51
|
Wilkerson AK, Uhde TW, Leslie K, Freeman WC, LaRowe SD, Schumann A, Cortese BM. Paradoxical olfactory function in combat veterans: The role of PTSD and odor factors. MILITARY PSYCHOLOGY 2018; 30:120-130. [PMID: 30220788 DOI: 10.1080/08995605.2018.1425063] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/13/2022]
Abstract
Stress- and trauma-related disorders, including posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), are characterized by an increased sensitivity to threat cues. Given that threat detection is a critical function of olfaction and that combat trauma is commonly associated with burning odors, we sought a better understanding of general olfactory function as well as response to specific trauma-related (i.e. burning) odors in combat-related PTSD. Trauma-exposed combat veterans with (N = 22) and without (N = 25) PTSD were assessed for general and specific odor sensitivities using a variety of tools. Both groups had similar general odor detection thresholds. However, the combat veterans with PTSD, compared to combat veterans with comparable trauma exposure, but without PTSD, had increased ratings of odor intensity, negative valence, and odor-triggered PTSD symptoms, along with a blunted heart rate in response to burning rubber odor. These findings are discussed within the context of healthy versus pathological changes in olfactory processing that occur over time after psychological trauma.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Thomas W Uhde
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, MUSC, Charleston, SC, US
| | - Kimberly Leslie
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, MUSC, Charleston, SC, US
| | | | - Steven D LaRowe
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, MUSC, Charleston, SC, US.,Ralph H. Johnson VAMC, Charleston, SC, US
| | - Aicko Schumann
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, MUSC, Charleston, SC, US
| | | |
Collapse
|
52
|
Influence of emotional valence on perceived psychological distance depends on emotional intensity. EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 2018. [DOI: 10.1002/ejsp.2361] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
|
53
|
Padmala S, Sambuco N, Codispoti M, Pessoa L. Attentional capture by simultaneous pleasant and unpleasant emotional distractors. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2018; 18:1189-1194. [PMID: 29494204 DOI: 10.1037/emo0000401] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Both high-arousal pleasant and unpleasant task-irrelevant stimuli capture attention and divert processing away from the main task leading to impaired behavioral performance in concurrent tasks. Most studies have separately investigated interference effects of unpleasant and pleasant stimuli on behavior. Thus, little is known about how pleasant and unpleasant task-irrelevant stimuli influence behavior simultaneously. In the present study, we investigated this question during a visual-letter search task. We tested two alternative hypotheses about the influence of simultaneous pleasant and unpleasant task-irrelevant stimuli on task performance. If behavior is purely determined by the intensity of the distractor stimuli (independent of valence), then we would expect the interference effect of simultaneous pleasant and unpleasant distractors to be similar to the influence of two pleasant or two unpleasant distractor stimuli. In contrast, because of opponent interactions between appetitive and aversive motivational systems, the interference effect of simultaneous pleasant and unpleasant stimuli might be weakened. We found that the interference effect of a compound pleasant-plus-unpleasant stimulus was greater than that of a neutral-plus-emotional stimulus and similar to that of two pleasant or two unpleasant stimuli. These results suggest that at the level of behavior, the influence of joint pleasant and unpleasant task-irrelevant stimuli during perception is mainly determined by the intensity of the stimuli, and independent of their valence. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2018 APA, all rights reserved).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | | | - Luiz Pessoa
- Department of Psychology, University of Maryland, College Park
| |
Collapse
|
54
|
Perry DC, Datta S, Sturm VE, Wood KA, Zakrzewski J, Seeley WW, Miller BL, Kramer JH, Rosen HJ. Reward deficits in behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia include insensitivity to negative stimuli. Brain 2017; 140:3346-3356. [PMID: 29053832 DOI: 10.1093/brain/awx259] [Citation(s) in RCA: 37] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/05/2017] [Accepted: 08/17/2017] [Indexed: 12/11/2022] Open
Abstract
During reward processing individuals weigh positive and negative features of a stimulus to determine whether they will pursue or avoid it. Though patients with behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia display changes in their pursuit of rewards, such as food, alcohol, money, and sex, the basis for these shifts is not clearly established. In particular, it is unknown whether patients' behaviour results from excessive focus on rewards, insensitivity to punishment, or to dysfunction in a particular stage of reward processing, such as anticipation, consumption, or action selection. Our goal was to determine the nature of the reward deficit in behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia and its underlying anatomy. We devised a series of tasks involving pleasant, unpleasant, and neutral olfactory stimuli, designed to separate distinct phases of reward processing. In a group of 25 patients with behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia and 21 control subjects, diagnosis by valence interactions revealed that patients with behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia rated unpleasant odours as less aversive than did controls and displayed lower skin conductance responses when anticipating an upcoming aversive odour. Subjective pleasantness ratings and skin conductance responses did not differ between behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia and controls for pleasant or neutral smells. In a task designed to measure the effort subjects would expend to smell or avoid smelling a stimulus, patients with behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia were less motivated, and therefore less successful than control subjects, at avoiding what they preferred not to smell, but had equivalent success at obtaining stimuli they found rewarding. Voxel-based morphometry of patients with behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia revealed that the inability to subjectively differentiate the valence of pleasant and unpleasant odours correlated with atrophy in right ventral mid-insula and right amygdala. High pleasantness ratings of unpleasant stimuli correlated with left dorsal anterior insula and frontal pole atrophy. These findings indicate that insensitivity to negative information may be a key component of the reward-seeking behaviours in behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia, and may relate to degeneration of structures that are involved in representing the emotional salience of sensory information.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- David C Perry
- Department of Neurology, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, USA
| | - Samir Datta
- Department of Neurology, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, USA
| | - Virginia E Sturm
- Department of Neurology, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, USA
| | - Kristie A Wood
- Department of Neurology, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, USA
| | - Jessica Zakrzewski
- Department of Neurology, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, USA
| | - William W Seeley
- Department of Neurology, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, USA
| | - Bruce L Miller
- Department of Neurology, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, USA
| | - Joel H Kramer
- Department of Neurology, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, USA
| | - Howard J Rosen
- Department of Neurology, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
55
|
Pellegrino R, Drechsler E, Hummel C, Warr J, Hummel T. Bimodal odor processing with a trigeminal component at sub- and suprathreshold levels. Neuroscience 2017; 363:43-49. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.07.030] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/14/2017] [Revised: 07/11/2017] [Accepted: 07/13/2017] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
|
56
|
Cecchetto C, Rumiati RI, Aiello M. Alexithymia and emotional reactions to odors. Sci Rep 2017; 7:14097. [PMID: 29074970 PMCID: PMC5658372 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-017-14404-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/07/2017] [Accepted: 10/02/2017] [Indexed: 12/29/2022] Open
Abstract
Alexithymia is a psychological construct characterized by deficits in processing emotional stimuli. However, little is known about the processing of odours in alexithymia, even though there is extensive proof that emotion and olfaction are closely linked. The present study is aimed at investigating how alexithymic individuals process emotions conveyed by odors. Emotional responses to unpleasant, neutral odors and clean air were collected through self-report ratings and psychophysiological measures in a sample of 62 healthy participants with high (HA), medium (MA) and low (LA) levels of alexithymia. Moreover, participants performed tests on odors identification and threshold and completed questionnaires assessing olfactory imagery and awareness. Two main results have been found: first, HA and MA groups showed altered physiological responses to odors, compared to LA, while no differences among the groups were observed in odor ratings; and second, affective and cognitive alexithymia components were differently associated with the performance on olfactory tests, skin conductance response to odors, reaction times in the rating task, and scores on olfactory questionnaires. We conclude that alexithymia is characterized by altered physiological reactions to olfactory stimuli; moreover, we stress the importance of evaluating the different alexithymia components since they affect emotional stimuli processing in different ways.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Cinzia Cecchetto
- SISSA - International School for Advanced Studies, Neuroscience Area, Via Bonomea, 265, 34136, Trieste, Italy.
- Institute of Psychology, University of Graz, Graz, Austria.
- BioTechMed, Graz, Austria.
| | - Raffaella Ida Rumiati
- SISSA - International School for Advanced Studies, Neuroscience Area, Via Bonomea, 265, 34136, Trieste, Italy
- ANVUR - Agenzia Nazionale della Valutazione del Sistema Universitario e della Ricerca, Via Ippolito Nievo, 35 - 00153, Roma, Italy
| | - Marilena Aiello
- SISSA - International School for Advanced Studies, Neuroscience Area, Via Bonomea, 265, 34136, Trieste, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
57
|
Mutic S, Brünner YF, Rodriguez-Raecke R, Wiesmann M, Freiherr J. Chemosensory danger detection in the human brain: Body odor communicating aggression modulates limbic system activation. Neuropsychologia 2017; 99:187-198. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.02.018] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/27/2016] [Revised: 02/20/2017] [Accepted: 02/22/2017] [Indexed: 01/23/2023]
|
58
|
Abstract
Olfactory and emotional higher processing pathways share common anatomical substrates. Hence, depression is often accompanied by alterations in olfactory function. These alterations are negative in nature and may involve decreased activation in olfactory eloquent structures or decreased volume in the olfactory bulb (OB). We suggest that olfaction and depression interact in two ways. First, olfactory function in depression is impaired as a consequence of reduced olfactory attention and diminished olfactory receptor turnover rates. Second, the OB may constitute a marker for enhanced vulnerability to depression. Closer analysis of these interactions may help to explain observed experimental data, as well as to elucidate new therapeutic strategies involving olfaction. Because of the difficulties to disentangle cause from consequence in the relationship between olfaction and depression, longitudinal and intervention studies are necessary to elucidate this further.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ilona Croy
- Department of Psychotherapy and Psychosomatic Medicine, Carl Gustav Carus Faculty of Medicine, Technische Universität Dresden, Fetscherstr. 74, 01307, Dresden, Germany
| | - Thomas Hummel
- Department of Otorhinolaryngology, Smell and Taste Clinic, Carl Gustav Carus Faculty of Medicine, Technische Universität Dresden, Fetscherstrasse 74, 01307, Dresden, Germany.
| |
Collapse
|
59
|
Abstract
The nature and neural implementation of emotions is the subject of vigorous debate. Here, we use Bayesian decision theory to address key complexities in this field and conceptualize emotions in terms of their relationship to survival-relevant behavioural choices. Decision theory indicates which behaviours are optimal in a given situation; however, the calculations required are radically intractable. We therefore conjecture that the brain uses a range of pre-programmed algorithms that provide approximate solutions. These solutions seem to produce specific behavioural manifestations of emotions and can also be associated with core affective dimensions. We identify principles according to which these algorithms are implemented in the brain and illustrate our approach by considering decision making in the face of proximal threat.
