1
|
Dong WK. Modulation of multisensory nociceptive neurons in monkey cortical area 7b and behavioral correlates. J Neurophysiol 2024; 132:544-569. [PMID: 38985936 PMCID: PMC11427044 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00377.2023] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/13/2023] [Revised: 07/08/2024] [Accepted: 07/08/2024] [Indexed: 07/12/2024] Open
Abstract
Wide-range thermoreceptive neurons (WRT-EN) in monkey cortical area 7b that encoded innocuous and nocuous cutaneous thermal and threatening visuosensory stimulation with high fidelity were studied to identify their multisensory integrative response properties. Emphasis was given to characterizing the spatial and temporal effects of threatening visuosensory input on the thermal stimulus-response properties of these multisensory nociceptive neurons. Threatening visuosensory stimulation was most efficacious in modulating thermal evoked responses when presented as a downward ("looming"), spatially congruent, approaching and closely proximal target in relation to the somatosensory receptive field. Both temporal alignment and misalignment of spatially aligned threatening visual and thermal stimulation significantly increased mean discharge frequencies above those evoked by thermal stimulation alone, particularly at near noxious (43°C) and mildly noxious (45°C) temperatures. The enhanced multisensory discharge frequencies were equivalent to the discharge frequency evoked by overtly noxious thermal stimulation alone at 47°C (monkey pain tolerance threshold). A significant increase in behavioral mean escape frequency with shorter escape latency was evoked by multisensory stimulation at near noxious temperature (43°C), which was equivalent to that evoked by noxious stimulation alone (47°C). The remarkable concordance of elevating both neural discharge and escape frequency from a nonnociceptive and prepain level by near noxious thermal stimulation to a nociceptive and pain level by multisensory visual and near noxious thermal stimulation and integration is an elegantly designed defensive neural mechanism that in effect lowers both nociceptive response and pain thresholds to preemptively engage nocifensive behavior and, consequently, avert impending and actual injurious noxious thermal stimulation.NEW & NOTEWORTHY Multisensory nociceptive neurons in cortical area 7b are engaged in integration of threatening visuosensory and a wide range of innocuous and nocuous somatosensory (thermoreceptive) inputs. The enhancement of neuronal activity and escape behavior in monkey by multisensory integration is consistent and supportive of human psychophysical studies. The spatial features of visuosensory stimulation in peripersonal space in relation to somatic stimulation in personal space are critical to multisensory integration, nociception, nocifensive behavior, and pain.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Willie K Dong
- Department of Anesthesiology and Pain Medicine, School of Medicine, University of Washington, Seattle, Washington, United States
- Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, University of Illinois Urbana-Champaign, Urbana, Illinois, United States
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Lemmens JS. Persistence and pleasure in VR: Enhancing Exercise Endurance and Enjoyment through Virtual Environments. PSYCHOLOGY OF SPORT AND EXERCISE 2023; 69:102494. [PMID: 37665929 DOI: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2023.102494] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/05/2022] [Revised: 06/25/2023] [Accepted: 07/13/2023] [Indexed: 09/06/2023]
Abstract
Virtual Reality (VR) provides an enjoyable addition to stationary physical exercise and can improve performance while exercising. The aim of this study is to explore the effectiveness of three interactive virtual environments (i.e., social, relaxing, stressful) on enjoyment and persistence during strength-based exercises. In a within-subjects experiment, 97 healthy young adults completed four consecutive sets of two strength-based exercises. Participants completed one set as baseline and then each participant completed three more sets in three different interactive environments, experienced through a VR headset. Results showed that both the stressful virtual environment, where participants were hanging suspended over a city, and the social virtual environment where participants were audibly encouraged in a stadium, increased participants' persistence in both exercises, when compared to the relaxing virtual environment. Specifically, the relaxing sunny beach environment caused poorer performances in a dead hang exercise among men (n = 30), and poorer performances in a core exercise among women (n = 66). Somewhat paradoxically, this relaxing virtual beach environment was considered the most enjoyable environment among both male and female participants. The potential of VR in exercise lies in its ability to provide pleasurable and performance-enhancing immersive environments that may be too expensive or dangerous in reality.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jeroen S Lemmens
- Amsterdam School of Communication Research, University of Amsterdam, the Netherlands.
