1
|
Nielsen KJ, Connor CE. How Shape Perception Works, in Two Dimensions and Three Dimensions. Annu Rev Vis Sci 2024; 10:47-68. [PMID: 38848596 DOI: 10.1146/annurev-vision-112823-031607] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/09/2024]
Abstract
The ventral visual pathway transforms retinal images into neural representations that support object understanding, including exquisite appreciation of precise 2D pattern shape and 3D volumetric shape. We articulate a framework for understanding the goals of this transformation and how they are achieved by neural coding at successive ventral pathway stages. The critical goals are (a) radical compression to make shape information communicable across axonal bundles and storable in memory, (b) explicit coding to make shape information easily readable by the rest of the brain and thus accessible for cognition and behavioral control, and (c) representational stability to maintain consistent perception across highly variable viewing conditions. We describe how each transformational step in ventral pathway vision serves one or more of these goals. This three-goal framework unifies discoveries about ventral shape processing into a neural explanation for our remarkable experience of shape as a vivid, richly detailed aspect of the natural world.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kristina J Nielsen
- Krieger Mind/Brain Institute and Department of Neuroscience, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, Maryland, USA; ,
| | - Charles E Connor
- Krieger Mind/Brain Institute and Department of Neuroscience, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, Maryland, USA; ,
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Xiao ZC, Lin KK, Young LS. Efficient models of cortical activity via local dynamic equilibria and coarse-grained interactions. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2024; 121:e2320454121. [PMID: 38923983 PMCID: PMC11228477 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.2320454121] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/21/2023] [Accepted: 05/14/2024] [Indexed: 06/28/2024] Open
Abstract
Biologically detailed models of brain circuitry are challenging to build and simulate due to the large number of neurons, their complex interactions, and the many unknown physiological parameters. Simplified mathematical models are more tractable, but harder to evaluate when too far removed from neuroanatomy/physiology. We propose that a multiscale model, coarse-grained (CG) while preserving local biological details, offers the best balance between biological realism and computability. This paper presents such a model. Generally, CG models focus on the interaction between groups of neurons-here termed "pixels"-rather than individual cells. In our case, dynamics are alternately updated at intra- and interpixel scales, with one informing the other, until convergence to equilibrium is achieved on both scales. An innovation is how we exploit the underlying biology: Taking advantage of the similarity in local anatomical structures across large regions of the cortex, we model intrapixel dynamics as a single dynamical system driven by "external" inputs. These inputs vary with events external to the pixel, but their ranges can be estimated a priori. Precomputing and tabulating all potential local responses speed up the updating procedure significantly compared to direct multiscale simulation. We illustrate our methodology using a model of the primate visual cortex. Except for local neuron-to-neuron variability (necessarily lost in any CG approximation) our model reproduces various features of large-scale network models at a tiny fraction of the computational cost. These include neuronal responses as a consequence of their orientation selectivity, a primary function of visual neurons.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Zhuo-Cheng Xiao
- New York University - East China Normal University Institute of Mathematical Sciences, New York University, Shanghai200124, China
- Institute of Brain and Cognitive Science, New York University - East China Normal University, New York University, Shanghai200124, China
- College of Art and Sciences, New York University, Shanghai200124, China
| | - Kevin K. Lin
- Department of Mathematics, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ85721
| | - Lai-Sang Young
- Department of Mathematics, Courant Institute of Mathematical Sciences, New York University, New York, NY10012
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Wu J, Chen Y, Veeraraghavan A, Seidemann E, Robinson JT. Mesoscopic calcium imaging in a head-unrestrained male non-human primate using a lensless microscope. Nat Commun 2024; 15:1271. [PMID: 38341403 PMCID: PMC10858944 DOI: 10.1038/s41467-024-45417-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/06/2023] [Accepted: 01/22/2024] [Indexed: 02/12/2024] Open
Abstract
Mesoscopic calcium imaging enables studies of cell-type specific neural activity over large areas. A growing body of literature suggests that neural activity can be different when animals are free to move compared to when they are restrained. Unfortunately, existing systems for imaging calcium dynamics over large areas in non-human primates (NHPs) are table-top devices that require restraint of the animal's head. Here, we demonstrate an imaging device capable of imaging mesoscale calcium activity in a head-unrestrained male non-human primate. We successfully miniaturize our system by replacing lenses with an optical mask and computational algorithms. The resulting lensless microscope can fit comfortably on an NHP, allowing its head to move freely while imaging. We are able to measure orientation columns maps over a 20 mm2 field-of-view in a head-unrestrained macaque. Our work establishes mesoscopic imaging using a lensless microscope as a powerful approach for studying neural activity under more naturalistic conditions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jimin Wu
- Department of Bioengineering, Rice University, 6100 Main Street, Houston, TX, 77005, USA
| | - Yuzhi Chen
- Department of Neuroscience, University of Texas at Austin, 100 E 24th St., Austin, TX, 78712, USA
- Department of Psychology, University of Texas at Austin, 108 E Dean Keeton St., Austin, TX, 78712, USA
| | - Ashok Veeraraghavan
- Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering, Rice University, 6100 Main Street, Houston, TX, 77005, USA
- Department of Computer Science, Rice University, 6100 Main Street, Houston, TX, 77005, USA
| | - Eyal Seidemann
- Department of Neuroscience, University of Texas at Austin, 100 E 24th St., Austin, TX, 78712, USA.
- Department of Psychology, University of Texas at Austin, 108 E Dean Keeton St., Austin, TX, 78712, USA.
| | - Jacob T Robinson
- Department of Bioengineering, Rice University, 6100 Main Street, Houston, TX, 77005, USA.
- Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering, Rice University, 6100 Main Street, Houston, TX, 77005, USA.
- Department of Neuroscience, Baylor College of Medicine, One Baylor Plaza, Houston, TX, 77030, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Himmelberg MM, Winawer J, Carrasco M. Polar angle asymmetries in visual perception and neural architecture. Trends Neurosci 2023; 46:445-458. [PMID: 37031051 PMCID: PMC10192146 DOI: 10.1016/j.tins.2023.03.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 13.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/05/2023] [Revised: 03/06/2023] [Accepted: 03/10/2023] [Indexed: 04/10/2023]
Abstract
Human visual performance changes with visual field location. It is best at the center of gaze and declines with eccentricity, and also varies markedly with polar angle. These perceptual polar angle asymmetries are linked to asymmetries in the organization of the visual system. We review and integrate research quantifying how performance changes with visual field location and how this relates to neural organization at multiple stages of the visual system. We first briefly review how performance varies with eccentricity and the neural foundations of this effect. We then focus on perceptual polar angle asymmetries and their neural foundations. Characterizing perceptual and neural variations across and around the visual field contributes to our understanding of how the brain translates visual signals into neural representations which form the basis of visual perception.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marc M Himmelberg
- Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, NY 10003, USA; Center for Neural Science, New York University, New York, NY 10003, USA.
| | - Jonathan Winawer
- Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, NY 10003, USA; Center for Neural Science, New York University, New York, NY 10003, USA
| | - Marisa Carrasco
- Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, NY 10003, USA; Center for Neural Science, New York University, New York, NY 10003, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Benson NC, Yoon JMD, Forenzo D, Engel SA, Kay KN, Winawer J. Variability of the Surface Area of the V1, V2, and V3 Maps in a Large Sample of Human Observers. J Neurosci 2022; 42:8629-8646. [PMID: 36180226 PMCID: PMC9671582 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.0690-21.2022] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/01/2021] [Revised: 09/08/2022] [Accepted: 09/16/2022] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
How variable is the functionally defined structure of early visual areas in human cortex and how much variability is shared between twins? Here we quantify individual differences in the best understood functionally defined regions of cortex: V1, V2, V3. The Human Connectome Project 7T Retinotopy Dataset includes retinotopic measurements from 181 subjects (109 female, 72 male), including many twins. We trained four "anatomists" to manually define V1-V3 using retinotopic features. These definitions were more accurate than automated anatomical templates and showed that surface areas for these maps varied more than threefold across individuals. This threefold variation was little changed when normalizing visual area size by the surface area of the entire cerebral cortex. In addition to varying in size, we find that visual areas vary in how they sample the visual field. Specifically, the cortical magnification function differed substantially among individuals, with the relative amount of cortex devoted to central vision varying by more than a factor of 2. To complement the variability analysis, we examined the similarity of visual area size and structure across twins. Whereas the twin sample sizes are too small to make precise heritability estimates (50 monozygotic pairs, 34 dizygotic pairs), they nonetheless reveal high correlations, consistent with strong effects of the combination of shared genes and environment on visual area size. Collectively, these results provide the most comprehensive account of individual variability in visual area structure to date, and provide a robust population benchmark against which new individuals and developmental and clinical populations can be compared.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Areas V1, V2, and V3 are among the best studied functionally defined regions in human cortex. Using the largest retinotopy dataset to date, we characterized the variability of these regions across individuals and the similarity between twin pairs. We find that the size of visual areas varies dramatically (up to 3.5×) across healthy young adults, far more than the variability of the cerebral cortex size as a whole. Much of this variability appears to arise from inherited factors, as we find very high correlations in visual area size between monozygotic twin pairs, and lower but still substantial correlations between dizygotic twin pairs. These results provide the most comprehensive assessment of how functionally defined visual cortex varies across the population to date.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Noah C Benson
- eScience Institute, University of Washington, Seattle, Washington 98195
| | - Jennifer M D Yoon
- Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, New York 10003
- Center for Neural Sciences, New York University, New York, New York 10003
| | - Dylan Forenzo
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania 15213
| | - Stephen A Engel
- Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, Minnesota 55455
| | - Kendrick N Kay
- Center for Magnetic Resonance Research, Department of Radiology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, Minnesota 55455
| | - Jonathan Winawer
- Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, New York 10003
- Center for Neural Sciences, New York University, New York, New York 10003
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Sedigh-Sarvestani M, Fitzpatrick D. What and Where: Location-Dependent Feature Sensitivity as a Canonical Organizing Principle of the Visual System. Front Neural Circuits 2022; 16:834876. [PMID: 35498372 PMCID: PMC9039279 DOI: 10.3389/fncir.2022.834876] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/13/2021] [Accepted: 03/01/2022] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Traditionally, functional representations in early visual areas are conceived as retinotopic maps preserving ego-centric spatial location information while ensuring that other stimulus features are uniformly represented for all locations in space. Recent results challenge this framework of relatively independent encoding of location and features in the early visual system, emphasizing location-dependent feature sensitivities that reflect specialization of cortical circuits for different locations in visual space. Here we review the evidence for such location-specific encoding including: (1) systematic variation of functional properties within conventional retinotopic maps in the cortex; (2) novel periodic retinotopic transforms that dramatically illustrate the tight linkage of feature sensitivity, spatial location, and cortical circuitry; and (3) retinotopic biases in cortical areas, and groups of areas, that have been defined by their functional specializations. We propose that location-dependent feature sensitivity is a fundamental organizing principle of the visual system that achieves efficient representation of positional regularities in visual experience, and reflects the evolutionary selection of sensory and motor circuits to optimally represent behaviorally relevant information. Future studies are necessary to discover mechanisms underlying joint encoding of location and functional information, how this relates to behavior, emerges during development, and varies across species.