Collapse
|
60
|
Anzellotti S, Kliemann D, Jacoby N, Saxe R. Directed network discovery with dynamic network modelling. Neuropsychologia 2017; 99:1-11. [PMID: 28215697 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.02.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/09/2016] [Revised: 01/18/2017] [Accepted: 02/03/2017] [Indexed: 12/13/2022]
Abstract
Cognitive tasks recruit multiple brain regions. Understanding how these regions influence each other (the network structure) is an important step to characterize the neural basis of cognitive processes. Often, limited evidence is available to restrict the range of hypotheses a priori, and techniques that sift efficiently through a large number of possible network structures are needed (network discovery). This article introduces a novel modelling technique for network discovery (Dynamic Network Modelling or DNM) that builds on ideas from Granger Causality and Dynamic Causal Modelling introducing three key changes: (1) efficient network discovery is implemented with statistical tests on the consistency of model parameters across participants, (2) the tests take into account the magnitude and sign of each influence, and (3) variance explained in independent data is used as an absolute (rather than relative) measure of the quality of the network model. In this article, we outline the functioning of DNM, we validate DNM in simulated data for which the ground truth is known, and we report an example of its application to the investigation of influences between regions during emotion recognition, revealing top-down influences from brain regions encoding abstract representations of emotions (medial prefrontal cortex and superior temporal sulcus) onto regions engaged in the perceptual analysis of facial expressions (occipital face area and fusiform face area) when participants are asked to switch between reporting the emotional valence and the age of a face.
Collapse
|
61
|
Dalenberg JR, Weitkamp L, Renken RJ, Nanetti L, ter Horst GJ. Flavor pleasantness processing in the ventral emotion network. PLoS One 2017; 12:e0170310. [PMID: 28207751 PMCID: PMC5312947 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0170310] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/02/2016] [Accepted: 01/02/2017] [Indexed: 11/28/2022] Open
Abstract
The ventral emotion network-encompassing the amygdala, insula, ventral striatum, and ventral regions of the prefrontal cortex-has been associated with the identification of emotional significance of perceived external stimuli and the production of affective states. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies investigating chemosensory stimuli have associated parts of this network with pleasantness coding. In the current study, we independently analyzed two datasets in which we measured brain responses to flavor stimuli in young adult men. In the first dataset, participants evaluated eight regular off the shelf drinking products while participants evaluated six less familiar oral nutritional supplements (ONS) in the second dataset. Participants provided pleasantness ratings 20 seconds after tasting. Using independent component analysis (ICA) and mixed effect models, we identified one brain network in the regular products dataset that was associated with flavor pleasantness. This network was very similar to the ventral emotion network. Although we identified an identical network in the ONS dataset using ICA, we found no linear relation between activation of any network and pleasantness scores within this dataset. Our results indicate that flavor pleasantness is processed in a network encompassing amygdala, ventral prefrontal, insular, striatal and parahippocampal regions for familiar drinking products. For more unfamiliar ONS products the association is not obvious, which could be related to the unfamiliarity of these products.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jelle R. Dalenberg
- Top Institute Food and Nutrition, Wageningen, The Netherlands
- Neuroimaging Center Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands
| | - Liselore Weitkamp
- Top Institute Food and Nutrition, Wageningen, The Netherlands
- Neuroimaging Center Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands
| | - Remco J. Renken
- Top Institute Food and Nutrition, Wageningen, The Netherlands
- Neuroimaging Center Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands
| | - Luca Nanetti
- Neuroimaging Center Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands
| | - Gert J. ter Horst
- Top Institute Food and Nutrition, Wageningen, The Netherlands
- Neuroimaging Center Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
62
|
Cecchetto C, Rumiati RI, Parma V. Relative Contribution of Odour Intensity and Valence to Moral Decisions. Perception 2017; 46:447-474. [PMID: 28084905 DOI: 10.1177/0301006616689279] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/23/2023]
Abstract
Meta-analytic evidence showed that the chemical senses affect moral decisions. However, how odours impact on morality is currently unclear. Through a set of three studies, we assess whether and how odour intensity biases moral choices (Study 1a), its psychophysiological responses (Study 1b), as well as the behavioural and psychophysiological effects of odour valence on moral choices (Study 2). Study 1a suggests that the presence of an odour plays a role in shaping moral choice. Study 1b reveals that of two iso-pleasant versions of the same neutral odour, only the one presented sub-threshold (vs. supra-threshold) favours deontological moral choices, those based on the principle of not harming others even when such harm provides benefits. As expected, this odour intensity effect is tracked by skin conductance responses, whereas no difference in cardiac activity - proxy for the valence dimension - is revealed. Study 2 suggests that the same neutral odour presented sub-threshold increases deontological choices even when compared to iso-intense ambiguous odour, perceived as pleasant or unpleasant by half of the participants, respectively. Skin conductance responses, as expected, track odour pleasantness, but cardiac activity fails to do so. Results are discussed in the context of mechanisms alternative to disgust induction underlying moral choices.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Cinzia Cecchetto
- SISSA - International School for Advanced Studies, Trieste, Italy
| | - Raffaella Ida Rumiati
- SISSA - International School for Advanced Studies, Trieste, Italy; ANVUR - Agenzia Nazionale della Valutazione del sistema Universitario e della Ricerca, Roma, Italy
| | - Valentina Parma
- SISSA - International School for Advanced Studies, Trieste, Italy; Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden; Center for Autism Research, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
63
|
Leathers ML, Olson CR. In monkeys making value-based decisions, amygdala neurons are sensitive to cue value as distinct from cue salience. J Neurophysiol 2017; 117:1499-1511. [PMID: 28077664 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00564.2016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/13/2016] [Revised: 01/05/2017] [Accepted: 01/05/2017] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Neurons in the lateral intraparietal (LIP) area of macaque monkey parietal cortex respond to cues predicting rewards and penalties of variable size in a manner that depends on the motivational salience of the predicted outcome (strong for both large reward and large penalty) rather than on its value (positive for large reward and negative for large penalty). This finding suggests that LIP mediates the capture of attention by salient events and does not encode value in the service of value-based decision making. It leaves open the question whether neurons elsewhere in the brain encode value in the identical task. To resolve this issue, we recorded neuronal activity in the amygdala in the context of the task employed in the LIP study. We found that responses to reward-predicting cues were similar between areas, with the majority of reward-sensitive neurons responding more strongly to cues that predicted large reward than to those that predicted small reward. Responses to penalty-predicting cues were, however, markedly different. In the amygdala, unlike LIP, few neurons were sensitive to penalty size, few penalty-sensitive neurons favored large over small penalty, and the dependence of firing rate on penalty size was negatively correlated with its dependence on reward size. These results indicate that amygdala neurons encoded cue value under circumstances in which LIP neurons exhibited sensitivity to motivational salience. However, the representation of negative value, as reflected in sensitivity to penalty size, was weaker than the representation of positive value, as reflected in sensitivity to reward size.NEW & NOTEWORTHY This is the first study to characterize amygdala neuronal responses to cues predicting rewards and penalties of variable size in monkeys making value-based choices. Manipulating reward and penalty size allowed distinguishing activity dependent on motivational salience from activity dependent on value. This approach revealed in a previous study that neurons of the lateral intraparietal (LIP) area encode motivational salience. Here, it reveals that amygdala neurons encode value. The results establish a sharp functional distinction between the two areas.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marvin L Leathers
- Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania; and .,Department of Neuroscience, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania
| | - Carl R Olson
- Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania; and.,Department of Neuroscience, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania
| |
Collapse
|
64
|
Lin H, Mueller-Bardorff M, Mothes-Lasch M, Buff C, Brinkmann L, Miltner WHR, Straube T. Effects of Intensity of Facial Expressions on Amygdalar Activation Independently of Valence. Front Hum Neurosci 2016; 10:646. [PMID: 28066216 PMCID: PMC5167742 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00646] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/02/2016] [Accepted: 12/05/2016] [Indexed: 11/25/2022] Open
Abstract
For several stimulus categories (e.g., pictures, odors, and words), the arousal of both negative and positive stimuli has been shown to modulate amygdalar activation. In contrast, previous studies did not observe similar amygdalar effects in response to negative and positive facial expressions with varying intensity of facial expressions. Reasons for this discrepancy may be related to analytical strategies, experimental design and stimuli. Therefore, the present study aimed at re-investigating whether the intensity of facial expressions modulates amygdalar activation by circumventing limitations of previous research. Event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging was used to assess brain activation while participants observed a static neutral expression and positive (happy) and negative (angry) expressions of either high or low intensity from an ecologically valid, novel stimulus set. The ratings of arousal and intensity were highly correlated. We found that amygdalar activation followed a u-shaped activation pattern with highest activation to high intense facial expressions as compared to low intensity facial expressions and to the neutral expression irrespective of valence, suggesting a critical role of the amygdala in valence-independent arousal processing of facial expressions. Additionally, consistent with previous studies, intensity effects were also found in visual areas and generally increased activation to angry versus happy faces were found in visual cortex and insula, indicating enhanced visual representations of high arousing facial expressions and increased visual and somatosensory representations of threat.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Huiyan Lin
