1
|
Self JL, Togunov RR, Edwards MA, Derocher AE. Influence of wind on movement behaviour in Arctic grizzly bears. Behav Processes 2025; 227:105177. [PMID: 40101809 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2025.105177] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/19/2024] [Revised: 03/04/2025] [Accepted: 03/09/2025] [Indexed: 03/20/2025]
Abstract
Odours are emitted from organic matter and can contain important information about an animal's surroundings, including the presence and location of other organisms. Wind acts as a conduit of olfactory information, affecting the spread and direction of odour dispersal across terrestrial landscapes. To increase the likelihood of detecting an odour molecule, individuals may exhibit anemotaxis - orientation bias to wind during movement - where the theoretical optimal olfactory search strategy is to move crosswind. We tested for biased movement relative to wind in Arctic grizzly bears (Ursus arctos) during the spring hypophagic period in the Mackenzie Delta, Northwest Territories, Canada using modelled winds and satellite-linked telemetry data (n = 12,430 locations) from 40 Arctic grizzly bears monitored between 2003 and 2010. Our results show that orientation relative to wind varied with movement rate, a proxy for active search effort. During steps where bears had high movement rates (> 90th percentile), bears predominantly oriented crosswind. We also found a positive relationship between movement rate and crosswind orientation: as bears moved faster, they increased their crosswind component of orientation. These results suggest an adaptive pattern of movement in response to wind, where bears oriented relative to the wind in a way that increased the likelihood of odour detection during active search. We suggest that future studies could include wind data in habitat selection and foraging models to examine its influence on habitat selection and use.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Julia L Self
- Department of Psychology, University of Alberta, Edmonton, Canada.
| | - Ron R Togunov
- Department of Mathematical Sciences, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, Trondheim, Norway; Gjærevoll Centre for Biodiversity Foresight Analyses, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, Trondheim, Norway
| | - Mark A Edwards
- Department of Renewable Resources, University of Alberta, Edmonton, Canada
| | - Andrew E Derocher
- Department of Biological Sciences, University of Alberta, Edmonton, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Kim D, Thompson PR, Wolfson DW, Merkle JA, Oliveira-Santos LGR, Forester JD, Avgar T, Lewis MA, Fieberg J. Identifying signals of memory from observations of animal movements. MOVEMENT ECOLOGY 2024; 12:72. [PMID: 39558435 PMCID: PMC11575436 DOI: 10.1186/s40462-024-00510-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/23/2024] [Accepted: 10/03/2024] [Indexed: 11/20/2024]
Abstract
Incorporating memory (i.e., some notion of familiarity or experience with the landscape) into models of animal movement is a rising challenge in the field of movement ecology. The recent proliferation of new methods offers new opportunities to understand how memory influences movement. However, there are no clear guidelines for practitioners wishing to parameterize the effects of memory on moving animals. We review approaches for incorporating memory into step-selection analyses (SSAs), a frequently used movement modeling framework. Memory-informed SSAs can be constructed by including spatial-temporal covariates (or maps) that define some aspect of familiarity (e.g., whether, how often, or how long ago the animal visited different spatial locations) derived from long-term telemetry data. We demonstrate how various familiarity covariates can be included in SSAs using a series of coded examples in which we fit models to wildlife tracking data from a wide range of taxa. We discuss how these different approaches can be used to address questions related to whether and how animals use information from past experiences to inform their future movements. We also highlight challenges and decisions that the user must make when applying these methods to their tracking data. By reviewing different approaches and providing code templates for their implementation, we hope to inspire practitioners to investigate further the importance of memory in animal movements using wildlife tracking data.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dongmin Kim
- Department of Ecology, Evolution and Behavior, University of Minnesota, St. Paul, MN, USA.