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Legrain V, Filbrich L, Vanderclausen C. Letter on the pain of blind people for the use of those who can see their pain. Pain 2023; 164:1451-1456. [PMID: 36728808 DOI: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000002862] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/28/2022] [Accepted: 12/12/2022] [Indexed: 02/03/2023]
Affiliation(s)
- Valéry Legrain
- Institute of Neuroscience, Université catholique de Louvain, Brussels, Belgium
- Psychological Sciences Research Institute, Université catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium
- Louvain Bionics, Université catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium
| | - Lieve Filbrich
- Institute of Neuroscience, Université catholique de Louvain, Brussels, Belgium
- Psychological Sciences Research Institute, Université catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium
| | - Camille Vanderclausen
- Institute of Neuroscience, Université catholique de Louvain, Brussels, Belgium
- Neuropsychological Rehabilitation Unit, Saint-Luc University Hospital, Brussels, Belgium
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Manfron L, Filbrich L, Molitor V, Farnè A, Mouraux A, Legrain V. Perceptual simultaneity between nociceptive and visual stimuli depends on their spatial congruence. Exp Brain Res 2023:10.1007/s00221-023-06637-2. [PMID: 37222776 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-023-06637-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/08/2022] [Accepted: 05/12/2023] [Indexed: 05/25/2023]
Abstract
To protect our body against physical threats, it is important to integrate the somatic and extra-somatic inputs generated by these stimuli. Temporal synchrony is an important parameter determining multisensory interaction, and the time taken by a given sensory input to reach the brain depends on the length and conduction velocity of the specific pathways through which it is transmitted. Nociceptive inputs are transmitted through very slow conducting unmyelinated C and thinly myelinated Aδ nociceptive fibers. It was previously shown that to perceive a visual stimulus and a thermo-nociceptive stimulus applied on the hand as coinciding in time, the nociceptive stimulus must precede the visual one by 76 ms for nociceptive inputs conveyed by Aδ fibers and 577 ms for inputs conveyed by C fibers. Since spatial proximity is also hypothesized to contribute to multisensory interaction, the present study investigated the effect of spatial congruence between visual and nociceptive stimuli. Participants judged the temporal order of visual and nociceptive stimuli, with the visual stimuli flashed either next to the stimulated hand or next to the opposite unstimulated hand, and with nociceptive stimuli evoking responses mediated by either Aδ or C fibers. The amount of time by which the nociceptive stimulus had to precede the visual stimulus for them to be perceived as appearing concomitantly was smaller when the visual stimulus occurred near the hand receiving the nociceptive stimulus as compared to when it occurred near the contralateral hand. This illustrates the challenge for the brain to process the synchrony between nociceptive and non-nociceptive stimuli to enable their efficient interaction to optimize defensive reaction against physical dangers.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Louise Manfron
- Institute of Neuroscience, Université catholique de Louvain, Avenue Mounier 53, boite COSY B1.53.04, 1200, Brussels, Belgium
- Psychological Sciences Research Institute, Université catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium
| | - Lieve Filbrich
- Institute of Neuroscience, Université catholique de Louvain, Avenue Mounier 53, boite COSY B1.53.04, 1200, Brussels, Belgium
- Psychological Sciences Research Institute, Université catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium
| | - Victoria Molitor
- Institute of Neuroscience, Université catholique de Louvain, Avenue Mounier 53, boite COSY B1.53.04, 1200, Brussels, Belgium
| | - Alessandro Farnè
- INSERM U1028, CNRS UMR5292, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, ImpAct Team, Lyon, France
| | - André Mouraux
- Institute of Neuroscience, Université catholique de Louvain, Avenue Mounier 53, boite COSY B1.53.04, 1200, Brussels, Belgium
- Psychological Sciences Research Institute, Université catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium
- Louvain Bionics, Université catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium
| | - Valéry Legrain
- Institute of Neuroscience, Université catholique de Louvain, Avenue Mounier 53, boite COSY B1.53.04, 1200, Brussels, Belgium.