Collapse
|
7
|
Ta D, Tu Y, Lu ZL, Wang Y. Quantitative characterization of the human retinotopic map based on quasiconformal mapping. Med Image Anal 2022; 75:102230. [PMID: 34666194 PMCID: PMC8678293 DOI: 10.1016/j.media.2021.102230] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/12/2021] [Revised: 07/11/2021] [Accepted: 09/10/2021] [Indexed: 01/03/2023]
Abstract
The retinotopic map depicts the cortical neurons' response to visual stimuli on the retina and has contributed significantly to our understanding of human visual system. Although recent advances in high field functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) have made it possible to generate the in vivo retinotopic map with great detail, quantifying the map remains challenging. Existing quantification methods do not preserve surface topology and often introduce large geometric distortions to the map. In this study, we developed a new framework based on computational conformal geometry and quasiconformal Teichmüller theory to quantify the retinotopic map. Specifically, we introduced a general pipeline, consisting of cortical surface conformal parameterization, surface-spline-based cortical activation signal smoothing, and vertex-wise Beltrami coefficient-based map description. After correcting most of the violations of the topological conditions, the result was a "Beltrami coefficient map" (BCM) that rigorously and completely characterizes the retinotopic map by quantifying the local quasiconformal mapping distortion at each visual field location. The BCM provided topological and fully reconstructable retinotopic maps. We successfully applied the new framework to analyze the V1 retinotopic maps from the Human Connectome Project (n=181), the largest state of the art retinotopy dataset currently available. With unprecedented precision, we found that the V1 retinotopic map was quasiconformal and the local mapping distortions were similar across observers. The new framework can be applied to other visual areas and retinotopic maps of individuals with and without eye diseases, and improve our understanding of visual cortical organization in normal and clinical populations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Duyan Ta
- School of Computing and Augmented Intelligence, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ, USA
| | - Yanshuai Tu
- School of Computing and Augmented Intelligence, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ, USA
| | - Zhong-Lin Lu
- Division of Arts and Sciences, New York University Shanghai, Shanghai, China; Center for Neural Science and Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, NY, USA; NYU-ECNU Institute of Brain and Cognitive Science at NYU Shanghai, Shanghai, China
| | - Yalin Wang
- School of Computing and Augmented Intelligence, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Kupers ER, Benson NC, Carrasco M, Winawer J. Asymmetries around the visual field: From retina to cortex to behavior. PLoS Comput Biol 2022; 18:e1009771. [PMID: 35007281 PMCID: PMC8782511 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1009771] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/20/2020] [Revised: 01/21/2022] [Accepted: 12/19/2021] [Indexed: 11/29/2022] Open
Abstract
Visual performance varies around the visual field. It is best near the fovea compared to the periphery, and at iso-eccentric locations it is best on the horizontal, intermediate on the lower, and poorest on the upper meridian. The fovea-to-periphery performance decline is linked to the decreases in cone density, retinal ganglion cell (RGC) density, and V1 cortical magnification factor (CMF) as eccentricity increases. The origins of polar angle asymmetries are not well understood. Optical quality and cone density vary across the retina, but recent computational modeling has shown that these factors can only account for a small percentage of behavior. Here, we investigate how visual processing beyond the cone photon absorptions contributes to polar angle asymmetries in performance. First, we quantify the extent of asymmetries in cone density, midget RGC density, and V1 CMF. We find that both polar angle asymmetries and eccentricity gradients increase from cones to mRGCs, and from mRGCs to cortex. Second, we extend our previously published computational observer model to quantify the contribution of phototransduction by the cones and spatial filtering by mRGCs to behavioral asymmetries. Starting with photons emitted by a visual display, the model simulates the effect of human optics, cone isomerizations, phototransduction, and mRGC spatial filtering. The model performs a forced choice orientation discrimination task on mRGC responses using a linear support vector machine classifier. The model shows that asymmetries in a decision maker's performance across polar angle are greater when assessing the photocurrents than when assessing isomerizations and are greater still when assessing mRGC signals. Nonetheless, the polar angle asymmetries of the mRGC outputs are still considerably smaller than those observed from human performance. We conclude that cone isomerizations, phototransduction, and the spatial filtering properties of mRGCs contribute to polar angle performance differences, but that a full account of these differences will entail additional contribution from cortical representations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Eline R. Kupers
- Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, New York, United States of America
- Center for Neural Sciences, New York University, New York, New York, United States of America
| | - Noah C. Benson
- Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, New York, United States of America
- Center for Neural Sciences, New York University, New York, New York, United States of America
| | - Marisa Carrasco
- Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, New York, United States of America
- Center for Neural Sciences, New York University, New York, New York, United States of America
| | - Jonathan Winawer
- Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, New York, United States of America
- Center for Neural Sciences, New York University, New York, New York, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Sedigh-Sarvestani M, Lee KS, Jaepel J, Satterfield R, Shultz N, Fitzpatrick D. A sinusoidal transformation of the visual field is the basis for periodic maps in area V2. Neuron 2021; 109:4068-4079.e6. [PMID: 34687665 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2021.09.053] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/29/2021] [Revised: 07/21/2021] [Accepted: 09/23/2021] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
Retinotopic maps of many visual areas are thought to follow the fundamental principles described for the primary visual cortex (V1), where nearby points on the retina map to nearby points on the surface of V1, and orthogonal axes of the retinal surface are represented along orthogonal axes of the cortical surface. Here we demonstrate a striking departure from this mapping in the secondary visual area (V2) of the tree shrew best described as a sinusoidal transformation of the visual field. This sinusoidal topography is ideal for achieving uniform coverage in an elongated area like V2, as predicted by mathematical models designed for wiring minimization, and provides a novel explanation for periodic banded patterns of intra-cortical connections and functional response properties in V2 of tree shrews as well as several other species. Our findings suggest that cortical circuits flexibly implement solutions to sensory surface representation, with dramatic consequences for large-scale cortical organization.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Madineh Sedigh-Sarvestani
- Functional Architecture and Development of Cerebral Cortex, Max Planck Florida Institute for Neuroscience, Jupiter, FL 33458, USA.
| | - Kuo-Sheng Lee
- Department of Basic Neurosciences, University of Geneva, Geneva 1211, Switzerland
| | - Juliane Jaepel
- Functional Architecture and Development of Cerebral Cortex, Max Planck Florida Institute for Neuroscience, Jupiter, FL 33458, USA
| | - Rachel Satterfield
- Functional Architecture and Development of Cerebral Cortex, Max Planck Florida Institute for Neuroscience, Jupiter, FL 33458, USA
| | - Nicole Shultz
- Functional Architecture and Development of Cerebral Cortex, Max Planck Florida Institute for Neuroscience, Jupiter, FL 33458, USA
| | - David Fitzpatrick
- Functional Architecture and Development of Cerebral Cortex, Max Planck Florida Institute for Neuroscience, Jupiter, FL 33458, USA
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Tehovnik EJ, Froudarakis E, Scala F, Smirnakis SM, Patel SS, Tolias AS. Visuomotor control in mice and primates. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2021; 130:185-200. [PMID: 34416241 PMCID: PMC10508359 DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.08.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/21/2021] [Revised: 07/30/2021] [Accepted: 08/09/2021] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
Abstract
We conduct a comparative evaluation of the visual systems from the retina to the muscles of the mouse and the macaque monkey noting the differences and similarities between these two species. The topics covered include (1) visual-field overlap, (2) visual spatial resolution, (3) V1 cortical point-image [i.e., V1 tissue dedicated to analyzing a unit receptive field], (4) object versus motion encoding, (5) oculomotor range, (6) eye, head, and body movement coordination, and (7) neocortical and cerebellar function. We also discuss blindsight in rodents and primates which provides insights on how the neocortex mediates conscious vision in these species. This review is timely because the field of visuomotor neurophysiology is expanding beyond the macaque monkey to include the mouse; there is therefore a need for a comparative analysis between these two species on how the brain generates visuomotor responses.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- E J Tehovnik
- Department of Neuroscience, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, TX, USA; Center for Neuroscience and Artificial Intelligence, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, TX, USA.