- Institute of Applied Psychology, Guangdong University of FinanceGuangzhou, China; Laboratory for Behavioral and Regional Finance, Guangdong University of Finance, GuangzhouGuangdong, China; Institute of Medical Psychology and Systems Neuroscience, University of MuensterMuenster, Germany
| | - Miriam Mueller-Bardorff
- Institute of Medical Psychology and Systems Neuroscience, University of Muenster Muenster, Germany
| | - Martin Mothes-Lasch
- Institute of Medical Psychology and Systems Neuroscience, University of Muenster Muenster, Germany
| | - Christine Buff
- Institute of Medical Psychology and Systems Neuroscience, University of Muenster Muenster, Germany
| | - Leonie Brinkmann
- Institute of Medical Psychology and Systems Neuroscience, University of Muenster Muenster, Germany
| | - Wolfgang H R Miltner
- Department of Biological and Clinical Psychology, Friedrich-Schiller-University Jena Jena, Germany
| | - Thomas Straube
- Institute of Medical Psychology and Systems Neuroscience, University of Muenster Muenster, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
65
|
Odor-induced recall of emotional memories in PTSD–Review and new paradigm for research. Exp Neurol 2016; 284:168-180. [DOI: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2016.08.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 36] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/28/2016] [Revised: 08/01/2016] [Accepted: 08/04/2016] [Indexed: 01/09/2023]
|
66
|
Olfaction as a marker for depression. J Neurol 2016; 264:631-638. [DOI: 10.1007/s00415-016-8227-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 89] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/14/2016] [Revised: 07/01/2016] [Accepted: 07/02/2016] [Indexed: 12/29/2022]
|
67
|
Affiliation(s)
- Hailan Hu
- Interdisciplinary Institute of Neuroscience and Technology, Qiushi Academy for Advanced Studies, Zhejiang University, Hangzhou 310012, People's Republic of China;
- Center for Neuroscience, School of Medicine, Zhejiang University, Hangzhou 310058, People's Republic of China
| |
Collapse
|
68
|
Croy I, Drechsler E, Hamilton P, Hummel T, Olausson H. Olfactory modulation of affective touch processing — A neurophysiological investigation. Neuroimage 2016; 135:135-41. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.04.046] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/21/2016] [Revised: 03/22/2016] [Accepted: 04/19/2016] [Indexed: 10/21/2022] Open
|
69
|
Munyan BG, Neer SM, Beidel DC, Jentsch F. Olfactory Stimuli Increase Presence in Virtual Environments. PLoS One 2016; 11:e0157568. [PMID: 27310253 PMCID: PMC4910977 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0157568] [Citation(s) in RCA: 35] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/13/2016] [Accepted: 06/01/2016] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND Exposure therapy (EXP) is the most empirically supported treatment for anxiety and trauma-related disorders. EXP consists of repeated exposure to a feared object or situation in the absence of the feared outcome in order to extinguish associated anxiety. Key to the success of EXP is the need to present the feared object/event/situation in as much detail and utilizing as many sensory modalities as possible, in order to augment the sense of presence during exposure sessions. Various technologies used to augment the exposure therapy process by presenting multi-sensory cues (e.g., sights, smells, sounds). Studies have shown that scents can elicit emotionally charged memories, but no prior research has examined the effect of olfactory stimuli upon the patient's sense of presence during simulated exposure tasks. METHODS 60 adult participants navigated a mildly anxiety-producing virtual environment (VE) similar to those used in the treatment of anxiety disorders. Participants had no autobiographical memory associated with the VE. State anxiety, Presence ratings, and electrodermal (EDA) activity were collected throughout the experiment. RESULTS Utilizing a Bonferroni corrected Linear Mixed Model, our results showed statistically significant relationships between olfactory stimuli and presence as assessed by both the Igroup Presence Questionnaire (IPQ: R2 = 0.85, (F(3,52) = 6.625, p = 0.0007) and a single item visual-analogue scale (R2 = 0.85, (F(3,52) = 5.382, p = 0.0027). State anxiety was unaffected by the presence or absence of olfactory cues. EDA was unaffected by experimental condition. CONCLUSION Olfactory stimuli increase presence in virtual environments that approximate those typical in exposure therapy, but did not increase EDA. Additionally, once administered, the removal of scents resulted in a disproportionate decrease in presence. Implications for incorporating the use of scents to increase the efficacy of exposure therapy is discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Benson G. Munyan
- Department of Psychology, University of Central Florida, Orlando, Florida, United States of America
- * E-mail:
| | - Sandra M. Neer
- Department of Psychology, University of Central Florida, Orlando, Florida, United States of America
| | - Deborah C. Beidel
- Department of Psychology, University of Central Florida, Orlando, Florida, United States of America
| | - Florian Jentsch
- Department of Psychology, University of Central Florida, Orlando, Florida, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
70
|
Differences in the central-nervous processing of olfactory stimuli according to their hedonic and arousal characteristics. Neuroscience 2016; 324:62-8. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.03.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/03/2015] [Revised: 03/02/2016] [Accepted: 03/03/2016] [Indexed: 12/18/2022]
|
71
|
Lapate RC, Rokers B, Tromp DPM, Orfali NS, Oler JA, Doran ST, Adluru N, Alexander AL, Davidson RJ. Awareness of Emotional Stimuli Determines the Behavioral Consequences of Amygdala Activation and Amygdala-Prefrontal Connectivity. Sci Rep 2016; 6:25826. [PMID: 27181344 PMCID: PMC4867584 DOI: 10.1038/srep25826] [Citation(s) in RCA: 34] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/12/2016] [Accepted: 04/21/2016] [Indexed: 12/23/2022] Open
Abstract
Conscious awareness of negative cues is thought to enhance emotion-regulatory capacity, but the neural mechanisms underlying this effect are unknown. Using continuous flash suppression (CFS) in the MRI scanner, we manipulated visual awareness of fearful faces during an affect misattribution paradigm, in which preferences for neutral objects can be biased by the valence of a previously presented stimulus. The amygdala responded to fearful faces independently of awareness. However, when awareness of fearful faces was prevented, individuals with greater amygdala responses displayed a negative bias toward unrelated novel neutral faces. In contrast, during the aware condition, inverse coupling between the amygdala and prefrontal cortex reduced this bias, particularly among individuals with higher structural connectivity in the major white matter pathway connecting the prefrontal cortex and amygdala. Collectively, these results indicate that awareness promotes the function of a critical emotion-regulatory network targeting the amygdala, providing a mechanistic account for the role of awareness in emotion regulation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- R C Lapate
- Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin - Madison, 1500 Highland Avenue, Madison, Wisconsin 53705, USA.,Waisman Laboratory for Brain Imaging &Behavior, University of Wisconsin - Madison, 1500 Highland Avenue, Madison, Wisconsin 53705, USA.,Center for Healthy Minds, University of Wisconsin - Madison, 1500 Highland Avenue, Madison, Wisconsin 53705, USA
| | - B Rokers
- Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin - Madison, 1500 Highland Avenue, Madison, Wisconsin 53705, USA.,Waisman Laboratory for Brain Imaging &Behavior, University of Wisconsin - Madison, 1500 Highland Avenue, Madison, Wisconsin 53705, USA.,Department of Psychology, Utrecht University, Heidelberglaan 2, 3584 CS, Utrecht, the Netherlands
| | - D P M Tromp
- Waisman Laboratory for Brain Imaging &Behavior, University of Wisconsin - Madison, 1500 Highland Avenue, Madison, Wisconsin 53705, USA.,Department of Psychiatry, University of Wisconsin - Madison, 6001 Research Park Boulevard, Madison, Wisconsin 53719, USA
| | - N S Orfali
- Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin - Madison, 1500 Highland Avenue, Madison, Wisconsin 53705, USA.,Waisman Laboratory for Brain Imaging &Behavior, University of Wisconsin - Madison, 1500 Highland Avenue, Madison, Wisconsin 53705, USA.,Center for Healthy Minds, University of Wisconsin - Madison, 1500 Highland Avenue, Madison, Wisconsin 53705, USA
| | - J A Oler
- Department of Psychiatry, University of Wisconsin - Madison, 6001 Research Park Boulevard, Madison, Wisconsin 53719, USA
| | - S T Doran
- Waisman Laboratory for Brain Imaging &Behavior, University of Wisconsin - Madison, 1500 Highland Avenue, Madison, Wisconsin 53705, USA
| | - N Adluru
- Waisman Laboratory for Brain Imaging &Behavior, University of Wisconsin - Madison, 1500 Highland Avenue, Madison, Wisconsin 53705, USA.,Center for Healthy Minds, University of Wisconsin - Madison, 1500 Highland Avenue, Madison, Wisconsin 53705, USA
| | - A L Alexander
- Waisman Laboratory for Brain Imaging &Behavior, University of Wisconsin - Madison, 1500 Highland Avenue, Madison, Wisconsin 53705, USA.,Department of Psychiatry, University of Wisconsin - Madison, 6001 Research Park Boulevard, Madison, Wisconsin 53719, USA.,Department of Medical Physics, University of Wisconsin - Madison, 1500 Highland Avenue, Madison, Wisconsin 53705, USA
| | - R J Davidson
- Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin - Madison, 1500 Highland Avenue, Madison, Wisconsin 53705, USA.,Waisman Laboratory for Brain Imaging &Behavior, University of Wisconsin - Madison, 1500 Highland Avenue, Madison, Wisconsin 53705, USA.,Center for Healthy Minds, University of Wisconsin - Madison, 1500 Highland Avenue, Madison, Wisconsin 53705, USA.,Department of Psychiatry, University of Wisconsin - Madison, 6001 Research Park Boulevard, Madison, Wisconsin 53719, USA
| |
Collapse
|
72
|
Fournel A, Ferdenzi C, Sezille C, Rouby C, Bensafi M. Multidimensional representation of odors in the human olfactory cortex. Hum Brain Mapp 2016; 37:2161-72. [PMID: 26991044 DOI: 10.1002/hbm.23164] [Citation(s) in RCA: 37] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/02/2015] [Revised: 02/09/2016] [Accepted: 02/18/2016] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
Abstract