- Psychological Sciences Research Institute, Université catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium.
- Louvain Bionics, Université catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium.
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Zanini A, Salemme R, Farnè A, Brozzoli C. Associative learning in peripersonal space: fear responses are acquired in hand-centered coordinates. J Neurophysiol 2021; 126:864-874. [PMID: 34379522 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00157.2021] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Space coding affects perception of stimuli associated to negative valence: threatening stimuli presented within the peripersonal space (PPS) speed up behavioral responses compared with nonthreatening events. However, it remains unclear whether the association between stimuli and their negative valence is acquired in a body part-centered reference system, a main feature of the PPS coding. Here we test the hypothesis that associative learning takes place in hand-centered coordinates and can therefore remap according to hand displacement. In two experiments, we used a Pavlovian fear-learning paradigm to associate a visual stimulus [light circle, the conditioned stimulus (CS)] with an aversive stimulus (electrocutaneous shock) applied on the right hand only when the CS was displayed close (CS+) but when not far from it (CS-). Measuring the skin conductance response (SCR), we observed successful fear conditioning, with increased anticipatory fear responses associated with CS+. Crucially, experiment I showed a remapping of these responses following hand displacement, with a generalization to both types of CS. Experiment II corroborated and further extended our findings by ruling out the novelty of the experimental context as a driving factor of such modulations. Indeed, fear responses were present only for stimuli within the PPS but not for new stimuli displayed outside the PPS. By revealing a hand-centered (re)mapping of the conditioning effect, these findings indicate that associative learning can arise in hand-centered coordinates. They further suggest that the threatening valence of an object also depends on its basic spatial relationship with our body.NEW & NOTEWORTHY Associative fear learning takes place in hand-centered coordinates. Using a Pavlovian fear-learning paradigm, we show that the anticipatory skin conductance response indicating the association between the negative value and an initially neutral stimulus is acquired and then remapped in space when the stimulated body part moves to a different position. These results demonstrate the relationship between the representation of peripersonal space and the encoding of threatening stimuli. Hypotheses concerning the underlying neural network are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- A Zanini
- Impact-Integrative Multisensory Perception Action and Cognition Team, Lyon Neuroscience Research Centre, INSERM U1028, CNRS UMR5292, Lyon, France.,University Claude Bernard Lyon I, Lyon, France
| | - R Salemme
- Impact-Integrative Multisensory Perception Action and Cognition Team, Lyon Neuroscience Research Centre, INSERM U1028, CNRS UMR5292, Lyon, France.,University Claude Bernard Lyon I, Lyon, France.,Hospices Civils de Lyon, Neuro-immersion-Mouvement et Handicap, Lyon, France
| | - A Farnè
- Impact-Integrative Multisensory Perception Action and Cognition Team, Lyon Neuroscience Research Centre, INSERM U1028, CNRS UMR5292, Lyon, France.,University Claude Bernard Lyon I, Lyon, France.,Hospices Civils de Lyon, Neuro-immersion-Mouvement et Handicap, Lyon, France.,Center for Mind/Brain Sciences, University of Trento, Rovereto, Italy
| | - C Brozzoli
- Impact-Integrative Multisensory Perception Action and Cognition Team, Lyon Neuroscience Research Centre, INSERM U1028, CNRS UMR5292, Lyon, France.,University Claude Bernard Lyon I, Lyon, France.,Hospices Civils de Lyon, Neuro-immersion-Mouvement et Handicap, Lyon, France.,Department of Neurobiology, Care Sciences and Society, Aging Research Center, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Measuring the sensitivity of tactile temporal order judgments in sighted and blind participants using the adaptive psi method. Atten Percept Psychophys 2021; 83:2995-3007. [PMID: 34036536 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-021-02301-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 03/14/2021] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Spatial locations of somatosensory stimuli are coded according to somatotopic (anatomical distribution of the sensory receptors on the skin surface) and spatiotopic (position of the body parts in external space) reference frames. This was mostly evidenced by means of temporal order judgment (TOJ) tasks in which participants discriminate the temporal order of two tactile stimuli, one applied on each hand. Because crossing the hands generates a conflict between anatomical and spatial responses, TOJ performance is decreased in such posture, except for congenitally blind people, suggesting a role of visual experience in