| | - E Froudarakis
- Institute of Molecular Biology and Biotechnology, Foundation for Research and Technology Hellas, Heraklion, Greece
| | - F Scala
- Department of Neuroscience, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, TX, USA; Center for Neuroscience and Artificial Intelligence, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, TX, USA
| | - S M Smirnakis
- Department of Neurology, Brigham and Women's Hospital and Jamaica Plain Veterans Administration Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
| | - S S Patel
- Department of Neuroscience, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, TX, USA; Center for Neuroscience and Artificial Intelligence, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, TX, USA
| | - A S Tolias
- Department of Neuroscience, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, TX, USA; Center for Neuroscience and Artificial Intelligence, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, TX, USA; Department of Electrical Engineering and Computer Engineering, Rice University, Houston, TX, USA
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Linton P. V1 as an egocentric cognitive map. Neurosci Conscious 2021; 2021:niab017. [PMID: 34532068 PMCID: PMC8439394 DOI: 10.1093/nc/niab017] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/13/2021] [Revised: 05/21/2021] [Accepted: 06/08/2021] [Indexed: 01/20/2023] Open
Abstract
We typically distinguish between V1 as an egocentric perceptual map and the hippocampus as an allocentric cognitive map. In this article, we argue that V1 also functions as a post-perceptual egocentric cognitive map. We argue that three well-documented functions of V1, namely (i) the estimation of distance, (ii) the estimation of size, and (iii) multisensory integration, are better understood as post-perceptual cognitive inferences. This argument has two important implications. First, we argue that V1 must function as the neural correlates of the visual perception/cognition distinction and suggest how this can be accommodated by V1's laminar structure. Second, we use this insight to propose a low-level account of visual consciousness in contrast to mid-level accounts (recurrent processing theory; integrated information theory) and higher-level accounts (higher-order thought; global workspace theory). Detection thresholds have been traditionally used to rule out such an approach, but we explain why it is a mistake to equate visibility (and therefore the presence/absence of visual experience) with detection thresholds.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Paul Linton
- Centre for Applied Vision Research, City, University of London, Northampton Square, London EC1V 0HB, UK
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Benson NC, Kupers ER, Barbot A, Carrasco M, Winawer J. Cortical magnification in human visual cortex parallels task performance around the visual field. eLife 2021; 10:e67685. [PMID: 34342581 PMCID: PMC8378846 DOI: 10.7554/elife.67685] [Citation(s) in RCA: 45] [Impact Index Per Article: 11.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/19/2021] [Accepted: 08/02/2021] [Indexed: 12/03/2022] Open
Abstract
Human vision has striking radial asymmetries, with performance on many tasks varying sharply with stimulus polar angle. Performance is generally better on the horizontal than vertical meridian, and on the lower than upper vertical meridian, and these asymmetries decrease gradually with deviation from the vertical meridian. Here, we report cortical magnification at a fine angular resolution around the visual field. This precision enables comparisons between cortical magnification and behavior, between cortical magnification and retinal cell densities, and between cortical magnification in twin pairs. We show that cortical magnification in the human primary visual cortex, measured in 163 subjects, varies substantially around the visual field, with a pattern similar to behavior. These radial asymmetries in the cortex are larger than those found in the retina, and they are correlated between monozygotic twin pairs. These findings indicate a tight link between cortical topography and behavior, and suggest that visual field asymmetries are partly heritable.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Noah C Benson
- Department of Psychology, New York UniversityNew YorkUnited States
- Center for Neural Sciences, New York UniversityNew YorkUnited States
| | - Eline R Kupers
- Department of Psychology, New York UniversityNew YorkUnited States
- Center for Neural Sciences, New York UniversityNew YorkUnited States
| | - Antoine Barbot
- Department of Psychology, New York UniversityNew YorkUnited States
- Center for Neural Sciences, New York UniversityNew YorkUnited States
| | - Marisa Carrasco
- Department of Psychology, New York UniversityNew YorkUnited States
- Center for Neural Sciences, New York UniversityNew YorkUnited States
| | - Jonathan Winawer
- Department of Psychology, New York UniversityNew YorkUnited States
- Center for Neural Sciences, New York UniversityNew YorkUnited States
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Tu Y, Ta D, Lu ZL, Wang Y. Topology-preserving smoothing of retinotopic maps. PLoS Comput Biol 2021; 17:e1009216. [PMID: 34339414 PMCID: PMC8360528 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1009216] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/21/2020] [Revised: 08/12/2021] [Accepted: 06/27/2021] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Retinotopic mapping, i.e., the mapping between visual inputs on the retina and neuronal activations in cortical visual areas, is one of the central topics in visual neuroscience. For human observers, the mapping is obtained by analyzing functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) signals of cortical responses to slowly moving visual stimuli on the retina. Although it is well known from neurophysiology that the mapping is topological (i.e., the topology of neighborhood connectivity is preserved) within each visual area, retinotopic maps derived from the state-of-the-art methods are often not topological because of the low signal-to-noise ratio and spatial resolution of fMRI. The violation of topological condition is most severe in cortical regions corresponding to the neighborhood of the fovea (e.g., < 1 degree eccentricity in the Human Connectome Project (HCP) dataset), significantly impeding accurate analysis of retinotopic maps. This study aims to directly model the topological condition and generate topology-preserving and smooth retinotopic maps. Specifically, we adopted the Beltrami coefficient, a metric of quasiconformal mapping, to define the topological condition, developed a mathematical model to quantify topological smoothing as a constrained optimization problem, and elaborated an efficient numerical method to solve the problem. The method was then applied to V1, V2, and V3 simultaneously in the HCP dataset. Experiments with both simulated and real retinotopy data demonstrated that the proposed method could generate topological and smooth retinotopic maps.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yanshuai Tu
- School of Computing, Informatics, and Decision Systems Engineering, Arizona State University, Tempe, Arizona, United States of America
| | - Duyan Ta
- School of Computing, Informatics, and Decision Systems Engineering, Arizona State University, Tempe, Arizona, United States of America
| | - Zhong-Lin Lu
- Division of Arts and Sciences, New York University Shanghai, Shanghai, China
- Center for Neural Science and Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, United States of America
- NYU-ECNU Institute of Brain and Cognitive Science, NYU Shanghai, Shanghai, China
| | - Yalin Wang
- School of Computing, Informatics, and Decision Systems Engineering, Arizona State University, Tempe, Arizona, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Barbot A, Xue S, Carrasco M. Asymmetries in visual acuity around the visual field. J Vis 2021; 21:2. [PMID: 33393963 PMCID: PMC7794272 DOI: 10.1167/jov.21.1.2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 61] [Impact Index Per Article: 15.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/13/2020] [Accepted: 11/10/2020] [Indexed: 12/21/2022] Open
Abstract
Human vision is heterogeneous around the visual field. At a fixed eccentricity, performance is better along the horizontal than the vertical meridian and along the lower than the upper vertical meridian. These asymmetric patterns, termed performance fields, have been found in numerous visual tasks, including those mediated by contrast sensitivity and spatial resolution. However, it is unknown whether spatial resolution asymmetries are confined to the cardinal meridians or whether and how far they extend into the upper and lower hemifields. Here, we measured visual acuity at isoeccentric peripheral locations (10 deg eccentricity), every 15° of polar angle. On each trial, observers judged the orientation (± 45°) of one of four equidistant, suprathreshold grating stimuli varying in spatial frequency (SF). On each block, we measured performance as a function of stimulus SF at 4 of 24 isoeccentric locations. We estimated the 75%-correct SF threshold, SF cutoff point (i.e., chance-level), and slope of the psychometric function for each location. We found higher SF estimates (i.e., better acuity) for the horizontal than the vertical meridian and for the lower than the upper vertical meridian. These asymmetries were most pronounced at the cardinal meridians and decreased gradually as the angular distance from the vertical meridian increased. This gradual change in acuity with polar angle reflected a shift of the psychometric function without changes in slope. The same pattern was found under binocular and monocular viewing conditions. These findings advance our understanding of visual processing around the visual field and help constrain models of visual perception.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Antoine Barbot
- Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, NY, USA
- Center for Neural Science, New York University, New York, NY, USA
- Spinoza Centre for Neuroimaging, Amsterdam, Netherlands
| | - Shutian Xue
- Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, NY, USA
| | - Marisa Carrasco
- Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, NY, USA
- Center for Neural Science, New York University, New York, NY, USA
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Tu Y, Ta D, Lu ZL, Wang Y. DIFFEOMORPHIC SMOOTHING FOR RETINOTOPIC MAPPING. PROCEEDINGS. IEEE INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON BIOMEDICAL IMAGING 2020; 2020:534-538. [PMID: 32765810 DOI: 10.1109/isbi45749.2020.9098316] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Retinotopic mapping, the mapping of visual input on the retina to cortical neurons, is an important topic in vision science. Typically, cortical neurons are related to visual input on the retina using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) of cortical responses to slowly moving visual stimuli on the retina. Although it is well known from neurophysiology studies that retinotopic mapping is locally diffeomorphic (i.e., smooth, differentiable, and invertible) within each local area, the retinotopic maps from fMRI are often not diffeomorphic, especially near the fovea, because of the low signal-noise ratio of fMRI. The aim of this study is to develop and solve a mathematical model that produces diffeomorphic retinotopic mapping from fMRI data. Specifically, we adopt a geometry concept, the Beltrami coefficient, as the tool to define diffeomorphism, and model the problem in an optimization framework. Efficient numerical methods are proposed to solve the model. Experimental results with both synthetic and real retinotopy data demonstrate that the proposed method is superior to conventional smoothing methods.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yanshuai Tu
- School of Computing, Informatics, Decision Systems Engineering, Arizona State Univ., Tempe, AZ
| | - Duyan Ta
- School of Computing, Informatics, Decision Systems Engineering, Arizona State Univ., Tempe, AZ
| | - Zhong-Lin Lu
- Division of Arts and Sciences, NYU Shanghai, Shanghai, China.,Center for Neural Science and Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, NY
| | - Yalin Wang
- School of Computing, Informatics, Decision Systems Engineering, Arizona State Univ., Tempe, AZ
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Zajzon B, Mahmoudian S, Morrison A, Duarte R. Passing the Message: Representation Transfer in Modular Balanced Networks. Front Comput Neurosci 2019; 13:79. [PMID: 31920605 PMCID: PMC6915101 DOI: 10.3389/fncom.2019.00079] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/19/2019] [Accepted: 10/29/2019] [Indexed: 01/08/2023] Open
Abstract
Neurobiological systems rely on hierarchical and modular architectures to carry out intricate computations using minimal resources. A prerequisite for such systems to operate adequately is the capability to reliably and efficiently transfer information across multiple modules. Here, we study the features enabling a robust transfer of stimulus representations in modular networks of spiking neurons, tuned to operate in a balanced regime. To capitalize on the complex, transient dynamics that such networks exhibit during active processing, we apply reservoir computing principles and probe the systems' computational efficacy with specific tasks. Focusing on the comparison of random feed-forward connectivity and biologically inspired topographic maps, we find that, in a sequential set-up, structured projections between the modules are strictly necessary for information to propagate accurately to deeper modules. Such mappings not only improve computational performance and efficiency, they also reduce response variability, increase robustness against interference effects, and boost memory capacity. We further investigate how information from two separate input streams is integrated and demonstrate that it is more advantageous to perform non-linear computations on the input locally, within a given module, and subsequently transfer the result downstream, rather than transferring intermediate information and performing the computation downstream. Depending on how information is integrated early on in the system, the networks achieve similar task-performance using different strategies, indicating that the dimensionality of the neural responses does not necessarily correlate with nonlinear integration, as predicted by previous studies. These findings highlight a key role of topographic maps in supporting fast, robust, and accurate neural communication over longer distances. Given the prevalence of such structural feature, particularly in the sensory systems, elucidating their functional purpose remains an important challenge toward which this work provides relevant, new insights. At the same time, these results shed new light on important requirements for designing functional hierarchical spiking networks.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Barna Zajzon
- Jülich Research Centre, Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine (INM-6), Institute for Advanced Simulation (IAS-6) and JARA Institute Brain Structure-Function Relationships (JBI-1/INM-10), Jülich, Germany
- Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, RWTH Aachen University, Aachen, Germany
| | - Sepehr Mahmoudian
- Department of Data-Driven Analysis of Biological Networks, Campus Institute for Dynamics of Biological Networks, Georg August University Göttingen, Göttingen, Germany
- MEG Unit, Brain Imaging Center, Goethe University, Frankfurt, Germany
| | - Abigail Morrison
- Jülich Research Centre, Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine (INM-6), Institute for Advanced Simulation (IAS-6) and JARA Institute Brain Structure-Function Relationships (JBI-1/INM-10), Jülich, Germany
- Faculty of Psychology, Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, Ruhr-University Bochum, Bochum, Germany
| | - Renato Duarte
- Jülich Research Centre, Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine (INM-6), Institute for Advanced Simulation (IAS-6) and JARA Institute Brain Structure-Function Relationships (JBI-1/INM-10), Jülich, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
To MPS, Carvey KM, Carvey RJ, Liu CH. Averaging sets of expressive faces is modulated by eccentricity. J Vis 2019; 19:2. [PMID: 31480073 DOI: 10.1167/19.11.2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
Research has shown that participants can extract the average facial expression from a set of faces when these were presented at fixation. In this study, we investigated whether this performance would be modulated by eccentricity given that neural resources are limited outside the foveal region. We also examined whether or not there would be compulsory averaging in the parafovea as has been previously reported for the orientation of Gabor patches by Parkes, Lund, Angelucci, Solomon, and Morgan (2001). Participants were presented with expressive faces (alone or in sets of nine, at fixation or at 3° to the left or right) and were asked to identify the expression of the central target face or to estimate the average expression of the set. Our results revealed that, although participants were able to extract average facial expressions in central and parafoveal conditions, their performance was superior in the parafovea, suggesting facilitated averaging outside the fovea by peripheral mechanisms. Furthermore, regardless of whether the task was to judge the expression of the central target or set average, participants had a tendency to identify central targets' expressions in the fovea but were compelled to average in the parafovea, a finding consistent with compulsory averaging. The data also supported averaging over substitution models of crowding. We conclude that the ability to extract average expressions in sets of faces and identify single targets' facial expressions is influenced by eccentricity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michelle P S To
- Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, Lancaster, UK.,Department of Psychology, University of Hull, Hull, UK
| | | | | | - Chang Hong Liu
- Department of Psychology Bournemouth University, Poole, UK.,Department of Psychology, University of Hull, Hull, UK
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Linkage between retinal ganglion cell density and the nonuniform spatial integration across the visual field. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2019; 116:3827-3836. [PMID: 30737290 PMCID: PMC6397585 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1817076116] [Citation(s) in RCA: 32] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
The integration of visual information over space is critical to human pattern vision. For either luminance detection or object recognition, the position of the target in the visual field governs the size of a window within which visual information is integrated. Here we analyze the relationship between the topographic distribution of ganglion cell density and the nonuniform spatial integration across the visual field. We find that the variation in the retinal ganglion cell (RGC) density across the human retina is closely matched to the variation in the extent of spatial integration. Our study suggests that a fixed number of RGCs subserves spatial integration of visual input, independent of the visual-field location. The ability to integrate visual information over space is a fundamental component of human pattern vision. Regardless of whether it is for detecting luminance contrast or for recognizing objects in a cluttered scene, the position of the target in the visual field governs the size of a window within which visual information is integrated. Here we analyze the relationship between the topographic distribution of ganglion cell density and the nonuniform spatial integration across the visual field. The extent of spatial integration for luminance detection (Ricco’s area) and object recognition (crowding zone) are measured at various target locations. The number of retinal ganglion cells (RGCs) underlying Ricco’s area or crowding zone is estimated by computing the product of Ricco’s area (or crowding zone) and RGC density for a given target location. We find a quantitative agreement between the behavioral data and the RGC density: The variation in the sampling density of RGCs across the human retina is closely matched to the variation in the extent of spatial integration required for either luminance detection or object recognition. Our empirical data combined with the simulation results of computational models suggest that a fixed number of RGCs subserves spatial integration of visual input, independent of the visual-field location.