What is known as an odor object is an integrated representation constructed from physical features, and perceptual attributes mainly mediated by the olfactory and trigeminal systems. The aim of the present study was to comprehend how this multidimensional representation is organized, by deciphering how similarities in the physical, olfactory and trigeminal perceptual spaces of odors are represented in the human brain. To achieve this aim, we combined psychophysics, functional MRI and multivariate representational similarity analysis. Participants were asked to smell odors diffused by an fMRI-compatible olfactometer and to rate each smell along olfactory dimensions (pleasantness, intensity, familiarity and edibility) and trigeminal dimensions (irritation, coolness, warmth and pain). An event-related design was implemented, presenting different odorants. Results revealed that (i) pairwise odorant similarities in anterior piriform cortex (PC) activity correlated with pairwise odorant similarities in chemical properties (P < 0.005), (ii) similarities in posterior PC activity correlated with similarities in olfactory perceptual properties (P <0.01), and (iii) similarities in amygdala activity correlated with similarities in trigeminal perceptual properties (P < 0.01). These findings provide new evidence that extraction of physical, olfactory and trigeminal features is based on specific fine processing of similarities between odorous stimuli in a distributed manner in the olfactory system. Hum Brain Mapp 37:2161-2172, 2016. © 2016 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- A Fournel
- Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, CNRS UMR5292, INSERM U1028, University Lyon, F-69000, France
| | - C Ferdenzi
- Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, CNRS UMR5292, INSERM U1028, University Lyon, F-69000, France
| | - C Sezille
- Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, CNRS UMR5292, INSERM U1028, University Lyon, F-69000, France
| | - C Rouby
- Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, CNRS UMR5292, INSERM U1028, University Lyon, F-69000, France
| | - M Bensafi
- Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, CNRS UMR5292, INSERM U1028, University Lyon, F-69000, France
| |
Collapse
|
73
|
Gschwind M, Picard F. Ecstatic Epileptic Seizures: A Glimpse into the Multiple Roles of the Insula. Front Behav Neurosci 2016; 10:21. [PMID: 26924970 PMCID: PMC4756129 DOI: 10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00021] [Citation(s) in RCA: 31] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/06/2015] [Accepted: 02/02/2016] [Indexed: 01/18/2023] Open
Abstract
Ecstatic epileptic seizures are a rare but compelling epileptic entity. During the first seconds of these seizures, ecstatic auras provoke feelings of well-being, intense serenity, bliss, and "enhanced self-awareness." They are associated with the impression of time dilation, and can be described as a mystic experience by some patients. The functional neuroanatomy of ecstatic seizures is still debated. During recent years several patients presenting with ecstatic auras have been reported by others and us (in total n = 52); a few of them in the setting of presurgical evaluation including electrical brain stimulation. According to the recently recognized functions of the insula, and the results of nuclear brain imaging and electrical stimulation, the ecstatic symptoms in these patients seem to localize to a functional network centered around the anterior insular cortex, where we thus propose to locate this rare ictal phenomenon. Here we summarize the role of the multiple sensory, autonomic, affective, and cognitive functions of the insular cortex, which are integrated into the creation of self-awareness, and we suggest how this system may become dysfunctional on several levels during ecstatic aura.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Markus Gschwind
- Department of Neurology, University Hospital and Medical School of GenevaGeneva, Switzerland
- Functional Brain Mapping Laboratory, Department of Neuroscience, Biotech Campus, University of GenevaGeneva, Switzerland
| | - Fabienne Picard
- Department of Neurology, University Hospital and Medical School of GenevaGeneva, Switzerland
| |
Collapse
|
74
|
Wicker B, Monfardini E, Royet JP. Olfactory processing in adults with autism spectrum disorders. Mol Autism 2016; 7:4. [PMID: 26788281 PMCID: PMC4717566 DOI: 10.1186/s13229-016-0070-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 33] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/26/2015] [Accepted: 01/12/2016] [Indexed: 12/04/2022] Open
Abstract
Background As evidenced in the DSM-V, autism spectrum disorders (ASD) are often characterized by atypical sensory behavior (hyper- or hypo-reactivity), but very few studies have evaluated olfactory abilities in individuals with ASD. Methods Fifteen adults with ASD and 15 typically developing participants underwent olfactory tests focused on superficial (suprathreshold detection task), perceptual (intensity and pleasantness judgment tasks), and semantic (identification task) odor processing. Results In terms of suprathreshold detection performance, decreased discrimination scores and increased bias scores were observed in the ASD group. Furthermore, the participants with ASD exhibited increased intensity judgment scores and impaired scores for pleasantness judgments of unpleasant odorants. Decreased identification performance was also observed in the participants with ASD compared with the typically developing participants. This decrease was partly attributed to a higher number of near misses (a category close to veridical labels) among the participants with ASD than was observed among the typically developing participants. Conclusions The changes in discrimination and bias scores were the result of a high number of false alarms among the participants with ASD, which suggests the adoption of a liberal attitude in their responses. Atypical intensity and pleasantness ratings were associated with hyperresponsiveness and flattened emotional reactions, respectively, which are typical of participants with ASD. The high number of near misses as non-veridical labels suggested that categorical processing is functional in individuals with ASD and could be explained by attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. These findings are discussed in terms of dysfunction of the olfactory system.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Bruno Wicker
- Institut de Neurosciences de la Timone. CNRS & Université Aix-Marseille, Campus Santé Timone 27, Boulevard Jean Moulin, 13385 Marseille cedex 05, France
| | - Elisabetta Monfardini
- Integrative, Multisensory, Perception, Action and Cognition Team, Lyon, F-69000 France ; University Lyon 1, Lyon, F-69000 France ; Institut de Médecine Environnementale, Paris, France
| | - Jean-Pierre Royet
- INSERM U1028, CNRS UMR5292, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, Olfaction : From Coding to Memory Team, Lyon, F-69000 France ; University Lyon 1, Lyon, F-69000 France
| |
Collapse
|
75
|
Juran SA, Lundström JN, Geigant M, Kumlien E, Fredrikson M, Åhs F, Olsson MJ. Unilateral Resection of the Anterior Medial Temporal Lobe Impairs Odor Identification and Valence Perception. Front Psychol 2016; 6:2015. [PMID: 26779109 PMCID: PMC4705303 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.02015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/08/2015] [Accepted: 12/16/2015] [Indexed: 12/03/2022] Open
Abstract
The anterior medial temporal lobe (TL), including the amygdala, has been implicated in olfactory processing, e.g., coding for intensity and valence, and seems also involved in memory. With this background, the present study evaluated whether anterior medial TL-resections in TL epilepsy affected intensity and valence ratings, as well as free and cued identification of odors. These aspects of odor perception were assessed in 31 patients with unilateral anterior medial TL-resections (17 left, 14 right) and 16 healthy controls. Results suggest that the anterior medial TL is in particular necessary for free, but also cued, odor identification. TL resection was also found to impair odor valence, but not intensity ratings. Left resected patients rated nominally pleasant and unpleasant odors as more neutral suggesting a special role for the left anterior TL in coding for emotional saliency in response to odors.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Stephanie A Juran
- Division of Psychology, Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Karolinska InstitutetStockholm, Sweden; Unit of Work Environment Toxicology, Institute of Environmental Medicine, Karolinska InstitutetStockholm, Sweden
| | - Johan N Lundström
- Division of Psychology, Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Karolinska InstitutetStockholm, Sweden; Monell Chemical Senses CenterPhiladelphia, PA, USA; Department of Psychology, University of PennsylvaniaPhiladelphia, PA, USA
| | - Michael Geigant
- Mental Health Care, Stockholm County Council Stockholm, Sweden
| | - Eva Kumlien
- Neurology, Department of Neuroscience, Uppsala University Uppsala, Sweden
| | - Mats Fredrikson
- Division of Psychology, Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Karolinska InstitutetStockholm, Sweden; Department of Psychology, Uppsala UniversityUppsala, Sweden
| | - Fredrik Åhs
- Division of Psychology, Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Karolinska InstitutetStockholm, Sweden; Department of Psychology, Uppsala UniversityUppsala, Sweden
| | - Mats J Olsson
- Division of Psychology, Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Karolinska Institutet Stockholm, Sweden
| |
Collapse
|
76
|
Pacharra M, Schäper M, Kleinbeck S, Blaszkewicz M, Wolf OT, van Thriel C. Stress lowers the detection threshold for foul-smelling 2-mercaptoethanol. Stress 2016; 19:18-27. [PMID: 26553419 DOI: 10.3109/10253890.2015.1105212] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/01/2023] Open
Abstract
Previous studies have reported enhanced vigilance for threat-related information in response to acute stress. While it is known that acute stress modulates sensory systems in humans, its impact on olfaction and the olfactory detection of potential threats is less clear. Two psychophysical experiments examined, if acute stress lowers the detection threshold for foul-smelling 2-mercaptoethanol. Participants in Experiment 1 (N = 30) and Experiment 2 (N = 32) were randomly allocated to a control group or a stress group. Participants in the stress group underwent a purely psychosocial stressor (public mental arithmetic) in Experiment 1 and a stressor that combined a physically demanding task with social-evaluative threat in Experiment 2 (socially evaluated cold-pressor test). In both experiments, olfactory detection thresholds were repeatedly assessed by means of dynamic dilution olfactometry. Each threshold measurement consisted of three trials conducted using an ascending method of limits. Participants in the stress groups showed the expected changes in heart rate, salivary cortisol, and mood measures in response to stress. About 20 min after the stressor, participants in the stress groups could detect 2-mercaptoethanol at a lower concentration than participants in the corresponding control groups. Our results show that acute stress lowers the detection threshold for a malodor.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marlene Pacharra