- Department of Organismic and Evolutionary Biology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA.
| | - Peter R Thompson
- School of Environmental Science, Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, BC, Canada
| | - David W Wolfson
- Department of Fisheries, Wildlife and Conservation Biology, University of Minnesota, St. Paul, MN, USA
| | - Jerod A Merkle
- Department of Zoology and Physiology, University of Wyoming, Laramie, WY, USA
| | - L G R Oliveira-Santos
- Departmento de Ecologia, Universidade Federal do Mato Grosso do Sul, Campo Grande, Brazil
| | - James D Forester
- Department of Fisheries, Wildlife and Conservation Biology, University of Minnesota, St. Paul, MN, USA
| | - Tal Avgar
- Department of Biology, University of British Columbia-Okanagan and Wildlife Science Centre, Biodiversity Pathways Ltd, Kelowna, BC, Canada
| | - Mark A Lewis
- Department of Mathematics and Statistics and Department of Biology, University of Victoria, Victoria, BC, Canada
| | - John Fieberg
- Department of Fisheries, Wildlife and Conservation Biology, University of Minnesota, St. Paul, MN, USA
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Thompson PR, Harrington PD, Mallory CD, Lele SR, Bayne EM, Derocher AE, Edwards MA, Campbell M, Lewis MA. Simultaneous estimation of the temporal and spatial extent of animal migration using step lengths and turning angles. MOVEMENT ECOLOGY 2024; 12:1. [PMID: 38191509 PMCID: PMC10775566 DOI: 10.1186/s40462-023-00444-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/11/2023] [Accepted: 12/19/2023] [Indexed: 01/10/2024]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Animals of many different species, trophic levels, and life history strategies migrate, and the improvement of animal tracking technology allows ecologists to collect increasing amounts of detailed data on these movements. Understanding when animals migrate is important for managing their populations, but is still difficult despite modelling advancements. METHODS We designed a model that parametrically estimates the timing of migration from animal tracking data. Our model identifies the beginning and end of migratory movements as signaled by change-points in step length and turning angle distributions. To this end, we can also use the model to estimate how an animal's movement changes when it begins migrating. In addition to a thorough simulation analysis, we tested our model on three datasets: migratory ferruginous hawks (Buteo regalis) in the Great Plains, barren-ground caribou (Rangifer tarandus groenlandicus) in northern Canada, and non-migratory brown bears (Ursus arctos) from the Canadian Arctic. RESULTS Our simulation analysis suggests that our model is most useful for datasets where an increase in movement speed or directional autocorrelation is clearly detectable. We estimated the beginning and end of migration in caribou and hawks to the nearest day, while confirming a lack of migratory behaviour in the brown bears. In addition to estimating when caribou and ferruginous hawks migrated, our model also identified differences in how they migrated; ferruginous hawks achieved efficient migrations by drastically increasing their movement rates while caribou migration was achieved through significant increases in directional persistence. CONCLUSIONS Our approach is applicable to many animal movement studies and includes parameters that can facilitate comparison between different species or datasets. We hope that rigorous assessment of migration metrics will aid understanding of both how and why animals move.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Peter R Thompson
- Department of Biological Sciences, University of Alberta, Edmonton, AB, Canada.