Collapse
|
19
|
van Es DM, Theeuwes J, Knapen T. Spatial sampling in human visual cortex is modulated by both spatial and feature-based attention. eLife 2018; 7:e36928. [PMID: 30526848 PMCID: PMC6286128 DOI: 10.7554/elife.36928] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/26/2018] [Accepted: 11/13/2018] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Spatial attention changes the sampling of visual space. Behavioral studies suggest that feature-based attention modulates this resampling to optimize the attended feature's sampling. We investigate this hypothesis by estimating spatial sampling in visual cortex while independently varying both feature-based and spatial attention. Our results show that spatial and feature-based attention interacted: resampling of visual space depended on both the attended location and feature (color vs. temporal frequency). This interaction occurred similarly throughout visual cortex, regardless of an area's overall feature preference. However, the interaction did depend on spatial sampling properties of voxels that prefer the attended feature. These findings are parsimoniously explained by variations in the precision of an attentional gain field. Our results demonstrate that the deployment of spatial attention is tailored to the spatial sampling properties of units that are sensitive to the attended feature.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Daniel Marten van Es
- Behavioural and Movement SciencesVrije Universiteit AmsterdamAmsterdamThe Netherlands
| | - Jan Theeuwes
- Behavioural and Movement SciencesVrije Universiteit AmsterdamAmsterdamThe Netherlands
| | - Tomas Knapen
- Behavioural and Movement SciencesVrije Universiteit AmsterdamAmsterdamThe Netherlands
- Spinoza Centre for NeuroimagingRoyal Academy of SciencesAmsterdamThe Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Abstract
A long-term goal of visual neuroscience is to develop and test quantitative models that account for the moment-by-moment relationship between neural responses in early visual cortex and human performance in natural visual tasks. This review focuses on efforts to address this goal by measuring and perturbing the activity of primary visual cortex (V1) neurons while nonhuman primates perform demanding, well-controlled visual tasks. We start by describing a conceptual approach-the decoder linking model (DLM) framework-in which candidate decoding models take neural responses as input and generate predicted behavior as output. The ultimate goal in this framework is to find the actual decoder-the model that best predicts behavior from neural responses. We discuss key relevant properties of primate V1 and review current literature from the DLM perspective. We conclude by discussing major technological and theoretical advances that are likely to accelerate our understanding of the link between V1 activity and behavior.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Eyal Seidemann
- Center for Perceptual Systems, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, Texas 78712, USA; ,
- Department of Psychology, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, Texas 78712, USA
- Department of Neuroscience, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, Texas 78712, USA
| | - Wilson S Geisler
- Center for Perceptual Systems, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, Texas 78712, USA; ,
- Department of Psychology, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, Texas 78712, USA
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Abstract
The thalamocortical pathway is the main route of communication between the eye and the cerebral cortex. During embryonic development, thalamocortical afferents travel to L4 and are sorted by receptive field position, eye of origin, and contrast polarity (i.e., preference for light or dark stimuli). In primates and carnivores, this sorting involves numerous afferents, most of which sample a limited region of the binocular field. Devoting abundant thalamocortical resources to process a limited visual field has a clear advantage: It allows many stimulus combinations to be sampled at each spatial location. Moreover, the sampling efficiency can be further enhanced by organizing the afferents in a cortical grid for eye input and contrast polarity. We argue that thalamocortical interactions within this eye-polarity grid can be used to represent multiple stimulus combinations found in nature and to build an accurate cortical map for multidimensional stimulus space.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jens Kremkow
- Neuroscience Research Center, Charité - Universitätsmedizin Berlin, 10117 Berlin, Germany.,Institute for Theoretical Biology, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, 10115 Berlin, Germany
| | - Jose-Manuel Alonso
- Department of Biological and Visual Sciences, College of Optometry, State University of New York, New York, NY 10036, USA;
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Kirschfeld K. How we perceive our own retina. Proc Biol Sci 2018; 284:rspb.2017.1904. [PMID: 29070728 PMCID: PMC5666110 DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2017.1904] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/23/2017] [Accepted: 09/28/2017] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Ever since the days of René Descartes, in the seventeenth century, the search for the relationship between subjective perception and neural activity has been an ongoing challenge. In neuroscience, an approach to the problem via the visual system has produced a paradigm using perceptual suppression, changing with time. Cortical areas in which the neural activity was modulated in temporal correlation with this percept could be traced. Although these areas may lead directly to perception, such temporal correlation of neural activity does not suffice as ultimate proof that they actually do so. In this article, I will use a different method to show that, for the perception of our own retina, any brain area leading directly to this perception also needs to represent the retina without distortion. Furthermore, I will demonstrate that the phenomenon of size constancy must be realized in this area.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kuno Kirschfeld
- Max-Planck-Institute for Biological Cybernetics, Spemannstrasse 41, 72076 Tuebingen, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Silva MF, Brascamp JW, Ferreira S, Castelo-Branco M, Dumoulin SO, Harvey BM. Radial asymmetries in population receptive field size and cortical magnification factor in early visual cortex. Neuroimage 2018; 167:41-52. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.11.021] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/07/2016] [Revised: 09/08/2017] [Accepted: 11/12/2017] [Indexed: 10/18/2022] Open
|
24
|
Oga T, Okamoto T, Fujita I. Basal Dendrites of Layer-III Pyramidal Neurons do not Scale with Changes in Cortical Magnification Factor in Macaque Primary Visual Cortex. Front Neural Circuits 2016; 10:74. [PMID: 27708563 PMCID: PMC5030257 DOI: 10.3389/fncir.2016.00074] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/09/2016] [Accepted: 09/08/2016] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Neurons in the mammalian primary visual cortex (V1) are systematically arranged across the cortical surface according to the location of their receptive fields (RFs), forming a visuotopic (or retinotopic) map. Within this map, the foveal visual field is represented by a large cortical surface area, with increasingly peripheral visual fields gradually occupying smaller cortical areas. Although cellular organization in the retina, such as the spatial distribution of ganglion cells, can partially account for the eccentricity-dependent differences in the size of cortical representation, whether morphological differences exist across V1 neurons representing different eccentricities is unclear. In particular, morphological differences in dendritic field diameter might contribute to the magnified representation of the central visual field. Here, we addressed this question by measuring the basal dendritic arbors of pyramidal neurons of layer-IIIC and adjoining layer III sublayers (in the Hassler's nomenclature) in macaque V1. We labeled layer-III pyramidal neurons at various retinotopic positions in V1 by injecting lightly fixed brain tissue with intracellular dye, and then compared dendritic morphology across regions in the retinotopic map representing 0-20° of eccentricity. The dendritic field area, total dendritic length, number of principal dendrites, branching complexity, spine density and total number of spines were all consistent across different retinotopic regions of V1. These results indicate that dendrites in layer-III pyramidal neurons are relatively homogeneous according to these morphometric parameters irrespective of their locations in this portion of the retinotopic map. The homogeneity of dendritic morphology in these neurons suggests that the emphasis of central visual field representation is not attributable to changes in the basal dendritic arbors of pyramidal neurons in layer III, but is likely the result of successive processes earlier in the retino-geniculo-striate pathway.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tomofumi Oga
- Graduate School of Frontier Biosciences, Osaka UniversitySuita, Osaka, Japan
| | - Tsuguhisa Okamoto
- Graduate School of Frontier Biosciences, Osaka UniversitySuita, Osaka, Japan
| | - Ichiro Fujita
- Graduate School of Frontier Biosciences, Osaka UniversitySuita, Osaka, Japan
- Center for Information and Neural Networks (CiNet), National Institute of Information and Communications Technology and Osaka UniversitySuita, Osaka, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Seidemann E, Chen Y, Bai Y, Chen SC, Mehta P, Kajs BL, Geisler WS, Zemelman BV. Calcium imaging with genetically encoded indicators in behaving primates. eLife 2016; 5. [PMID: 27441501 PMCID: PMC4956408 DOI: 10.7554/elife.16178] [Citation(s) in RCA: 41] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/18/2016] [Accepted: 06/16/2016] [Indexed: 11/30/2022] Open
Abstract
Understanding the neural basis of behaviour requires studying brain activity in behaving subjects using complementary techniques that measure neural responses at multiple spatial scales, and developing computational tools for understanding the mapping between these measurements. Here we report the first results of widefield imaging of genetically encoded calcium indicator (GCaMP6f) signals from V1 of behaving macaques. This technique provides a robust readout of visual population responses at the columnar scale over multiple mm2 and over several months. To determine the quantitative relation between the widefield GCaMP signals and the locally pooled spiking activity, we developed a computational model that sums the responses of V1 neurons characterized by prior single unit measurements. The measured tuning properties of the GCaMP signals to stimulus contrast, orientation and spatial position closely match the predictions of the model, suggesting that widefield GCaMP signals are linearly related to the summed local spiking activity. DOI:http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.16178.001 An important question in brain research is how neurons and the circuits they form process information to produce behavior. To understand what happens in a human brain, it is necessary to study a brain of similar complexity, such as that of a primate. Examining how the neurons in a brain region called the visual cortex process information about what we see is especially informative. This is because animals can be taught to perform different visual tasks, and because the visual cortex is relatively easy to access. In principle, therefore, it should be possible to use modern genetic and imaging techniques to study the primate visual system, but, until now, that has not been the case. Like much of the brain, the visual cortex consists of different classes of neurons that can excite, inhibit or modulate the activity of neighboring neurons. One way to study how these different classes of neurons interact with each other is to alter the animal’s DNA, such that only one cell type stands out during the experiment, allowing its role in the brain to be closely monitored. This technique has been used to study the interactions among neurons in the rodent brain, because rodent DNA is easy to alter. However, it is not easy to manipulate primate DNA. Seidemann et al. have, therefore, developed a new technique that can target a specific class of neurons, allowing the activity of just these cells to be distinguished from the rest. The method uses specially designed harmless viruses to produce foreign proteins in the excitatory neurons of the visual cortex in an adult macaque. The optical properties of the proteins change when the neuron they are in is active, allowing the activity of the excitatory neurons to be detected and tracked in awake animals while they perform a visual task. Previously, the activity of neurons in the primate visual cortex could only be measured using dyes that indiscriminately reported the activity of all the neurons present. Seidemann et al. found that, in addition to being more selective than the dye-based method, the new technique also more accurately depicted neuronal action potentials, which are the primary units of information in the brain. Seidemann et al. now plan to use a similar method to study the activity of the inhibitory neurons of the primate visual cortex. Further examination of both excitatory and inhibitory neurons at much higher magnification, using a different microscopy technique, will also reveal more subtle features of their responses during visual tasks. DOI:http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.16178.002