- a Leibniz Research Centre for Working Environment and Human Factors at the TU Dortmund University , Dortmund , Germany and
| | - Michael Schäper
- a Leibniz Research Centre for Working Environment and Human Factors at the TU Dortmund University , Dortmund , Germany and
| | - Stefan Kleinbeck
- a Leibniz Research Centre for Working Environment and Human Factors at the TU Dortmund University , Dortmund , Germany and
| | - Meinolf Blaszkewicz
- a Leibniz Research Centre for Working Environment and Human Factors at the TU Dortmund University , Dortmund , Germany and
| | - Oliver T Wolf
- b Department of Cognitive Psychology , Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, Faculty of Psychology, Ruhr-University Bochum , Bochum , Germany
| | - Christoph van Thriel
- a Leibniz Research Centre for Working Environment and Human Factors at the TU Dortmund University , Dortmund , Germany and
| |
Collapse
|
77
|
Pichon AM, Coppin G, Cayeux I, Porcherot C, Sander D, Delplanque S. Sensitivity of Physiological Emotional Measures to Odors Depends on the Product and the Pleasantness Ranges Used. Front Psychol 2015; 6:1821. [PMID: 26648888 PMCID: PMC4664615 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01821] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/30/2015] [Accepted: 11/11/2015] [Indexed: 12/21/2022] Open
Abstract
Emotions are characterized by synchronized changes in several components of an organism. Among them, physiological variations provide energy support for the expression of approach/avoid action tendencies induced by relevant stimuli, while self-reported subjective pleasantness feelings integrate all other emotional components and are plastic. Consequently, emotional responses evoked by odors should be highly differentiated when they are linked to different functions of olfaction (e.g., avoiding environmental hazards). As this differentiation has been observed for contrasted odors (very pleasant or unpleasant), we questioned whether subjective and physiological emotional response indicators could still disentangle subtle affective variations when no clear functional distinction is made (mildly pleasant or unpleasant fragrances). Here, we compared the sensitivity of behavioral and physiological [respiration, skin conductance, facial electromyography (EMG), and heart rate] indicators in differentiating odor-elicited emotions in two situations: when a wide range of odor families was presented (e.g., fruity, animal), covering different functional meanings; or in response to a restricted range of products in one particular family (fragrances). Results show clear differences in physiological indicators to odors that display a wide range of reported pleasantness, but these differences almost entirely vanish when fragrances are used even though their subjective pleasantness still differed. Taken together, these results provide valuable information concerning the ability of classic verbal and psychophysiological measures to investigate subtle differences in emotional reactions to a restricted range of similar olfactory stimuli.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Aline M Pichon
- Swiss Center for Affective Sciences, University of Geneva Geneva, Switzerland
| | - Géraldine Coppin
- Swiss Center for Affective Sciences, University of Geneva Geneva, Switzerland ; Emotion, Elicitation and Expression Laboratory, Department of Psychology, Faculté de Psychologie et des Sciences de l'Éducation, University of Geneva Geneva, Switzerland
| | | | | | - David Sander
- Swiss Center for Affective Sciences, University of Geneva Geneva, Switzerland ; Emotion, Elicitation and Expression Laboratory, Department of Psychology, Faculté de Psychologie et des Sciences de l'Éducation, University of Geneva Geneva, Switzerland
| | - Sylvain Delplanque
- Swiss Center for Affective Sciences, University of Geneva Geneva, Switzerland ; Emotion, Elicitation and Expression Laboratory, Department of Psychology, Faculté de Psychologie et des Sciences de l'Éducation, University of Geneva Geneva, Switzerland
| |
Collapse
|
78
|
Jin J, Zelano C, Gottfried JA, Mohanty A. Human Amygdala Represents the Complete Spectrum of Subjective Valence. J Neurosci 2015; 35:15145-56. [PMID: 26558785 PMCID: PMC4642243 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.2450-15.2015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 72] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/26/2015] [Revised: 09/04/2015] [Accepted: 10/06/2015] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
Although the amygdala is a major locus for hedonic processing, how it encodes valence information is poorly understood. Given the hedonic potency of odor stimuli and the amygdala's anatomical proximity to the peripheral olfactory system, we combined high-resolution fMRI with pattern-based multivariate techniques to examine how valence information is encoded in the amygdala. Ten human subjects underwent fMRI scanning while smelling 9 odorants that systematically varied in perceived valence. Representational similarity analyses showed that amygdala codes the entire dimension of valence, ranging from pleasantness to unpleasantness. This unidimensional representation significantly correlated with self-reported valence ratings but not with intensity ratings. Furthermore, within-trial valence representations evolved over time, prioritizing earlier differentiation of unpleasant stimuli. Together, these findings underscore the idea that both spatial and temporal features uniquely encode pleasant and unpleasant odor valence in the amygdala. The availability of a unidimensional valence code in the amygdala, distributed in both space and time, would create greater flexibility in determining the pleasantness or unpleasantness of stimuli, providing a mechanism by which expectation, context, attention, and learning could influence affective boundaries for guiding behavior. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Our findings elucidate the mechanisms of affective processing in the amygdala by demonstrating that this brain region represents the entire valence dimension from pleasant to unpleasant. An important implication of this unidimensional valence code is that pleasant and unpleasant valence cannot coexist in the amygdale because overlap of fMRI ensemble patterns for these two valence extremes obscures their unique content. This functional architecture, whereby subjective valence maps onto a pattern continuum between pleasant and unpleasant poles, offers a robust mechanism by which context, expectation, and experience could alter the set-point for valence-based behavior. Finally, identification of spatial and temporal differentiation of valence in amygdala may shed new insights into individual differences in emotional responding, with potential relevance for affective disorders.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jingwen Jin
- Stony Brook University, Department of Psychology, Stony Brook, New York 11794-2500, and
| | - Christina Zelano
- Northwestern University Feinberg School of Medicine, Department of Neurology, Chicago, Illinois 60611
| | - Jay A Gottfried
- Northwestern University Feinberg School of Medicine, Department of Neurology, Chicago, Illinois 60611
| | - Aprajita Mohanty
- Stony Brook University, Department of Psychology, Stony Brook, New York 11794-2500, and
| |
Collapse
|
79
|
Human amygdala activations during nasal chemoreception. Neuropsychologia 2015; 78:171-94. [PMID: 26459095 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.10.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/09/2015] [Revised: 08/25/2015] [Accepted: 10/06/2015] [Indexed: 02/07/2023]
Abstract
This review serves as a comprehensive discussion of chemosensory stimulation of the amygdala in healthy humans. Following an introduction of the neuroanatomy of chemosensory processing in primary and secondary olfactory structures, functional resonance magnetic imaging and positron imaging tomography studies are systematically categorized based on valence of stimuli, stimulus concentration, and paradigm-dependent amygdala activation. The amygdala shows patterns of lateralization due to stimulus valence. Main findings include pleasant odors being associated with bilateral or left amygdala activation, and unpleasant odors being associated with activation of the right amygdala, suggesting a crucial role of the right amygdala in evolutionary preservation. Potentially threatening social stimuli, however, might be processed apart from the olfactory system and tend to activate the left amygdala. Amygdala response to chemosensory stimuli correlated with simultaneous activation in the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), piriform cortex (PC), and insula, suggesting a close-knit network of these areas during stimulus processing.
Collapse
|
80
|
Hoogeveen HR, Dalenberg JR, Renken RJ, ter Horst GJ, Lorist MM. Neural processing of basic tastes in healthy young and older adults — an fMRI study. Neuroimage 2015; 119:1-12. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.06.017] [Citation(s) in RCA: 32] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/17/2014] [Revised: 05/13/2015] [Accepted: 06/04/2015] [Indexed: 11/29/2022] Open
|
81
|
Lowen SB, Rohan ML, Gillis TE, Thompson BS, Wellons CBW, Andersen SL. Cocaine-conditioned odor cues without chronic exposure: Implications for the development of addiction vulnerability. NEUROIMAGE-CLINICAL 2015; 8:652-9. [PMID: 27006904 PMCID: PMC4788503 DOI: 10.1016/j.nicl.2015.06.012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/28/2014] [Revised: 05/28/2015] [Accepted: 06/19/2015] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
Adolescents are highly vulnerable to addiction and are four times more likely to become addicted at first exposure than at any other age. The dopamine D1 receptor, which is typically overexpressed in the normal adolescent prefrontal cortex, is involved in drug cue responses and is associated with relapse in animal models. In human drug addicts, imaging methods have detected increased activation in response to drug cues in reward- and habit-associated brain regions. These same methods can be applied more quantitatively to rodent models. Here, changes in neuronal activation in response to cocaine-conditioned cues were observed using functional magnetic resonance imaging in juvenile rats that were made to over-express either D1 receptors or green fluorescent protein by viral-mediated transduction. Reduced activation was observed in the amygdala and dopamine cell body regions in the low cue-preferring/control juvenile rats in response to cocaine cues. In contrast, increased activation was observed in the dorsal striatum, nucleus accumbens, prefrontal cortex, and dopamine cell bodies in high cue-preferring/D1 juveniles. The increase in cue salience that is mediated by increased D1 receptor density, rather than excessive cocaine experience, appears to underlie the transition from aversion to reward in cue-induced neural response and may form the basis for habit-forming vulnerability. Increased D1 receptors in prefrontal cortex increase BOLD in addiction regions. Cocaine-associated cues activated the amygdala when cocaine was preferred. Cocaine cues deactivated the amygdala in the absence of cocaine preference. Genetic engineering can be used to isolate functional responses in neural circuitry.