somatosensory perception. In previous TOJ studies, stimuli were generally presented using the method of constant stimuli-that is, the repetition of a predefined sample of stimulus-onset asynchronies (SOA) separating the two stimuli. This method has the disadvantage that a large number of trials is needed to obtain reliable data when aiming at dissociating performances of groups characterized by different cognitive abilities. Indeed, each SOA among a large variety of different SOAs should be presented the same number of times irrespective of the participant's performance. This study aimed to replicate previous tactile TOJ data in sighted and blind participants with the adaptive psi method in order to validate a novel method that adapts the presented SOA according to the participant's performance. This allows to precisely estimate the temporal sensitivity of each participant while the presented stimuli are adapted to the participant's individual discrimination threshold. We successfully replicated previous findings in both sighted and blind participants, corroborating previous data using a more suitable psychophysical tool.
Collapse
|
7
|
Manfron L, Vanderclausen C, Legrain V. No Evidence for an Effect of the Distance Between the Hands on Tactile Temporal Order Judgments. Perception 2021; 50:294-307. [PMID: 33653176 DOI: 10.1177/0301006621998877] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Localizing somatosensory stimuli is an important process, as it allows us to spatially guide our actions toward the object entering in contact with the body. Accordingly, the positions of tactile inputs are coded according to both somatotopic and spatiotopic representations, the latter one considering the position of the stimulated limbs in external space. The spatiotopic representation has often been evidenced by means of temporal order judgment (TOJ) tasks. Participants' judgments about the order of appearance of two successive somatosensory stimuli are less accurate when the hands are crossed over the body midline than uncrossed but also when participants' hands are placed close together when compared with farther away. Moreover, these postural effects might depend on the vision of the stimulated limbs. The aim of this study was to test the influence of seeing the hands, on the modulation of tactile TOJ by the spatial distance between the stimulated limbs. The results showed no influence of the distance between the stimulated hands on TOJ performance and prevent us from concluding whether vision of the hands affects TOJ performance, or whether these variables interact. The reliability of such distance effect to investigate the spatial representations of tactile inputs is questioned.
Collapse
|
8
|
Manfron L, Filbrich L, Nijs E, Mouraux A, Legrain V. Investigating perceptual simultaneity between nociceptive and visual stimuli by means of temporal order judgments. Neurosci Lett 2020; 735:135156. [PMID: 32574797 DOI: 10.1016/j.neulet.2020.135156] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/20/2020] [Revised: 06/04/2020] [Accepted: 06/11/2020] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
Abstract
Multisensory interactions between pain and vision allow us to adapt our behavior to optimize detection and reaction against bodily threats. Interactions between different sensory inputs are enhanced when they are perceived closely in space and time. However, thermo-nociceptive and visual stimuli are conveyed to the cortex through specific pathways with their own conduction velocity. The present experiment aims to measure the necessary asynchrony between a nociceptive stimulus and a visual stimulus for both to be perceived as occurring simultaneously. Healthy volunteers performed a temporal order judgment task during which they discriminated the temporal order between a laser-induced nociceptive stimulus applied on one hand dorsum and a visual stimulus presented next to the stimulated hand. Laser stimulus temperature selectively activated Aδ- and/or C- fiber afferents. In order to be perceived as occurring simultaneously with a visual stimulus, a thermo-nociceptive input selectively conveyed by C-fiber afferents must precede the visual stimulus by 577 ms on average, while the stimulus-evoked input conveyed by Aδ-fiber afferents must precede it by 76 ms on average. This experiment focuses on the necessary asynchrony between thermo-nociceptive and visual inputs for them to be perceived simultaneously, to optimize the conditions under which they interact closely. Since C-fibers are unmyelinated, the asynchrony between a C-fiber stimulus and a visual stimulus is much greater than the asynchrony between a nociceptive stimulus additionally activating Aδ-fibers and that same visual stimulus. It is crucial to consider these discrepancies in further studies interested in multisensory interactions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Louise Manfron
- Institute of Neuroscience, Université catholique de Louvain, Brussels, Belgium; Psychological Sciences Research Institute, Université catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium.