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Eyal Seidemann
- Center for Perceptual Systems, University of Texas, Austin, United States.,Department of Psychology, University of Texas, Austin, United States.,Department of Neuroscience, University of Texas, Austin, United States
| | - Yuzhi Chen
- Center for Perceptual Systems, University of Texas, Austin, United States.,Department of Psychology, University of Texas, Austin, United States.,Department of Neuroscience, University of Texas, Austin, United States
| | - Yoon Bai
- Center for Perceptual Systems, University of Texas, Austin, United States.,Department of Psychology, University of Texas, Austin, United States.,Department of Neuroscience, University of Texas, Austin, United States
| | - Spencer C Chen
- Center for Perceptual Systems, University of Texas, Austin, United States.,Department of Psychology, University of Texas, Austin, United States.,Department of Neuroscience, University of Texas, Austin, United States
| | - Preeti Mehta
- Department of Neuroscience, University of Texas, Austin, United States.,Center for Learning and Memory, University of Texas, Austin, United States
| | - Bridget L Kajs
- Department of Neuroscience, University of Texas, Austin, United States.,Center for Learning and Memory, University of Texas, Austin, United States
| | - Wilson S Geisler
- Center for Perceptual Systems, University of Texas, Austin, United States.,Department of Psychology, University of Texas, Austin, United States
| | - Boris V Zemelman
- Department of Neuroscience, University of Texas, Austin, United States.,Center for Learning and Memory, University of Texas, Austin, United States
| |
Collapse
|
26
|
Zhu Y, Gabbiani F. Fine and distributed subcellular retinotopy of excitatory inputs to the dendritic tree of a collision-detecting neuron. J Neurophysiol 2016; 115:3101-12. [PMID: 27009157 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00044.2016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/16/2016] [Accepted: 03/21/2016] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Individual neurons in several sensory systems receive synaptic inputs organized according to subcellular topographic maps, yet the fine structure of this topographic organization and its relation to dendritic morphology have not been studied in detail. Subcellular topography is expected to play a role in dendritic integration, particularly when dendrites are extended and active. The lobula giant movement detector (LGMD) neuron in the locust visual system is known to receive topographic excitatory inputs on part of its dendritic tree. The LGMD responds preferentially to objects approaching on a collision course and is thought to implement several interesting dendritic computations. To study the fine retinotopic mapping of visual inputs onto the excitatory dendrites of the LGMD, we designed a custom microscope allowing visual stimulation at the native sampling resolution of the locust compound eye while simultaneously performing two-photon calcium imaging on excitatory dendrites. We show that the LGMD receives a distributed, fine retinotopic projection from the eye facets and that adjacent facets activate overlapping portions of the same dendritic branches. We also demonstrate that adjacent retinal inputs most likely make independent synapses on the excitatory dendrites of the LGMD. Finally, we show that the fine topographic mapping can be studied using dynamic visual stimuli. Our results reveal the detailed structure of the dendritic input originating from individual facets on the eye and their relation to that of adjacent facets. The mapping of visual space onto the LGMD's dendrites is expected to have implications for dendritic computation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ying Zhu
- Department of Neuroscience and Program of Structural and Computational Biology and Molecular Biophysics, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, Texas
| | - Fabrizio Gabbiani
- Department of Neuroscience and Program of Structural and Computational Biology and Molecular Biophysics, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, Texas
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Mahoney JR, Molholm S, Butler JS, Sehatpour P, Gomez-Ramirez M, Ritter W, Foxe JJ. Keeping in touch with the visual system: spatial alignment and multisensory integration of visual-somatosensory inputs. Front Psychol 2015; 6:1068. [PMID: 26300797 PMCID: PMC4525670 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01068] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/15/2015] [Accepted: 07/13/2015] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
Correlated sensory inputs coursing along the individual sensory processing hierarchies arrive at multisensory convergence zones in cortex where inputs are processed in an integrative manner. The exact hierarchical level of multisensory convergence zones and the timing of their inputs are still under debate, although increasingly, evidence points to multisensory integration (MSI) at very early sensory processing levels. While MSI is said to be governed by stimulus properties including space, time, and magnitude, violations of these rules have been documented. The objective of the current study was to determine, both psychophysically and electrophysiologically, whether differential visual-somatosensory (VS) integration patterns exist for stimuli presented to the same versus opposite hemifields. Using high-density electrical mapping and complementary psychophysical data, we examined multisensory integrative processing for combinations of visual and somatosensory inputs presented to both left and right spatial locations. We assessed how early during sensory processing VS interactions were seen in the event-related potential and whether spatial alignment of the visual and somatosensory elements resulted in differential integration effects. Reaction times to all VS pairings were significantly faster than those to the unisensory conditions, regardless of spatial alignment, pointing to engagement of integrative multisensory processing in all conditions. In support, electrophysiological results revealed significant differences between multisensory simultaneous VS and summed V + S responses, regardless of the spatial alignment of the constituent inputs. Nonetheless, multisensory effects were earlier in the aligned conditions, and were found to be particularly robust in the case of right-sided inputs (beginning at just 55 ms). In contrast to previous work on audio-visual and audio-somatosensory inputs, the current work suggests a degree of spatial specificity to the earliest detectable multisensory integrative effects in response to VS pairings.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jeannette R Mahoney
- The Cognitive Neurophysiology Laboratory, The Nathan S. Kline Institute for Psychiatric Research, Orangeburg NY, USA ; Division of Cognitive and Motor Aging, Department of Neurology, Albert Einstein College of Medicine, New York NY, USA
| | - Sophie Molholm
- The Cognitive Neurophysiology Laboratory, The Nathan S. Kline Institute for Psychiatric Research, Orangeburg NY, USA ; The Sheryl and Daniel R. Tishman Cognitive Neurophysiology Laboratory, Children's Evaluation and Rehabilitation Center, Department of Pediatrics, Albert Einstein College of Medicine and Montefiore Medical Center, New York NY, USA ; The Dominick P. Purpura Department of Neuroscience, Rose F. Kennedy Center, Albert Einstein College of Medicine, New York NY, USA
| | - John S Butler
- The Sheryl and Daniel R. Tishman Cognitive Neurophysiology Laboratory, Children's Evaluation and Rehabilitation Center, Department of Pediatrics, Albert Einstein College of Medicine and Montefiore Medical Center, New York NY, USA
| | - Pejman Sehatpour
- The Cognitive Neurophysiology Laboratory, The Nathan S. Kline Institute for Psychiatric Research, Orangeburg NY, USA
| | - Manuel Gomez-Ramirez
- The Cognitive Neurophysiology Laboratory, The Nathan S. Kline Institute for Psychiatric Research, Orangeburg NY, USA
| | - Walter Ritter
- The Cognitive Neurophysiology Laboratory, The Nathan S. Kline Institute for Psychiatric Research, Orangeburg NY, USA ; The Sheryl and Daniel R. Tishman Cognitive Neurophysiology Laboratory, Children's Evaluation and Rehabilitation Center, Department of Pediatrics, Albert Einstein College of Medicine and Montefiore Medical Center, New York NY, USA
| | - John J Foxe
- The Cognitive Neurophysiology Laboratory, The Nathan S. Kline Institute for Psychiatric Research, Orangeburg NY, USA ; The Sheryl and Daniel R. Tishman Cognitive Neurophysiology Laboratory, Children's Evaluation and Rehabilitation Center, Department of Pediatrics, Albert Einstein College of Medicine and Montefiore Medical Center, New York NY, USA ; The Dominick P. Purpura Department of Neuroscience, Rose F. Kennedy Center, Albert Einstein College of Medicine, New York NY, USA
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
Azzi JCB, Gattass R, Lima B, Soares JGM, Fiorani M. Precise visuotopic organization of the blind spot representation in primate V1. J Neurophysiol 2015; 113:3588-99. [PMID: 25761953 PMCID: PMC4461883 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00418.2014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/06/2014] [Accepted: 03/06/2015] [Indexed: 11/27/2022] Open
Abstract
The optic disk is a region of the retina consisting mainly of ganglion cell axons and blood vessels, which generates a visual scotoma known as the blind spot (BS). Information present in the surroundings of the BS can be used to complete the missing information. However, the neuronal mechanisms underlying these perceptual phenomena are poorly understood. We investigate the topography of the BS representation (BSR) in cortical area V1 of the capuchin monkey, using single and multiple electrodes. Receptive fields (RFs) of neurons inside the BSR were investigated using two distinct automatic bias-free mapping methods. The first method (local mapping) consisted of randomly flashing small white squares. For the second mapping method (global mapping), we used a single long bar that moved in one of eight directions. The latter stimulus was capable of eliciting neuronal activity inside the BSR, possibly attributable to long-range surround activity taking place outside the borders of the BSR. Importantly, we found that the neuronal activity inside the BSR is organized topographically in a manner similar to that found in other portions of V1. On average, the RFs inside the BS were larger than those outside. However, no differences in orientation or direction tuning were found between the two regions. We propose that area V1 exhibits a continuous functional topographic map, which is not interrupted in the BSR, as expected by the distribution of photoreceptors in the retina. Thus V1 topography is better described as “visuotopic” rather than as a discontinuous “retinotopic” map.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- João C B Azzi
- Institute of Biophysics Carlos Chagas Filho, Federal University of Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro, Brazil
| | - Ricardo Gattass
- Institute of Biophysics Carlos Chagas Filho, Federal University of Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro, Brazil
| | - Bruss Lima
- Institute of Biophysics Carlos Chagas Filho, Federal University of Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro, Brazil
| | - Juliana G M Soares
- Institute of Biophysics Carlos Chagas Filho, Federal University of Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro, Brazil
| | - Mario Fiorani
- Institute of Biophysics Carlos Chagas Filho, Federal University of Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro, Brazil
| |
Collapse
|
29
|
Spitschan M, Aguirre GK, Brainard DH. Selective stimulation of penumbral cones reveals perception in the shadow of retinal blood vessels. PLoS One 2015; 10:e0124328. [PMID: 25897842 PMCID: PMC4405364 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0124328] [Citation(s) in RCA: 38] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/15/2014] [Accepted: 03/03/2015] [Indexed: 12/13/2022] Open
Abstract