Collapse
Key Words
- BLA, basolateral amygdala
- BOLD, blood oxygenation level determination
- Cocaine
- Cue
- D1
- DSTR, dorsal striatum
- Development
- NAc, nucleus accumbens
- Odor
- PFC, prefrontal cortex
- ROI, region of interest
- SNc/r, substantia nigra pars compacta/reticulata
- Striatum
- VTA, ventral tegmental area
- fMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging
- pharmacoMRI, pharmacological magnetic resonance imaging
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Steven B Lowen
- Brain Imaging Center, McLean Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Belmont, MA 02478, USA; Department of Psychiatry, McLean Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Belmont, MA 02478, USA
| | - Michael L Rohan
- Brain Imaging Center, McLean Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Belmont, MA 02478, USA; Department of Psychiatry, McLean Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Belmont, MA 02478, USA
| | - Timothy E Gillis
- Brain Imaging Center, McLean Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Belmont, MA 02478, USA
| | - Britta S Thompson
- Laboratory for Developmental Neuropharmacology, McLean Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Belmont, MA 02478, USA
| | - Clara B W Wellons
- Brain Imaging Center, McLean Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Belmont, MA 02478, USA
| | - Susan L Andersen
- Laboratory for Developmental Neuropharmacology, McLean Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Belmont, MA 02478, USA; Department of Psychiatry, McLean Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Belmont, MA 02478, USA
| |
Collapse
|
82
|
Abstract
This essay assesses the two most significant changes in psychology over the past century: the attempt to localize psychological phenomena in restricted brain sites and the search for genetic contributions to behavior and psychopathology. Although there are advantages to these new developments, they are accompanied by some questionable assumptions. Because the investigators in these domains often relate variation in their biological measures to variation in personality traits evaluated with questionnaires, an analysis of the unique properties of the verbalreport questionnaires is presented. It is suggested that future research on human personality should try to combine semantic reports with behaviors and biological data in order to arrive at more fruitful constructs.
Collapse
|
83
|
Bonnet L, Comte A, Tatu L, Millot JL, Moulin T, Medeiros de Bustos E. The role of the amygdala in the perception of positive emotions: an "intensity detector". Front Behav Neurosci 2015. [PMID: 26217205 PMCID: PMC4493392 DOI: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00178] [Citation(s) in RCA: 54] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/04/2022] Open
Abstract
The specific role of the amygdala remains controversial even though the development of functional imaging techniques has established its implication in the emotional process. The aim of this study was to highlight the sensitivity of the amygdala to emotional intensity (arousal). We conducted an analysis of the modulation of amygdala activation according to variation in emotional intensity via an fMRI event-related protocol. Monitoring of electrodermal activity, a marker of psychophysiological emotional perception and a reflection of the activation of the autonomic nervous system, was carried out concurrently. Eighteen subjects (10 men; aged from 22 to 29 years) looked at emotionally positive photographs. We demonstrated that the left and right amygdalae were sensitive to changes in emotional intensity, activating more in response to stimuli with higher intensity. Furthermore, electrodermal responses were more frequent for the most intense stimuli, demonstrating the concomitant activation of the autonomic nervous system. These results highlight the sensitivity of the amygdala to the intensity of positively valenced visual stimuli, and in conjunction with results in the literature on negative emotions, reinforce the role of the amygdala in the perception of intensity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Louise Bonnet
- Department of Neurology, Besancon University Hospital Besancon, France ; Department of Research in Functional Imaging, CIC 808, Besancon University Hospital Besancon, France ; Laboratory of Integrative and Clinical Neuroscience, EA 481, SFR FED 4234 UFC-CHRU-EFS Besancon, France
| | - Alexandre Comte
- Department of Research in Functional Imaging, CIC 808, Besancon University Hospital Besancon, France ; Laboratory of Integrative and Clinical Neuroscience, EA 481, SFR FED 4234 UFC-CHRU-EFS Besancon, France
| | - Laurent Tatu
- Department of Neurology, Besancon University Hospital Besancon, France ; Department of Research in Functional Imaging, CIC 808, Besancon University Hospital Besancon, France ; Laboratory of Integrative and Clinical Neuroscience, EA 481, SFR FED 4234 UFC-CHRU-EFS Besancon, France
| | - Jean-Louis Millot
- Laboratory of Integrative and Clinical Neuroscience, EA 481, SFR FED 4234 UFC-CHRU-EFS Besancon, France
| | - Thierry Moulin
- Department of Neurology, Besancon University Hospital Besancon, France ; Department of Research in Functional Imaging, CIC 808, Besancon University Hospital Besancon, France ; Laboratory of Integrative and Clinical Neuroscience, EA 481, SFR FED 4234 UFC-CHRU-EFS Besancon, France
| | - Elisabeth Medeiros de Bustos
- Department of Neurology, Besancon University Hospital Besancon, France ; Department of Research in Functional Imaging, CIC 808, Besancon University Hospital Besancon, France ; Laboratory of Integrative and Clinical Neuroscience, EA 481, SFR FED 4234 UFC-CHRU-EFS Besancon, France
| |
Collapse
|
84
|
Abstract
It is commonly agreed upon a strong link between emotion and olfaction. Odor-evoked memories are experienced as more emotional compared with verbal, visual, and tactile stimuli. Moreover, the emotional quality of odor cues increases memory performance, but contrary to this, odors are poor retrieval cues for verbal labels. To examine the relation between the emotional quality of an odor and its likelihood of identification, this study evaluates how normative emotion ratings based on the 3-dimensional affective space model (that includes valence, arousal, and dominance), using the Self-Assessment Manikin by Bradley and Lang (Bradley MM, Lang PJ. 1994. Measuring emotion: the Self-Assessment Manikin and the Semantic Differential. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 25(1):49-59.) and the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. 1988. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol. 54(6):1063-1070.) predict the identification of odors in a multiple choice condition. The best fitting logistic regression model includes squared valence and dominance and thus, points to a significant role of specific emotional features of odors as a main clue for odor identification.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anne-Kathrin Bestgen
- Department of Psychology, Ruhr-University, Universitätsstraße 150, GAFO 04/423, 44801 Bochum, Germany and
| | - Patrick Schulze
- Department of Psychology, Ruhr-University, Universitätsstraße 150, GAFO 04/423, 44801 Bochum, Germany and Institute for Prevention and Occupational Medicine of the German Social Accident Insurance, Institute of the Ruhr-University Bochum (IPA), Bürkle-de-la-Camp-Platz 1, 44789 Bochum, Germany
| | - Lars Kuchinke
- Department of Psychology, Ruhr-University, Universitätsstraße 150, GAFO 04/423, 44801 Bochum, Germany and
| |
Collapse
|
85
|
Good KP, Sullivan RL. Olfactory function in psychotic disorders: Insights from neuroimaging studies. World J Psychiatry 2015; 5:210-221. [PMID: 26110122 PMCID: PMC4473492 DOI: 10.5498/wjp.v5.i2.210] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/02/2014] [Revised: 02/06/2015] [Accepted: 03/18/2015] [Indexed: 02/05/2023] Open
Abstract
Olfactory deficits on measures of identification, familiarity, and memory are consistently noted in patients with psychotic disorders relative to age-matched controls. Olfactory intensity ratings, however, appear to remain intact while the data on hedonics and detection threshold are inconsistent. Despite the behavioral abnormalities noted, no specific regional brain hypoactivity has been identified in psychosis patients, for any of the olfactory domains. However, an intriguing finding emerged from this review in that the amygdala and pirifom cortices were not noted to be abnormal in hedonic processing (nor was the amygdala identified abnormal in any study) in psychotic disorders. This finding is in contrast to the literature in healthy individuals, in that this brain region is strongly implicated in olfactory processing (particularly for unpleasant odorants). Secondary olfactory cortex (orbitofrontal cortices, thalamus, and insula) was abnormally activated in the studies examined, particularly for hedonic processing. Further research, using consistent methodology, is required for better understanding the neurobiology of olfactory deficits. The authors suggest taking age and sex differences into consideration and further contrasting olfactory subgroups (impaired vs intact) to better our understanding of the heterogeneity of psychotic disorders.
Collapse
|
86
|
Trost W, Frühholz S, Cochrane T, Cojan Y, Vuilleumier P. Temporal dynamics of musical emotions examined through intersubject synchrony of brain activity. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2015; 10:1705-21. [PMID: 25994970 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsv060] [Citation(s) in RCA: 59] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/05/2014] [Accepted: 05/08/2015] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
To study emotional reactions to music, it is important to consider the temporal dynamics of both affective responses and underlying brain activity. Here, we investigated emotions induced by music using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) with a data-driven approach based on intersubject correlations (ISC). This method allowed us to identify moments in the music that produced similar brain activity (i.e. synchrony) among listeners under relatively natural listening conditions. Continuous ratings of subjective pleasantness and arousal elicited by the music were also obtained for the music outside of the scanner. Our results reveal synchronous activations in left amygdala, left insula and right caudate nucleus that were associated with higher arousal, whereas positive valence ratings correlated with decreases in amygdala and caudate activity. Additional analyses showed that synchronous amygdala responses were driven by energy-related features in the music such as root mean square and dissonance, while synchrony in insula was additionally sensitive to acoustic event density. Intersubject synchrony also occurred in the left nucleus accumbens, a region critically implicated in reward processing. Our study demonstrates the feasibility and usefulness of an approach based on ISC to explore the temporal dynamics of music perception and emotion in naturalistic conditions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Wiebke Trost
- Swiss Center of Affective Sciences, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland, Neuroscience of Emotions and Affective Dynamics Laboratory, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland,
| | - Sascha Frühholz
- Swiss Center of Affective Sciences, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland, Neuroscience of Emotions and Affective Dynamics Laboratory, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
| | - Tom Cochrane
- Department of Philosophy, University of Sheffield, Sheffield, UK, and
| | - Yann Cojan
- Swiss Center of Affective Sciences, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland, Laboratory for Behavioral Neurology and Imaging of Cognition, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
| | - Patrik Vuilleumier
- Swiss Center of Affective Sciences, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland, Laboratory for Behavioral Neurology and Imaging of Cognition, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
| |
Collapse
|
87
|
Drueke B, Weichert L, Forkmann T, Mainz V, Gauggel S, Boecker M. Neural correlates of positive and negative performance feedback in younger and older adults. Behav Brain Funct 2015; 11:17. [PMID: 25889338 PMCID: PMC4417231 DOI: 10.1186/s12993-015-0062-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/24/2014] [Accepted: 03/26/2015] [Indexed: 11/10/2022] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND Recent studies with younger adults have shown that performance feedback can serve as a reward, and it elicits reward-related brain activations. This study investigated whether performance feedback is processed similarly in younger and older adults and whether there are differential aging effects for positive and negative performance feedback. METHODS We used event-related fMRI in a choice reaction-time task and provided performance feedback after each trial. RESULTS Although younger and older adults differed in task-related activation, they showed comparable reward-related activation. Positive performance feedback elicited the strongest striatal and amygdala activation, which was reflected behaviorally in slightly faster reaction times. CONCLUSIONS These results suggest that performance feedback serves as a reward in both younger and older adults.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Barbara Drueke
- Department of Medical Psychology and Medical Sociology, University Hospital of RWTH Aachen University, Pauwelsstr. 19, 52074, Aachen, Germany.