| | - Lieve Filbrich
- Institute of Neuroscience, Université catholique de Louvain, Brussels, Belgium; Psychological Sciences Research Institute, Université catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium
| | - Emmanuelle Nijs
- Institute of Neuroscience, Université catholique de Louvain, Brussels, Belgium
| | - André Mouraux
- Institute of Neuroscience, Université catholique de Louvain, Brussels, Belgium; Psychological Sciences Research Institute, Université catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium
| | - Valéry Legrain
- Institute of Neuroscience, Université catholique de Louvain, Brussels, Belgium; Psychological Sciences Research Institute, Université catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Testing the exteroceptive function of nociception: The role of visual experience in shaping the spatial representations of nociceptive inputs. Cortex 2020; 126:26-38. [PMID: 32062141 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.12.024] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/31/2019] [Revised: 05/21/2019] [Accepted: 12/14/2019] [Indexed: 01/30/2023]
Abstract
Adequately localizing pain is crucial to protect the body against physical damage and react to the stimulus in external space having caused such damage. Accordingly, it is hypothesized that nociceptive inputs are remapped from a somatotopic reference frame, representing the skin surface, towards a spatiotopic frame, representing the body parts in external space. This ability is thought to be developed and shaped by early visual experience. To test this hypothesis, normally sighted and early blind participants performed temporal order judgment tasks during which they judged which of two nociceptive stimuli applied on each hand's dorsum was perceived as first delivered. Crucially, tasks were performed with the hands either in an uncrossed posture or crossed over body midline. While early blinds were not affected by the posture, performances of the normally sighted participants decreased in the crossed condition relative to the uncrossed condition. This indicates that nociceptive stimuli were automatically remapped into a spatiotopic representation that interfered with somatotopy in normally sighted individuals, whereas early blinds seemed to mostly rely on a somatotopic representation to localize nociceptive inputs. Accordingly, the plasticity of the nociceptive system would not purely depend on bodily experiences but also on crossmodal interactions between nociception and vision during early sensory experience.
Collapse
|
10
|
Spaccasassi C, Maravita A. Peripersonal space is diversely sensitive to a temporary vs permanent state of anxiety. Cognition 2019; 195:104133. [PMID: 31734548 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2019.104133] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/19/2019] [Revised: 11/07/2019] [Accepted: 11/08/2019] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
Abstract
Peripersonal Space (PPS) is the multisensory space immediately surrounding our body. Visual and tactile stimuli here are promptly processed, since their interaction gradually strengthens as the distance between visual stimulus and the body decreases. Recently, a modified version of the Temporal Order Judgment (TOJ) task was proposed to assess PPS based on the spatial congruence between somatosensory and visual stimuli. Here, we used this paradigm to explore how a temporary vs a permanent state of anxiety can alter PPS. Indeed, previous research showed that PPS boundaries are not fixed, but they can be enlarged by contingent factors (i.e. emotional features). Participants performed the TOJ paradigm twice, just before and after completing an anxiety-inducing task (experimental breathing condition) or a neutral one (control breathing condition), while their trait and state anxiety levels were repeatedly measured. We found that the pattern of visuo-tactile integration in PPS changes in the very opposite way following the two breathing tasks for participants with high levels of temporary anxiety, by strengthening and weakening its power after the experimental and control conditions, respectively. On the contrary, both the breathing tasks are capable of reducing the cross-modal interplay as compared to baseline for high trait-anxious participants, who show an overall stronger visuo-tactile integration inside the PPS than low trait anxious individuals. These results are discussed in the light of the double dissociation between orienting and alerting attentional network over-functioning, reported in state anxiety participants, and impoverished prefrontal attentional control shown by trait anxiety individuals.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- C Spaccasassi
- University of Milano-Bicocca, Department of Psychology, piazza Ateneo Nuovo 1, 20126 Milan, Italy.