In 1819, Johann Purkinje described how a moving light source that displaces the shadow of the retinal blood vessels to adjacent cones can produce the entopic percept of a branching tree. Here, we describe a novel method for producing a similar percept. We used a device that mixes 56 narrowband primaries under computer control, in conjunction with the method of silent substitution, to present observers with a spectral modulation that selectively targeted penumbral cones in the shadow of the retinal blood vessels. Such a modulation elicits a clear Purkinje-tree percept. We show that the percept is specific to penumbral L and M cone stimulation and is not produced by selective penumbral S cone stimulation. The Purkinje-tree percept was strongest at 16 Hz and fell off at lower (8 Hz) and higher (32 Hz) temporal frequencies. Selective stimulation of open-field cones that are not in shadow, with penumbral cones silenced, also produced the percept, but it was not seen when penumbral and open-field cones were modulated together. This indicates the need for spatial contrast between penumbral and open-field cones to create the Purkinje-tree percept. Our observation provides a new means for studying the response of retinally stabilized images and demonstrates that penumbral cones can support spatial vision. Further, the result illustrates a way in which silent substitution techniques can fail to be silent. We show that inadvertent penumbral cone stimulation can accompany melanopsin-directed modulations that are designed only to silence open-field cones. This in turn can result in visual responses that might be mistaken as melanopsin-driven.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Manuel Spitschan
- Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, United States of America
| | - Geoffrey K. Aguirre
- Department of Neurology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, United States of America
- * E-mail: (DB); (GA)
| | - David H. Brainard
- Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, United States of America
- * E-mail: (DB); (GA)
| |
Collapse
|
30
|
Functional representation of vision within the mind: A visual consciousness model based in 3D default space. JOURNAL OF MEDICAL HYPOTHESES AND IDEAS 2015. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jmhi.2015.02.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
|
31
|
Philips RT, Chakravarthy VS. The mapping of eccentricity and meridional angle onto orthogonal axes in the primary visual cortex: an activity-dependent developmental model. Front Comput Neurosci 2015; 9:3. [PMID: 25688204 PMCID: PMC4310300 DOI: 10.3389/fncom.2015.00003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/24/2014] [Accepted: 01/07/2015] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Primate vision research has shown that in the retinotopic map of the primary visual cortex, eccentricity and meridional angle are mapped onto two orthogonal axes: whereas the eccentricity is mapped onto the nasotemporal axis, the meridional angle is mapped onto the dorsoventral axis. Theoretically such a map has been approximated by a complex log map. Neural models with correlational learning have explained the development of other visual maps like orientation maps and ocular-dominance maps. In this paper it is demonstrated that activity based mechanisms can drive a self-organizing map (SOM) into such a configuration that dilations and rotations of a particular image (in this case a rectangular bar) are mapped onto orthogonal axes. We further demonstrate using the Laterally Interconnected Synergetically Self Organizing Map (LISSOM) model, with an appropriate boundary and realistic initial conditions, that a retinotopic map which maps eccentricity and meridional angle to the horizontal and vertical axes respectively can be developed. This developed map bears a strong resemblance to the complex log map. We also simulated lesion studies which indicate that the lateral excitatory connections play a crucial role in development of the retinotopic map.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ryan T Philips
- Computational Neuroscience Laboratory, Department of Biotechnology, Indian Institute of Technology Madras Chennai, India
| | - V Srinivasa Chakravarthy
- Computational Neuroscience Laboratory, Department of Biotechnology, Indian Institute of Technology Madras Chennai, India
| |
Collapse
|
32
|
Sultan F. From cerebellar texture to movement optimization. BIOLOGICAL CYBERNETICS 2014; 108:677-688. [PMID: 25037239 DOI: 10.1007/s00422-014-0618-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/22/2013] [Accepted: 07/02/2014] [Indexed: 06/03/2023]
Abstract
The cerebellum is a major site for supervised procedural learning and appears to be crucial for optimizing sensorimotor performance. However, the site and origin of the supervising signal are still elusive. Furthermore, its relationship with the prominent neuronal circuitry remains puzzling. In this paper, I will review the relevant information and seek to synthesize a working hypothesis that explains the unique cerebellar structure. The aim of this review was to link the distinctive functions of the cerebellum, as derived from cerebellar lesion studies, with potential elementary computations, as observed by a bottom-up approach from the cerebellar microcircuitry. The parallel fiber geometry is ideal for performing millisecond computations that extract instructive signals. In this scenario, the higher time derivatives of kinematics such as acceleration and/or jerk that occur during motor performance are detected via a tidal wave mechanism and are used (with appropriate gating) as the instructive signal to guide motor smoothing. The advantage of such a mechanism is that movements are optimized by reducing "jerkiness" which, in turn, lowers their energy requirements.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Fahad Sultan
- Department of Cognitive Neurology, Hertie Institute for Clinical Brain Research, Otfried-Müller-Str. 21, 72076 , Tübingen, Germany,
| |
Collapse
|
33
|
Yu HH, Chaplin TA, Rosa MGP. Representation of central and peripheral vision in the primate cerebral cortex: Insights from studies of the marmoset brain. Neurosci Res 2014; 93:47-61. [PMID: 25242578 DOI: 10.1016/j.neures.2014.09.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/14/2014] [Revised: 08/27/2014] [Accepted: 08/28/2014] [Indexed: 01/06/2023]
Abstract
How the visual field is represented by neurons in the cerebral cortex is one of the most basic questions in visual neuroscience. However, research to date has focused heavily on the small part of the visual field within, and immediately surrounding the fovea. Studies on the cortical representation of the full visual field in the primate brain are still scarce. We have been investigating this issue with electrophysiological and anatomical methods, taking advantage of the small and lissencephalic marmoset brain, which allows easy access to the representation of the full visual field in many cortical areas. This review summarizes our main findings to date, and relates the results to a broader question: is the peripheral visual field processed in a similar manner to the central visual field, but with lower spatial acuity? Given the organization of the visual cortex, the issue can be addressed by asking: (1) Is visual information processed in the same way within a single cortical area? and (2) Are different cortical areas specialized for different parts of the visual field? The electrophysiological data from the primary visual cortex indicate that many aspects of spatiotemporal computation are remarkably similar across the visual field, although subtle variations are detectable. Our anatomical and electrophysiological studies of the extrastriate cortex, on the other hand, suggest that visual processing in the far peripheral visual field is likely to involve a distinct network of specialized cortical areas, located in the depths of the calcarine sulcus and interhemispheric fissure.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- H-H Yu
- Department of Physiology, Monash University, Clayton, VIC 3800, Australia; Australian Research Council Centre of Excellence for Integrative Brain Function, Monash University Node, Clayton, VIC 3800, Australia.
| | - T A Chaplin
- Department of Physiology, Monash University, Clayton, VIC 3800, Australia; Monash Vision Group, Monash University, Clayton, VIC 3800, Australia
| | - M G P Rosa
- Department of Physiology, Monash University, Clayton, VIC 3800, Australia; Australian Research Council Centre of Excellence for Integrative Brain Function, Monash University Node, Clayton, VIC 3800, Australia; Monash Vision Group, Monash University, Clayton, VIC 3800, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
34
|
Otaki S, Watanabe S, Fujita K. Differential motion processing between species facing Ternus-Pikler display: non-retinotopic humans versus retinotopic pigeons. Vision Res 2014; 103:32-40. [PMID: 25152320 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2014.08.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/01/2014] [Revised: 08/08/2014] [Accepted: 08/12/2014] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
Retinotopic encoding is preserved in primate visual cortex. However, several physiological and psychophysical studies have revealed that visual processes can be disengaged from retinotopic coordinates. We examined whether this non-retinotopic processing is common to humans and pigeons, two visually dominant vertebrate species with similar retinotopic organizations in their brains. We used a variant of Ternus-Pikler stimulus as a litmus test for non-retinotopic processing. Six humans and four pigeons were required to discriminate the rotational direction of a target disk placed among linearly arranged non-rotating disks. When all disks flickered in synchrony (a blank screen was inserted between the stimulus presentations) and moved in tandem back and forth, target localization was hampered in humans but not pigeons (Experiment 1). The duration of the blank screen (Experiment 2) and the connection between the disks (Experiment 3) did not affect the pigeons' performance. These results suggest that non-retinotopic processing in human vision is not a feature of pigeon vision, which is instead strictly retinotopic in case of motion. This may reflect the different mechanisms for stimulus selection in both species, in which local motion signals were pooled at later stages of visual processing in humans, but the signals were selected at early stages in pigeons.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sho Otaki
- Department of Psychology, Graduate School of Letters, Kyoto University, Yoshida-honmachi, Sakyo, Kyoto 606-8501, Japan; Japan Society for the Promotion of Science, Kojimachi Business Center Building, 5-3-1 Kojimachi, Chiyoda, Tokyo 102-0083, Japan.
| | - Sota Watanabe
- Japan Society for the Promotion of Science, Kojimachi Business Center Building, 5-3-1 Kojimachi, Chiyoda, Tokyo 102-0083, Japan; Course for School Teachers, Osaka Kyoiku University, 4-698-1 Asahigaoka, Kashiwara, Osaka 582-8582, Japan
| | - Kazuo Fujita
- Department of Psychology, Graduate School of Letters, Kyoto University, Yoshida-honmachi, Sakyo, Kyoto 606-8501, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
35
|
Speed of perceptual grouping in acquired brain injury. Exp Brain Res 2014; 232:2899-905. [PMID: 24820289 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-014-3970-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/16/2013] [Accepted: 04/19/2014] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
Abstract
Evidence exists that damage to white matter connections may contribute to reduced speed of information processing in traumatic brain injury and stroke. Damage to such axonal projections suggests a particular vulnerability to functions requiring integration across cortical sites. To test this prediction, measurements were made of perceptual grouping, which requires integration of stimulus components. A group of traumatic brain injury and cerebral vascular accident patients and a group of age-matched healthy control subjects viewed arrays of dots and indicated the pattern into which stimuli were perceptually grouped. Psychophysical measurements were made of perceptual grouping as well as processing speed. The patient group showed elevated grouping thresholds as well as extended processing time. In addition, most patients showed progressive slowing of processing speed across levels of difficulty, suggesting reduced resources to accommodate increased demands on grouping. These results support the prediction that brain injury results in a particular vulnerability to functions requiring integration of information across the cortex, which may result from dysfunction of long-range axonal connection.
Collapse
|
36
|
An illusion predicted by V1 population activity implicates cortical topography in shape perception. Nat Neurosci 2013; 16:1477-83. [PMID: 24036915 PMCID: PMC3889209 DOI: 10.1038/nn.3517] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/03/2013] [Accepted: 08/18/2013] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Mammalian primary visual cortex (V1) is topographically organized such that the pattern of neural activation in V1 reflects the location and spatial extent of visual elements in the retinal image, but it is unclear whether this organization contributes to visual perception. We combined computational modeling, voltage-sensitive dye imaging (VSDI) in behaving monkeys and behavioral measurements in humans to investigate whether the large-scale topography of V1 population responses influences shape judgments. Specifically, we used a computational model to design visual stimuli that had the same physical shape, but were predicted to elicit variable V1 response spread. We confirmed these predictions with VSDI. Finally, we designed a behavioral task in which human observers judged the shapes of these stimuli and found that their judgments were systematically distorted by the spread of V1 activity. This illusion suggests that the topographic pattern of neural population responses in visual cortex contributes to visual perception.