| | - Lydia Weichert
- Department of Medical Psychology and Medical Sociology, University Hospital of RWTH Aachen University, Pauwelsstr. 19, 52074, Aachen, Germany.
| | - Thomas Forkmann
- Department of Medical Psychology and Medical Sociology, University Hospital of RWTH Aachen University, Pauwelsstr. 19, 52074, Aachen, Germany.
| | - Verena Mainz
- Department of Medical Psychology and Medical Sociology, University Hospital of RWTH Aachen University, Pauwelsstr. 19, 52074, Aachen, Germany.
| | - Siegfried Gauggel
- Department of Medical Psychology and Medical Sociology, University Hospital of RWTH Aachen University, Pauwelsstr. 19, 52074, Aachen, Germany.
| | - Maren Boecker
- Department of Medical Psychology and Medical Sociology, University Hospital of RWTH Aachen University, Pauwelsstr. 19, 52074, Aachen, Germany.
| |
Collapse
|
88
|
Kragel PA, LaBar KS. Multivariate neural biomarkers of emotional states are categorically distinct. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2015; 10:1437-48. [PMID: 25813790 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsv032] [Citation(s) in RCA: 128] [Impact Index Per Article: 12.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/30/2014] [Accepted: 03/19/2015] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Understanding how emotions are represented neurally is a central aim of affective neuroscience. Despite decades of neuroimaging efforts addressing this question, it remains unclear whether emotions are represented as distinct entities, as predicted by categorical theories, or are constructed from a smaller set of underlying factors, as predicted by dimensional accounts. Here, we capitalize on multivariate statistical approaches and computational modeling to directly evaluate these theoretical perspectives. We elicited discrete emotional states using music and films during functional magnetic resonance imaging scanning. Distinct patterns of neural activation predicted the emotion category of stimuli and tracked subjective experience. Bayesian model comparison revealed that combining dimensional and categorical models of emotion best characterized the information content of activation patterns. Surprisingly, categorical and dimensional aspects of emotion experience captured unique and opposing sources of neural information. These results indicate that diverse emotional states are poorly differentiated by simple models of valence and arousal, and that activity within separable neural systems can be mapped to unique emotion categories.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Philip A Kragel
- Department of Psychology & Neuroscience and Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, NC, USA
| | - Kevin S LaBar
- Department of Psychology & Neuroscience and Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, NC, USA
| |
Collapse
|
89
|
Distinct populations of neurons respond to emotional valence and arousal in the human subthalamic nucleus. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2015; 112:3116-21. [PMID: 25713375 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1410709112] [Citation(s) in RCA: 40] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Both animal studies and studies using deep brain stimulation in humans have demonstrated the involvement of the subthalamic nucleus (STN) in motivational and emotional processes; however, participation of this nucleus in processing human emotion has not been investigated directly at the single-neuron level. We analyzed the relationship between the neuronal firing from intraoperative microrecordings from the STN during affective picture presentation in patients with Parkinson's disease (PD) and the affective ratings of emotional valence and arousal performed subsequently. We observed that 17% of neurons responded to emotional valence and arousal of visual stimuli according to individual ratings. The activity of some neurons was related to emotional valence, whereas different neurons responded to arousal. In addition, 14% of neurons responded to visual stimuli. Our results suggest the existence of neurons involved in processing or transmission of visual and emotional information in the human STN, and provide evidence of separate processing of the affective dimensions of valence and arousal at the level of single neurons as well.
Collapse
|
90
|
Omigie D, Dellacherie D, Hasboun D, Clément S, Baulac M, Adam C, Samson S. Intracranial markers of emotional valence processing and judgments in music. Cogn Neurosci 2014; 6:16-23. [PMID: 25496511 DOI: 10.1080/17588928.2014.988131] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
Abstract
The involvement of the amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex in the processing of valenced stimuli is well established. However, less is known about the extent to which activity in these regions reflects a stimulus' physical properties, the individual subjective experience it evokes, or both. We recorded cortical electrical activity from five epileptic patients implanted with depth electrodes for presurgical evaluation while they rated "consonant" and "dissonant" musical chords using a "pleasantness" scale. We compared the pattern of responses in the amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex when trials were sorted by pleasantness judgments relative to when they were sorted by the acoustic properties known to influence emotional reactions to musical chords. This revealed earlier differential activity in the amygdala in the physical properties-based, relative to in the judgment-based, analyses. Thus, our results demonstrate that the amygdala has, first and foremost, a high initial sensitivity to the physical properties of valenced stimuli. The finding that differentiations in the amygdala based on pleasantness ratings had a longer latency suggests that in this structure, mediation of emotional judgment follows accumulation of sensory information. This is in contrast to the orbitofrontal cortex where sensitivity to sensory information did not precede differentiation based on affective judgments.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Diana Omigie
- a Laboratoire de Neurosciences Fonctionnelles et Pathologies , Université de Lille , Lille , France
| | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
91
|
Vaughan DN, Jackson GD. The piriform cortex and human focal epilepsy. Front Neurol 2014; 5:259. [PMID: 25538678 PMCID: PMC4259123 DOI: 10.3389/fneur.2014.00259] [Citation(s) in RCA: 100] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/27/2014] [Accepted: 11/22/2014] [Indexed: 11/28/2022] Open
Abstract
It is surprising that the piriform cortex, when compared to the hippocampus, has been given relatively little significance in human epilepsy. Like the hippocampus, it has a phylogenetically preserved three-layered cortex that is vulnerable to excitotoxic injury, has broad connections to both limbic and cortical areas, and is highly epileptogenic – being critical to the kindling process. The well-known phenomenon of early olfactory auras in temporal lobe epilepsy highlights its clinical relevance in human beings. Perhaps because it is anatomically indistinct and difficult to approach surgically, as it clasps the middle cerebral artery, it has, until now, been understandably neglected. In this review, we emphasize how its unique anatomical and functional properties, as primary olfactory cortex, predispose it to involvement in focal epilepsy. From recent convergent findings in human neuroimaging, clinical epileptology, and experimental animal models, we make the case that the piriform cortex is likely to play a facilitating and amplifying role in human focal epileptogenesis, and may influence progression to epileptic intractability.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- David N Vaughan
- Florey Institute of Neuroscience and Mental Health , Heidelberg, VIC , Australia ; Department of Neurology, Austin Health , Heidelberg, VIC , Australia
| | - Graeme D Jackson
- Florey Institute of Neuroscience and Mental Health , Heidelberg, VIC , Australia ; Department of Neurology, Austin Health , Heidelberg, VIC , Australia ; Department of Medicine, University of Melbourne , Melbourne, VIC , Australia
| |
Collapse
|
92
|
Bach DR, Hurlemann R, Dolan RJ. Impaired threat prioritisation after selective bilateral amygdala lesions. Cortex 2014; 63:206-13. [PMID: 25282058 PMCID: PMC4317193 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.08.017] [Citation(s) in RCA: 43] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/04/2014] [Revised: 06/11/2014] [Accepted: 08/18/2014] [Indexed: 01/17/2023]
Abstract
The amygdala is proposed to process threat-related information in non-human animals. In humans, empirical evidence from lesion studies has provided the strongest evidence for a role in emotional face recognition and social judgement. Here we use a face-in-the-crowd (FITC) task which in healthy control individuals reveals prioritised threat processing, evident in faster serial search for angry compared to happy target faces. We investigate AM and BG, two individuals with bilateral amygdala lesions due to Urbach–Wiethe syndrome, and 16 control individuals. In lesion patients we show a reversal of a threat detection advantage indicating a profound impairment in prioritising threat information. This is the first direct demonstration that human amygdala lesions impair prioritisation of threatening faces, providing evidence that this structure has a causal role in responding to imminent danger.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dominik R Bach
- Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, University College London, UK; Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy, and Psychosomatics, University of Zurich, Switzerland.