| | - Peter D Harrington
- Department of Mathematics, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
- Department of Mathematical and Statistical Sciences, University of Alberta, Edmonton, AB, Canada
| | | | - Subhash R Lele
- Department of Mathematical and Statistical Sciences, University of Alberta, Edmonton, AB, Canada
| | - Erin M Bayne
- Department of Biological Sciences, University of Alberta, Edmonton, AB, Canada
| | - Andrew E Derocher
- Department of Biological Sciences, University of Alberta, Edmonton, AB, Canada
| | - Mark A Edwards
- Office of the Chief Scientist, Environment and Protected Areas, Government of Alberta, Edmonton, AB, Canada
- Department of Renewable Resources, University of Alberta, Edmonton, AB, Canada
| | | | - Mark A Lewis
- Department of Biological Sciences, University of Alberta, Edmonton, AB, Canada
- Department of Mathematical and Statistical Sciences, University of Alberta, Edmonton, AB, Canada
- Department of Biology, University of Victoria, Victoria, BC, Canada
- Department of Mathematics and Statistics, University of Victoria, Victoria, BC, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Homstol L, Raymond S, Edwards C, Hamilton AN, St. Clair CC. Aversive conditioning increases short-term wariness but does not change habitat use in black bears associated with conflict. PLoS One 2024; 19:e0295989. [PMID: 38166059 PMCID: PMC10760891 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0295989] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/08/2023] [Accepted: 12/03/2023] [Indexed: 01/04/2024] Open
Abstract
Conflict between humans and black bears (Ursus americanus) occurs throughout North America with increasing public demand to replace lethal management with non-lethal methods, such as aversive conditioning (AC). AC aims to teach animals to associate negative stimuli with humans or their infrastructure. We sought to test the efficacy of AC using radio-collared black bears in Whistler, British Columbia, by monitoring individuals and assigning those in conflict with people to control or treatment groups. We measured wariness using overt reaction distance, displacement distance, and reaction to researchers before, during and after executing 3-5-day AC programs that consisted of launching projectiles at bears in the treatment group. We also assessed predictors of successful AC events (i.e., leaving at a run), changes in bear use of human-dominated habitat during the day and at night, and the effects of including a sound stimulus to signal the beginning and end of AC events. Among treated bears, overt reaction distance increased by 46.5% and displacement distance increased by 69.0% following AC programs, whereas both overt reaction distance and displacement distance decreased over time among control group bears. Each additional AC event during the previous 30 days increased likelihood of bear departure in response to researcher presence by 4.5%. The success of AC events varied among individuals, declined with distance to cover, and increased with exposure to previous AC events. Projectiles launched from guns were slightly more effective at causing bears to displace compared to those launched from slingshots, and sound stimuli decreased the likelihood of a successful AC event. AC did not alter diurnal use by bears of human-dominated habitat. Our results suggest that AC effectively increases short-term wariness in black bears but does not alter bear use of human-dominated spaces, highlighting the importance of proactive attractant management and prevention of food conditioning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lori Homstol
- Department of Biological Sciences, University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta, Canada
- Provincial Government of British Columbia, Victoria, British Columbia, Canada
| | - Sage Raymond
- Department of Biological Sciences, University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta, Canada
| | - Claire Edwards
- Department of Biological Sciences, University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta, Canada
- Jasper National Park, Jasper, Alberta, Canada
| | - Anthony N. Hamilton
- Provincial Government of British Columbia, Victoria, British Columbia, Canada
| | | |
Collapse
|
5
|
Fagan WF, McBride F, Koralov L. Reinforced diffusions as models of memory-mediated animal movement. J R Soc Interface 2023; 20:20220700. [PMID: 36987616 PMCID: PMC10050924 DOI: 10.1098/rsif.2022.0700] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/23/2022] [Accepted: 03/07/2023] [Indexed: 03/30/2023] Open
Abstract
How memory shapes animals' movement paths is a topic of growing interest in ecology, with connections to planning for conservation and climate change. Empirical studies suggest that memory has both temporal and spatial components, and can include both attractive and aversive elements. Here, we introduce reinforced diffusions (the continuous time counterpart of reinforced random walks) as a modelling framework for understanding the role that memory plays in determining animal movements. This framework includes reinforcement via functions of time before present and of distance away from a current location. Focusing on the interplay between memory and central place attraction (a component of home ranging behaviour), we explore patterns of space usage that result from the reinforced diffusion. Our efforts identify three qualitatively different behaviours: bounded wandering behaviour that does not collapse spatially, collapse to a very small area, and, most intriguingly, convergence to a cycle. Subsequent applications show how reinforced diffusion can create movement trajectories emulating the learning of movement routes by homing pigeons and consolidation of ant travel paths. The mathematically explicit manner with which assumptions about the structure of memory can be stated and subsequently explored provides linkages to biological concepts like an animal's 'immediate surroundings' and memory decay.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- William F. Fagan
- Department of Biology, University of Maryland, College Park, MD 20742, USA
| | - Frank McBride
- Graduate Program in Applied Mathematics and Scientific Computing, University of Maryland, College Park, MD 20742, USA
| | - Leonid Koralov
- Department of Mathematics, University of Maryland, College Park, MD 20742, USA
| |
Collapse
|