological University, Singapore.,Lee Kong Chian School of Medicine, 54761Nanyang Technological University, Singapore.,National Institute of Education, 54761Nanyang Technological University, Singapore
| | - W Paul Boyce
- School of Psychology, 7800University of New South Wales, Australia.,School of Social Sciences, 54761Nanyang Technological University, Singapore.,Centre for Research and Development in Learning, 54761Nanyang Technological University, Singapore.,Lee Kong Chian School of Medicine, 54761Nanyang Technological University, Singapore.,National Institute of Education, 54761Nanyang Technological University, Singapore
| | - S H Annabel Chen
- School of Social Sciences, 54761Nanyang Technological University, Singapore.,Centre for Research and Development in Learning, 54761Nanyang Technological University, Singapore.,Lee Kong Chian School of Medicine, 54761Nanyang Technological University, Singapore.,National Institute of Education, 54761Nanyang Technological University, Singapore
| |
Collapse
|