Collapse
|
37
|
Chaplin TA, Yu HH, Rosa MGP. Representation of the visual field in the primary visual area of the marmoset monkey: magnification factors, point-image size, and proportionality to retinal ganglion cell density. J Comp Neurol 2013; 521:1001-19. [PMID: 22911425 DOI: 10.1002/cne.23215] [Citation(s) in RCA: 40] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/29/2012] [Revised: 07/25/2012] [Accepted: 08/14/2012] [Indexed: 11/11/2022]
Abstract
The primary visual area (V1) forms a systematic map of the visual field, in which adjacent cell clusters represent adjacent points of visual space. A precise quantification of this map is key to understanding the anatomical relationships between neurons located in different stations of the visual pathway, as well as the neural bases of visual performance in different regions of the visual field. We used computational methods to quantify the visual topography of V1 in the marmoset (Callithrix jacchus), a small diurnal monkey. The receptive fields of neurons throughout V1 were mapped in two anesthetized animals using electrophysiological recordings. Following histological reconstruction, precise 3D reconstructions of the V1 surface and recording sites were generated. We found that the areal magnification factor (M(A) ) decreases with eccentricity following a function that has the same slope as that observed in larger diurnal primates, including macaque, squirrel, and capuchin monkeys, and humans. However, there was no systematic relationship between M(A) and polar angle. Despite individual variation in the shape of V1, the relationship between M(A) and eccentricity was preserved across cases. Comparison between V1 and the retinal ganglion cell density demonstrated preferential magnification of central space in the cortex. The size of the cortical compartment activated by a punctiform stimulus decreased from the foveal representation towards the peripheral representation. Nonetheless, the relationship between the receptive field sizes of V1 cells and the density of ganglion cells suggested that each V1 cell receives information from a similar number of retinal neurons, throughout the visual field.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tristan A Chaplin
- Department of Physiology, Monash University, Clayton, VIC 3800, Australia
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
38
|
Abstract
Neurons in the primary visual cortex of mammals form an orderly representation of the visual field. A recent study shows that the cortical folding pattern of the human brain accurately predicts not only the extent of this area, but also the location of cells that represent different points of visual space, leading to further considerations of the cortical mapping principles operating in related species.
Collapse
|
39
|
Frey HP, Molholm S, Lalor EC, Russo NN, Foxe JJ. Atypical cortical representation of peripheral visual space in children with an autism spectrum disorder. Eur J Neurosci 2013; 38:2125-38. [PMID: 23692590 DOI: 10.1111/ejn.12243] [Citation(s) in RCA: 34] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/02/2013] [Accepted: 04/03/2013] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
A key feature of early visual cortical regions is that they contain discretely organized retinotopic maps. Titration of these maps must occur through experience, and the fidelity of their spatial tuning will depend on the consistency and accuracy of the eye movement system. Anomalies in fixation patterns and the ballistics of eye movements are well documented in autism spectrum disorder (ASD), with off-center fixations a hallmark of the phenotype. We hypothesized that these atypicalities might affect the development of visuo-spatial maps and specifically that peripheral inputs might receive altered processing in ASD. Using high-density recordings of visual evoked potentials (VEPs) and a novel system-identification approach known as VESPA (visual evoked spread spectrum analysis), we assessed sensory responses to centrally and peripherally presented stimuli. Additionally, input luminance was varied to bias responsiveness to the magnocellular system, given previous suggestions of magnocellular-specific deficits in ASD. Participants were 22 ASD children (7-17 years of age) and 31 age- and performance-IQ-matched neurotypical controls. Both VEP and VESPA responses to central presentations were indistinguishable between groups. In contrast, peripheral presentations resulted in significantly greater early VEP and VESPA amplitudes in the ASD cohort. We found no evidence that anomalous enhancement was restricted to magnocellular-biased responses. The extent of peripheral response enhancement was related to the severity of stereotyped behaviors and restricted interests, cardinal symptoms of ASD. The current results point to differential visuo-spatial cortical mapping in ASD, shedding light on the consequences of peculiarities in gaze and stereotyped visual behaviors often reported by clinicians working with this population.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hans-Peter Frey
- Department of Pediatrics and Neuroscience, The Sheryl and Daniel R Tishman Cognitive Neurophysiology Laboratory, Children's Evaluation and Rehabilitation Center, Albert Einstein College of Medicine, Bronx, NY, USA
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
40
|
Rossi EA, Achtman RL, Guidon A, Williams DR, Roorda A, Bavelier D, Carroll J. Visual function and cortical organization in carriers of blue cone monochromacy. PLoS One 2013; 8:e57956. [PMID: 23469117 PMCID: PMC3585243 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0057956] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/07/2012] [Accepted: 01/29/2013] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Carriers of blue cone monochromacy have fewer cone photoreceptors than normal. Here we examine how this disruption at the level of the retina affects visual function and cortical organization in these individuals. Visual resolution and contrast sensitivity was measured at the preferred retinal locus of fixation and visual resolution was tested at two eccentric locations (2.5° and 8°) with spectacle correction only. Adaptive optics corrected resolution acuity and cone spacing were simultaneously measured at several locations within the central fovea with adaptive optics scanning laser ophthalmoscopy (AOSLO). Fixation stability was assessed by extracting eye motion data from AOSLO videos. Retinotopic mapping using fMRI was carried out to estimate the area of early cortical regions, including that of the foveal confluence. Without adaptive optics correction, BCM carriers appeared to have normal visual function, with normal contrast sensitivity and visual resolution, but with AO-correction, visual resolution was significantly worse than normal. This resolution deficit is not explained by cone loss alone and is suggestive of an associated loss of retinal ganglion cells. However, despite evidence suggesting a reduction in the number of retinal ganglion cells, retinotopic mapping showed no reduction in the cortical area of the foveal confluence. These results suggest that ganglion cell density may not govern the foveal overrepresentation in the cortex. We propose that it is not the number of afferents, but rather the content of the information relayed to the cortex from the retina across the visual field that governs cortical magnification, as under normal viewing conditions this information is similar in both BCM carriers and normal controls.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ethan A Rossi
- School of Optometry, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, California, United States of America.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
41
|
Wu J, Yan T, Zhang Z, Jin F, Guo Q. Retinotopic mapping of the peripheral visual field to human visual cortex by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Hum Brain Mapp 2012; 33:1727-40. [PMID: 22438122 DOI: 10.1002/hbm.21324] [Citation(s) in RCA: 40] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/05/2008] [Revised: 01/09/2011] [Accepted: 03/08/2011] [Indexed: 12/15/2022] Open
Abstract
Retinotopic mapping is a key property of organization in the human occipital cortex. The retinotopic organization of the central visual field of visual areas V1, V2, and V3 has been well established. We used fMRI to measure the retinotopic map of the peripheral visual field (eccentricity up to 60°). We estimated the sizes of the visual areas between 0° and 60° and obtained results consistent with anatomical studies. We also estimated the cortical distances and magnification factors for reconstruction of the retinotopic map using the peripheral wedge dipole model. By comparing the retinotopic map with the flattened surface, we analyzed the datasets used to reconstruct the map. We found that: (1) the percentage of the striate cortex devoted to peripheral vision in humans is significantly larger than that in the macaque, (2) the estimate of the scaling factor in linear magnification is larger than that found in previous studies focusing on central vision, and (3) the estimate of the peripheral factor in the dipolar model is too large to make the curve direction of the dipolar map in the periphery equivalent to that in the center. On the basis of our results, we revised the dipolar map to fit our conditions. The revised map in humans has a similar elliptical shape to that of macaques, and the central parts of the two species are the same. The different parts of the map are the peripheral regions, for which the peripheral wedge dipole model in humans is reversed compared to that of macaques.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jinglong Wu
- Biomedical Engineering Laboratory, The Graduate School of Natural Science and Technology, Okayama University, Okayama, Japan.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
42
|
Chen Y, Palmer CR, Seidemann E. The relationship between voltage-sensitive dye imaging signals and spiking activity of neural populations in primate V1. J Neurophysiol 2012; 107:3281-95. [PMID: 22422999 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00977.2011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 36] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Voltage-sensitive dye imaging (VSDI) is a powerful technique for measuring neural population responses from a large cortical region simultaneously with millisecond temporal resolution and columnar spatial resolution. However, the relationship between the average VSDI signal and the average spiking activity of neural populations is largely unknown. To better understand this relationship, we compared visual responses measured from V1 of behaving monkeys using VSDI and single-unit electrophysiology. We found large and systematic differences between position and orientation tuning properties obtained with these two techniques. We then determined that a simple computational model could explain these tuning differences. This model, together with our experimental results, allowed us to estimate the quantitative relationship between the average VSDI signal and local spiking activity. We found that this relationship is similar to the previously reported nonlinear relationship between average membrane potential and spike rate in single V1 neurons, suggesting that VSDI signals are dominated by subthreshold synaptic activity. This model, together with the VSDI measured maps for spatial position (retinotopy) and orientation, also allowed us to estimate the spatial integration area over which neural responses contribute to the VSDI signal at a given location. We found that the VSDI-integration area is consistent with a Gaussian envelope with a space constant of ∼230 μm. Finally, we show how this model and estimated parameters can be used to predict the pattern of population responses at the level of spiking activity from VSDI responses.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yuzhi Chen
- Department of Psychology and Center for Perceptual Systems, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX, USA
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
43
|
Abrams J, Nizam A, Carrasco M. Isoeccentric locations are not equivalent: the extent of the vertical meridian asymmetry. Vision Res 2012; 52:70-8. [PMID: 22086075 PMCID: PMC3345502 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2011.10.016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 104] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/22/2011] [Revised: 10/06/2011] [Accepted: 10/31/2011] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
Performance in visual tasks is limited by the low-level mechanisms that sample the visual field. It is well documented that contrast sensitivity and spatial resolution decrease as a function of eccentricity and that those factors impair performance in "higher level" tasks, such as visual search. Performance also varies consistently at isoeccentric locations in the visual field. Specifically, at a fixed eccentricity, performance is better along the horizontal meridian than the vertical meridian, and along the lower than the upper vertical meridian. Whether these asymmetries in visual performance fields are confined to the vertical meridian or extend across the whole upper versus lower visual hemifield has been a matter of debate. Here, we measure the extent of the upper versus lower asymmetry. Results reveal that this asymmetry is most pronounced at the vertical meridian and that it decreases gradually as the angular distance (polar angle) from the vertical meridian increases, with eccentricity held constant. Beyond 30° of polar angle from the vertical meridian, the upper to lower asymmetry is no longer reliable. Thus, the vertical meridian is uniquely asymmetric and uniquely insensitive. This pattern of results is consistent with early anatomical properties of the visual system and reflects constraints that are critical to our understanding of visual information processing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jared Abrams
- Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, NY, USA.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