| | | | - Raymond J Dolan
- Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, University College London, UK
| |
Collapse
|
93
|
Naudin M, Atanasova B. Olfactory markers of depression and Alzheimer's disease. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2014; 45:262-70. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.06.016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 46] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/27/2014] [Revised: 05/23/2014] [Accepted: 06/27/2014] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
|
94
|
Hudry J, Ryvlin P, Saive AL, Ravel N, Plailly J, Royet JP. Lateralization of olfactory processing: differential impact of right and left temporal lobe epilepsies. Epilepsy Behav 2014; 37:184-90. [PMID: 25048728 DOI: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2014.06.034] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/16/2014] [Revised: 06/26/2014] [Accepted: 06/29/2014] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
Abstract
Olfactory processes were reported to be lateralized. The purpose of this study was to further explore this phenomenon and investigate the effect of the hemispheric localization of epileptogenic foci on olfactory deficits in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE). Olfactory functioning was assessed in 61 patients and 60 healthy control (HC) subjects. The patients and HC subjects were asked to rate the intensity, pleasantness, familiarity, and edibility of 12 common odorants and then identify them. Stimulations were delivered monorhinally in the nostril ipsilateral to the epileptogenic focus in TLE and arbitrarily in either the left or the right nostril in the HC subjects. The results demonstrated that regardless of the side of stimulation, patients with TLE had reduced performance in all olfactory tasks compared with the HC subjects. With regard to the side of the epileptogenic focus, patients with left TLE judged odors as less pleasant and had more difficulty with identification than patients with right TLE, underlining a privileged role of the left hemisphere in the emotional and semantic processing of odors. Finally, irrespective of group, a tendency towards a right-nostril advantage for judging odor familiarity was found in agreement with a prominent role of the right hemisphere in odor memory processing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Julie Hudry
- Olfaction: From Coding to Memory Team, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, INSERM U1028, CNRS UMR 5292, University of Lyon, France
| | - Philippe Ryvlin
- Department of Functional Neurology and Epileptology, Hospices Civils de Lyon, Lyon, France; TIGER, Neuroscience Research Center of Lyon, INSERM U1028, CNRS 5292, Lyon-1 University, Lyon, France; Department of Clinical Neurosciences, CHUV, Lausanne, Swizterland
| | - Anne-Lise Saive
- Olfaction: From Coding to Memory Team, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, INSERM U1028, CNRS UMR 5292, University of Lyon, France
| | - Nadine Ravel
- Olfaction: From Coding to Memory Team, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, INSERM U1028, CNRS UMR 5292, University of Lyon, France
| | - Jane Plailly
- Olfaction: From Coding to Memory Team, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, INSERM U1028, CNRS UMR 5292, University of Lyon, France
| | - Jean-Pierre Royet
- Olfaction: From Coding to Memory Team, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, INSERM U1028, CNRS UMR 5292, University of Lyon, France.
| |
Collapse
|
95
|
Mainland JD, Lundström JN, Reisert J, Lowe G. From molecule to mind: an integrative perspective on odor intensity. Trends Neurosci 2014; 37:443-54. [PMID: 24950600 PMCID: PMC4119848 DOI: 10.1016/j.tins.2014.05.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 64] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/29/2014] [Revised: 05/01/2014] [Accepted: 05/15/2014] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
A fundamental problem in systems neuroscience is mapping the physical properties of a stimulus to perceptual characteristics. In vision, wavelength translates into color; in audition, frequency translates into pitch. Although odorant concentration is a key feature of olfactory stimuli, we do not know how concentration is translated into perceived intensity by the olfactory system. A variety of neural responses at several levels of processing have been reported to vary with odorant concentration, suggesting specific coding models. However, it remains unclear which, if any, of these phenomena underlie the perception of odor intensity. Here, we provide an overview of current models at different stages of olfactory processing, and identify promising avenues for future research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Joel D Mainland
- Monell Chemical Senses Center, Philadelphia, PA, USA; Department of Neuroscience, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA.
| | - Johan N Lundström
- Monell Chemical Senses Center, Philadelphia, PA, USA; Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden; Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | | | - Graeme Lowe
- Monell Chemical Senses Center, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
96
|
Amygdala responses to valence and its interaction by arousal revealed by MEG. Int J Psychophysiol 2014; 93:121-33. [DOI: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.05.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/23/2012] [Revised: 04/17/2013] [Accepted: 05/10/2013] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
|
97
|
Griffiths KR, Morris RW, Balleine BW. Translational studies of goal-directed action as a framework for classifying deficits across psychiatric disorders. Front Syst Neurosci 2014; 8:101. [PMID: 24904322 PMCID: PMC4033402 DOI: 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00101] [Citation(s) in RCA: 93] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/18/2014] [Accepted: 05/09/2014] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
The ability to learn contingencies between actions and outcomes in a dynamic environment is critical for flexible, adaptive behavior. Goal-directed actions adapt to changes in action-outcome contingencies as well as to changes in the reward-value of the outcome. When networks involved in reward processing and contingency learning are maladaptive, this fundamental ability can be lost, with detrimental consequences for decision-making. Impaired decision-making is a core feature in a number of psychiatric disorders, ranging from depression to schizophrenia. The argument can be developed, therefore, that seemingly disparate symptoms across psychiatric disorders can be explained by dysfunction within common decision-making circuitry. From this perspective, gaining a better understanding of the neural processes involved in goal-directed action, will allow a comparison of deficits observed across traditional diagnostic boundaries within a unified theoretical framework. This review describes the key processes and neural circuits involved in goal-directed decision-making using evidence from animal studies and human neuroimaging. Select studies are discussed to outline what we currently know about causal judgments regarding actions and their consequences, action-related reward evaluation, and, most importantly, how these processes are integrated in goal-directed learning and performance. Finally, we look at how adaptive decision-making is impaired across a range of psychiatric disorders and how deepening our understanding of this circuitry may offer insights into phenotypes and more targeted interventions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kristi R Griffiths
- Behavioural Neuroscience Laboratory, Brain and Mind Research Institute, University of Sydney Camperdown, Sydney, NSW, Australia
| | - Richard W Morris
- Behavioural Neuroscience Laboratory, Brain and Mind Research Institute, University of Sydney Camperdown, Sydney, NSW, Australia
| | - Bernard W Balleine
- Behavioural Neuroscience Laboratory, Brain and Mind Research Institute, University of Sydney Camperdown, Sydney, NSW, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
98
|
Towards a Crossmodal Exploration of Cognitive Deficits in Psychopathology. Psychol Belg 2014. [DOI: 10.5334/pb.as] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/20/2022] Open
|
99
|
Delle-Vigne D, Wang W, Kornreich C, Verbanck P, Campanella S. Emotional facial expression processing in depression: data from behavioral and event-related potential studies. Neurophysiol Clin 2014; 44:169-187. [PMID: 24930940 DOI: 10.1016/j.neucli.2014.03.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 34] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/05/2012] [Revised: 03/14/2014] [Accepted: 03/17/2014] [Indexed: 12/21/2022] Open
Abstract
Behavioral literature investigating emotional processes in depressive populations (i.e., unipolar and bipolar depression) states that, compared to healthy controls, depressive subjects exhibit disrupted emotional processing, indexed by lower performance and/or delayed response latencies. The development of brain imaging techniques, such as functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), provided the possibility to visualize the brain regions engaged in emotional processes and how they fail to interact in psychiatric diseases. However, fMRI suffers from poor temporal resolution and cognitive function involves various steps and cognitive stages (serially or in parallel) to give rise to a normal performance. Thus, the origin of a behavioral deficit may result from the alteration of a cognitive stage differently situated along the information-processing stream, outlining the importance of access to this dynamic "temporal" information. In this paper, we will illustrate, through depression, the role that should be attributed to cognitive event-related potentials (ERPs). Indeed, owing to their optimal temporal resolution, ERPs can monitor the neural processes engaged in disrupted cognitive function and provide crucial information for its treatment, training of the impaired cognitive functions and guidelines for clinicians in the choice and monitoring of appropriate medication for the patient.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- D Delle-Vigne
- Laboratoire de Psychologie Médicale et d'Addictologie, ULB Neuroscience Institute (UNI), CHU Brugmann-Université Libre de Bruxelles (U.L.B.), place Van Gehuchten, 4, 1020 Bruxelles, Belgium
| | - W Wang
- Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychiatry, Zhejiang University School of Medicine, Hangzhou, China.
| | - C Kornreich
- Laboratoire de Psychologie Médicale et d'Addictologie, ULB Neuroscience Institute (UNI), CHU Brugmann-Université Libre de Bruxelles (U.L.B.), place Van Gehuchten, 4, 1020 Bruxelles, Belgium
| | - P Verbanck
- Laboratoire de Psychologie Médicale et d'Addictologie, ULB Neuroscience Institute (UNI), CHU Brugmann-Université Libre de Bruxelles (U.L.B.), place Van Gehuchten, 4, 1020 Bruxelles, Belgium
| | - S Campanella
- Laboratoire de Psychologie Médicale et d'Addictologie, ULB Neuroscience Institute (UNI), CHU Brugmann-Université Libre de Bruxelles (U.L.B.), place Van Gehuchten, 4, 1020 Bruxelles, Belgium.
| |
Collapse
|
100
|
Bissonette GB, Gentry RN, Padmala S, Pessoa L, Roesch MR. Impact of appetitive and aversive outcomes on brain responses: linking the animal and human literatures. Front Syst Neurosci 2014; 8:24. [PMID: 24624062 PMCID: PMC3941203 DOI: 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00024] [Citation(s) in RCA: 35] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/29/2013] [Accepted: 02/04/2014] [Indexed: 12/11/2022] Open
Abstract
Decision-making is motivated by the possibility of obtaining reward and/or avoiding punishment. Though many have investigated behavior associated with appetitive or aversive outcomes, few have examined behaviors that rely on both. Fewer still have addressed questions related to how anticipated appetitive and aversive outcomes interact to alter neural signals related to expected value, motivation, and salience. Here we review recent rodent, monkey, and human research that address these issues. Further development of this area will be fundamental to understanding the etiology behind human psychiatric diseases and cultivating more effective treatments.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Ronny N Gentry
- Department of Psychology, University of Maryland, College Park MD, USA
| | - Srikanth Padmala
- Department of Psychology, University of Maryland, College Park MD, USA
| | - Luiz Pessoa
- Department of Psychology, University of Maryland, College Park MD, USA
| | - Matthew R Roesch
- Department of Psychology, University of Maryland, College Park MD, USA
| |
Collapse
|