| | - A Maravita
- University of Milano-Bicocca, Department of Psychology, piazza Ateneo Nuovo 1, 20126 Milan, Italy; NeuroMi, Milan Centre for Neuroscience, Milan, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Filbrich L, Blandiaux S, Manfron L, Farnè A, De Keyser R, Legrain V. Unimodal and crossmodal extinction of nociceptive stimuli in healthy volunteers. Behav Brain Res 2019; 362:114-121. [PMID: 30630019 DOI: 10.1016/j.bbr.2019.01.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/26/2018] [Revised: 12/28/2018] [Accepted: 01/06/2019] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
Nociception, the physiological mechanisms specifically processing information about noxious and potentially painful stimuli, has the double function to warn about potential body damages (interoception) and about the cause of such potential damages (exteroception). The exteroceptive function is thought to rely on multisensory integration between somatic and extra-somatic stimuli, provided that extra-somatic stimuli occur near the stimulated body area. To corroborate this hypothesis, we succeeded to show in healthy volunteers that the perception of nociceptive stimuli applied on one hand can be extinguished, as compared to single presentation, by the simultaneous application of nociceptive stimuli on the opposite hand, as well as by the presentation of visual stimuli near the opposite hand. On the contrary, visual stimuli presented near the same stimulated hand facilitated the perception of nociceptive stimuli. This nociceptive extinction phenomenon indicates that the perception of noxious events does not merely rely on the specific activation of the nociceptive system, but also depends on other sensory experiences about the body and the space around it.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lieve Filbrich
- Institute of Neuroscience, Université catholique de Louvain, Brussels, Belgium; Psychological Sciences Research Institute, Université catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium
| | - Séverine Blandiaux
- Institute of Neuroscience, Université catholique de Louvain, Brussels, Belgium
| | - Louise Manfron
- Institute of Neuroscience, Université catholique de Louvain, Brussels, Belgium; Psychological Sciences Research Institute, Université catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium
| | - Alessandro Farnè
- ImpAct team, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, INSERM U1028, CNRS UMR5292, Lyon, France
| | - Roxane De Keyser
- Institute of Neuroscience, Université catholique de Louvain, Brussels, Belgium
| | - Valéry Legrain
- Institute of Neuroscience, Université catholique de Louvain, Brussels, Belgium; Psychological Sciences Research Institute, Université catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium.