44
|
Palmer CR, Chen Y, Seidemann E. Uniform spatial spread of population activity in primate parafoveal V1. J Neurophysiol 2011; 107:1857-67. [PMID: 22170967 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00117.2011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
What are the shape and size of the region in primate V1 that processes information from a single point in visual space? This region, a fundamental property termed cortical point image (CPI) (McIlwain 1986), represents the minimal population of V1 neurons that can be activated by a visual stimulus and therefore has important implications for population coding in the cortex. Previous indirect attempts to measure the CPI in macaque V1 using sparse microelectrode recordings resulted in conflicting findings. Whereas some early studies suggested that CPI size is constant throughout V1 (e.g., Hubel and Wiesel 1974), others have reported large changes in CPI size in parafoveal V1 (e.g., Van Essen et al. 1984). To resolve this controversy, we used voltage-sensitive dye imaging in V1 of fixating monkeys to directly measure the subthreshold CPI and several related properties across a range of parafoveal eccentricities. We found that despite large changes in other properties of the retinotopic map, the subthreshold CPI is approximately constant and extends over ∼6 × 8 mm(2). This large and invariant CPI ensures a uniform representation of each point in visual space, with a complete representation of all visual features in V1, as originally proposed by Hubel and Wiesel (1974). In addition, we found several novel and unexpected asymmetries and anisotropies in the shapes of the CPI and the population receptive field. These results expand our understanding of the representation of visual space in V1 and are likely to be relevant for the representations of stimuli in other sensory cortical areas.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chris R Palmer
- Dept. of Psychology and Center for Perceptual Systems, The Univ. of Texas at Austin, 1 Univ. Station A8000, Austin, TX 78712-0187, USA
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
45
|
Yan T, Jin F, He J, Wu J. Development of a wide-view visual presentation system for visual retinotopic mapping during functional MRI. J Magn Reson Imaging 2011; 33:441-7. [PMID: 21274987 DOI: 10.1002/jmri.22404] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022] Open
Abstract
PURPOSE To develop and validate the functionality of a novel wide-view visual presentation system with a horizontal and vertical eccentricity angle of 60° for retinotopic mapping by functional MRI (fMRI). MATERIALS AND METHODS The wide-view presentation system consisted of a 52-mm diameter optical fiber, an entrance apparatus and a presentation apparatus. The terminal edge of the optical fiber at the entrance is flat, while the terminal edge on the presentation apparatus is a sphere of 60 mm in diameter. The subjects wore contact lenses with +20, +22, or +25 magnification to focus on the stimulus, and the visual field eccentricity angle could reach 60°. The signal to noise ratio valuation experiment was performed to evaluate the clarity and quality of the MRI picture image. Checkerboard and random dot stimuli were used to prove that this system could be applied to retinotopic mapping by fMRI. RESULTS The results of the experiment demonstrated that the system is safe in the MRI environment with minimal distortion and can be used for visual retinotopic mapping studies. Wide-field mapping areas (V6, MST) were found in the human visual cortex. Compared with previous studies, the V1 and MT+ surface area approaches but does not fully cover the anatomical area. Nonetheless, the area achieved using the new system is larger than those achievable in previous fMRI studies. CONCLUSION We developed a versatile, low-cost system for presenting wide-view visual stimuli in the MRI environment. The fMRI retinotopic mapping results proved the viability of this system.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tianyi Yan
- Biomedical Engineering Laboratory, The Graduate School of Natural Science and Technology, Okayama University, Okayama, Japan
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
46
|
Bulatov A, Bertulis A, Bieliavicius A, Loginovic E. Effects of distractors on the perception of right angles. NEUROSCIENCE AND BEHAVIORAL PHYSIOLOGY 2010; 40:621-628. [PMID: 20532827 DOI: 10.1007/s11055-010-9315-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/12/2008] [Accepted: 06/09/2008] [Indexed: 05/29/2023]
Abstract
Psychophysiological experiments were performed in which imaginary right angles formed by three spots of light were supplemented by distracting line segments. The subjects' task was to restore the orthogonality disturbed by the distractors. Subjects' errors were assessed in relation to the distance between the basal spots and the distractors. The results led to the suggestion that there are specific lateral interaction processes involving local averaging of information on the positions of stimulus components. Quantitative evaluation of the parameters of this interaction was addressed using a simplified mathematical model constructed on the basis of the concept of displacements of the centers of gravity resulting from summation of the corresponding excitation patterns in the neural networks of the visual system. Good consistency between theoretical and experimental data was obtained.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- A Bulatov
- Institute of Biology, Kaunas, Lithuania.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
47
|
Kumano H, Uka T. The spatial profile of macaque MT neurons is consistent with Gaussian sampling of logarithmically coordinated visual representation. J Neurophysiol 2010; 104:61-75. [PMID: 20445031 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00040.2010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Neurons in extrastriate visual areas have large receptive fields (RFs) compared with those in primary visual cortex (V1), suggesting extensive spatial integration. To examine the spatial integration of neurons in area MT, we modeled the RFs of MT neurons based on a symmetrical (Gaussian) integration of V1 outputs and tested the model using single-unit recording in two fixating macaque monkeys. Because visual representation in V1 is logarithmically compressed along eccentricity, the resulting RF model is log-Gaussian along the radial axis in polar coordinates. To test the log-Gaussian model, the RF of each neuron was mapped on a 5 x 5 grid using a small patch of random dots drifting at the preferred velocity of the neuron. The majority of MT neurons had RFs with a steeper slope near the fovea and a shallower slope away from the fovea. Among various two-dimensional Gaussian models fitted to the RFs, the log-Gaussian model provided the best description. The fitted parameters revealed that the range of sampling by MT neurons has no systematic relationship with eccentricities, consistent with a recent study for V4 neurons. Our results suggest that MT neurons integrate inputs from constant-sized patches of V1 cortex.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hironori Kumano
- Department of Physiology 1, Faculty of Medicine, Juntendo University, Bunkyo, Tokyo, Japan
| | | |
Collapse
|
48
|
Alexander DM, Van Leeuwen C. Mapping of contextual modulation in the population response of primary visual cortex. Cogn Neurodyn 2010; 4:1-24. [PMID: 19898958 PMCID: PMC2837531 DOI: 10.1007/s11571-009-9098-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/22/2009] [Revised: 10/04/2009] [Accepted: 10/11/2009] [Indexed: 10/20/2022] Open
Abstract
We review the evidence of long-range contextual modulation in V1. Populations of neurons in V1 are activated by a wide variety of stimuli outside of their classical receptive fields (RF), well beyond their surround region. These effects generally involve extra-RF features with an orientation component. The population mapping of orientation preferences to the upper layers of V1 is well understood, as far as the classical RF properties are concerned, and involves organization into pinwheel-like structures. We introduce a novel hypothesis regarding the organization of V1's contextual response. We show that RF and extra-RF orientation preferences are mapped in related ways. Orientation pinwheels are the foci of both types of features. The mapping of contextual features onto the orientation pinwheel has a form that recapitulates the organization of the visual field: an iso-orientation patch within the pinwheel also responds to extra-RF stimuli of the same orientation. We hypothesize that the same form of mapping applies to other stimulus properties that are mapped out in V1, such as colour and contrast selectivity. A specific consequence is that fovea-like properties will be mapped in a systematic way to orientation pinwheels. We review the evidence that cytochrome oxidase blobs comprise the foci of this contextual remapping for colour and low contrasts. Neurodynamics and motion in the visual field are argued to play an important role in the shaping and maintenance of this type of mapping in V1.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- David M. Alexander
- Laboratory for Perceptual Dynamics, RIKEN Brain Science Institute, 2-1 Hirosawa, Wako-shi, Saitama 351-0198 Japan
| | - Cees Van Leeuwen
- Laboratory for Perceptual Dynamics, RIKEN Brain Science Institute, 2-1 Hirosawa, Wako-shi, Saitama 351-0198 Japan
| |
Collapse
|
49
|
Zirnsak M, Lappe M, Hamker FH. The spatial distribution of receptive field changes in a model of peri-saccadic perception: predictive remapping and shifts towards the saccade target. Vision Res 2010; 50:1328-37. [PMID: 20152853 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2010.02.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/08/2009] [Revised: 01/28/2010] [Accepted: 02/03/2010] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
Abstract
At the time of an impending saccade receptive fields (RFs) undergo dynamic changes, that is, their spatial profile is altered. This phenomenon has been observed in several monkey visual areas. Although their link to eye movements is obvious, neither the exact pattern nor their function is fully clear. Several RF shifts have been interpreted in terms of predictive remapping mediating visual stability. In particular, even prior to saccade onset some cells become responsive to stimuli presented in their future, post-saccadic RF. In visual area V4, however, the overall effect of RF dynamics consists of a shrinkage and shift of RFs towards the saccade target. These observations have been linked to a pre-saccadically enhanced processing of the future fixation. In order to better understand these seemingly different outcomes, we analyzed the RF shifts predicted by a recently proposed computational model of peri-saccadic perception (Hamker, Zirnsak, Calow, & Lappe, 2008). This model unifies peri-saccadic compression, pre-saccadic attention shifts, and peri-saccadic receptive field dynamics in a common framework of oculomotor reentry signals in extrastriate visual cortical maps. According to the simulations that we present in the current paper, a spatially selective oculomotor feedback signal leads to RF dynamics which are both consistent with the observations made in studies aiming to investigate predictive remapping and saccade target shifts. Thus, the seemingly distinct experimental observations could be grounded in the same neural mechanism leading to different RF dynamics dependent on the location of the RF in visual space.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marc Zirnsak
- Department of Psychology, Institute II, Westf. Wilhelms-University, Münster, Germany
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
50
|
Modeling magnification and anisotropy in the primate foveal confluence. PLoS Comput Biol 2010; 6:e1000651. [PMID: 20126528 PMCID: PMC2813258 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000651] [Citation(s) in RCA: 51] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/19/2009] [Accepted: 12/18/2009] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
A basic organizational principle of the primate visual system is that it maps the visual environment repeatedly and retinotopically onto cortex. Simple algebraic models can be used to describe the projection from visual space to cortical space not only for V1, but also for the complex of areas V1, V2 and V3. Typically a conformal (angle-preserving) projection ensuring local isotropy is regarded as ideal and primate visual cortex is often regarded as an approximation of this ideal. However, empirical data show systematic deviations from this ideal that are especially relevant in the foveal projection. The aims of this study were to map the nature of anisotropy predicted by existing models, to investigate the optimization targets faced by different types of retino-cortical maps, and finally to propose a novel map that better models empirical data than other candidates. The retino-cortical map can be optimized towards a space-conserving homogenous representation or a quasi-conformal mapping. The latter would require a significantly enlarged representation of specific parts of the cortical maps. In particular it would require significant enlargement of parafoveal V2 and V3 which is not supported by empirical data. Further, the recently published principal layout of the foveal singularity cannot be explained by existing models. We suggest a new model that accurately describes foveal data, minimizing cortical surface area in the periphery but suggesting that local isotropy dominates the most foveal part at the expense of additional cortical surface. The foveal confluence is an important example of the detailed trade-offs between the compromises required for the mapping of environmental space to a complex of neighboring cortical areas. Our models demonstrate that the organization follows clear morphogenetic principles that are essential for our understanding of foveal vision in daily life.
Collapse
|