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Shen W, Tu Y, Gollub RL, Ortiz A, Napadow V, Yu S, Wilson G, Park J, Lang C, Jung M, Gerber J, Mawla I, Chan ST, Wasan AD, Edwards RR, Kaptchuk T, Li S, Rosen B, Kong J. Visual network alterations in brain functional connectivity in chronic low back pain: A resting state functional connectivity and machine learning study. NEUROIMAGE-CLINICAL 2019; 22:101775. [PMID: 30927604 PMCID: PMC6444301 DOI: 10.1016/j.nicl.2019.101775] [Citation(s) in RCA: 66] [Impact Index Per Article: 11.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/23/2018] [Revised: 01/22/2019] [Accepted: 03/10/2019] [Indexed: 01/04/2023]
Abstract
Chronic low back pain (cLBP) is associated with widespread functional and structural changes in the brain. This study aims to investigate the resting state functional connectivity (rsFC) changes of visual networks in cLBP patients and the feasibility of distinguishing cLBP patients from healthy controls using machine learning methods. cLBP (n = 90) and control individuals (n = 74) were enrolled and underwent resting-state BOLD fMRI scans. Primary, dorsal, and ventral visual networks derived from independent component analysis were used as regions of interest to compare resting state functional connectivity changes between the cLBP patients and healthy controls. We then applied a support vector machine classifier to distinguish the cLBP patients and control individuals. These results were further verified in a new cohort of subjects. We found that the functional connectivity between the primary visual network and the somatosensory/motor areas were significantly enhanced in cLBP patients. The rsFC between the primary visual network and S1 was negatively associated with duration of cLBP. In addition, we found that the rsFC of the visual network could achieve a classification accuracy of 79.3% in distinguishing cLBP patients from HCs, and these results were further validated in an independent cohort of subjects (accuracy = 66.7%). Our results demonstrate significant changes in the rsFC of the visual networks in cLBP patients. We speculate these alterations may represent an adaptation/self-adjustment mechanism and cross-model interaction between the visual, somatosensory, motor, attention, and salient networks in response to cLBP. Elucidating the role of the visual networks in cLBP may shed light on the pathophysiology and development of the disorder. We investigated rsFC changes of visual networks in cLBP patients. rsFC of the primary visual network with S1 and M1 increased in cLBP patients. rsFC of the visual networks can differentiate cLBP patients from controls (with 79.3% accuracy). Classification results can be validated in an independent cohort (with 66.7% accuracy).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Wei Shen
- Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA; First Affiliated Hospital of Hainan Medical College, Hainan Medical University, Haikou, Hainan, China
| | - Yiheng Tu
- Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Randy L Gollub
- Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Ana Ortiz
- Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Vitaly Napadow
- Department of Radiology, Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Charlestown, MA, USA
| | - Siyi Yu
- Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Georgia Wilson
- Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Joel Park
- Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Courtney Lang
- Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Minyoung Jung
- Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Jessica Gerber
- Department of Radiology, Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Charlestown, MA, USA
| | - Ishtiaq Mawla
- Department of Radiology, Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Charlestown, MA, USA
| | - Suk-Tak Chan
- Department of Radiology, Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Charlestown, MA, USA
| | - Ajay D Wasan
- Department of Anesthesiology, Center for Pain Research, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, USA
| | - Robert R Edwards
- Department of Anesthesiology, Perioperative and Pain Medicine, Brigham and Women's Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Ted Kaptchuk
- Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Shasha Li
- Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Bruce Rosen
- Department of Radiology, Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Charlestown, MA, USA
| | - Jian Kong
- Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Abstract
The construction of a coherent representation of our body and the mapping of the space immediately surrounding it are of the highest ecological importance. This space has at least three specificities: it is a space where actions are planned in order to interact with our environment; it is a space that contributes to the experience of self and self-boundaries, through tactile processing and multisensory interactions; last, it is a space that contributes to the experience of body integrity against external events. In the last decades, numerous studies have been interested in peripersonal space (PPS), defined as the space directly surrounding us and which we can interact with (for reviews, see Cléry et al., 2015b; de Vignemont and Iannetti, 2015; di Pellegrino and Làdavas, 2015). These studies have contributed to the understanding of how this space is constructed, encoded and modulated. The majority of these studies focused on subparts of PPS (the hand, the face or the trunk) and very few of them investigated the interaction between PPS subparts. In the present review, we summarize the latest advances in this research and we discuss the new perspectives that are set forth for futures investigations on this topic. We describe the most recent methods used to estimate PPS boundaries by the means of dynamic stimuli. We then highlight how impact prediction and approaching stimuli modulate this space by social, emotional and action-related components involving principally a parieto-frontal network. In a next step, we review evidence that there is not a unique representation of PPS but at least three sub-sections (hand, face and trunk PPS). Last, we discuss how these subspaces interact, and we question whether and how bodily self-consciousness (BSC) is functionally and behaviorally linked to PPS.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Justine Cléry
- UMR5229, Institut des Sciences Cognitives Marc Jeannerod, CNRS-Université Claude Bernard Lyon I, Bron, France
| | - Suliann Ben Hamed
- UMR5229, Institut des Sciences Cognitives Marc Jeannerod, CNRS-Université Claude Bernard Lyon I, Bron, France
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
No perceptual prioritization of non-nociceptive vibrotactile and visual stimuli presented on a sensitized body part. Sci Rep 2018; 8:5359. [PMID: 29599492 PMCID: PMC5876401 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-018-23135-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/19/2017] [Accepted: 03/05/2018] [Indexed: 12/13/2022] Open
Abstract
High frequency electrical conditioning stimulation (HFS) is an experimental method to induce increased mechanical pinprick sensitivity in the unconditioned surrounding skin (secondary hyperalgesia). Secondary hyperalgesia is thought to be the result of central sensitization, i.e. increased responsiveness of nociceptive neurons in the central nervous system. Vibrotactile and visual stimuli presented in the area of secondary hyperalgesia also elicit enhanced brain responses, a finding that cannot be explained by central sensitization as it is currently defined. HFS may recruit attentional processes, which in turn affect the processing of all stimuli. In this study we have investigated whether HFS induces perceptual biases towards stimuli presented onto the sensitized arm by using Temporal Order Judgment (TOJ) tasks. In TOJ tasks, stimuli are presented in rapid succession on either arm, and participants have to indicate their perceived order. In case of a perceptual bias, the stimuli presented on the attended side are systematically reported as occurring first. Participants performed a tactile and a visual TOJ task before and after HFS. Analyses of participants' performance did not reveal any prioritization of the visual and tactile stimuli presented onto the sensitized arm. Our results provide therefore no evidence for a perceptual bias towards tactile and visual stimuli presented onto the sensitized arm.
Collapse
|
15
|
Legrain V, Manfron L, Garcia M, Filbrich L. Does Body Perception Shape Visuospatial Perception? Perception 2018; 47:507-520. [DOI: 10.1177/0301006618763269] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
How we perceive our body is shaped by sensory experiences with our surrounding environment, as witnessed by poor performance in tasks during which participants judge with their hands crossed the temporal order between two somatosensory stimuli, one applied on each hand. This suggests that somatosensory stimuli are not only processed according to a somatotopic representation but also a spatiotopic representation of the body. We investigated whether the perception of stimuli occurring in external space, such as visual stimuli, can also be influenced by the body posture and somatosensory stimuli. Participants performed temporal order judgements on pairs of visual stimuli, one in each side of space, with their hands uncrossed or crossed. In Experiment 1, participants’ hands were placed either near or far from the visual stimuli. In Experiment 2, the visual stimuli were preceded, either by 60 ms or 360 ms, by tactile stimuli applied on the hands placed near the visual stimuli. Manipulating the time interval was intended to activate either a somatotopic or a spatiotopic representation of somatic inputs. We did not obtain any evidence for an influence of body posture on visual temporal order judgment, suggesting that body perception is less relevant for processing extrabody stimuli than the reverse.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Valéry Legrain
- Institute of Neuroscience, Université catholique de Louvain, Brussels, Belgium
| | - Louise Manfron
- Institute of Neuroscience, Université catholique de Louvain, Brussels, Belgium
| | - Marynn Garcia
- Institute of Neuroscience, Université catholique de Louvain, Brussels, Belgium
| | - Lieve Filbrich
- Institute of Neuroscience, Université catholique de Louvain, Brussels, Belgium
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Investigating the spatial characteristics of the crossmodal interaction between nociception and vision using gaze direction. Conscious Cogn 2018; 57:106-115. [DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2017.11.011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/30/2017] [Revised: 10/02/2017] [Accepted: 11/27/2017] [Indexed: 02/04/2023]
|