201
|
Winandy L, Denoël M. The aggressive personality of an introduced fish affects foraging behavior in a polymorphic newt. Behav Ecol 2015. [DOI: 10.1093/beheco/arv101] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/16/2023] Open
|
202
|
Blaser R, Heyser C. Spontaneous object recognition: a promising approach to the comparative study of memory. Front Behav Neurosci 2015; 9:183. [PMID: 26217207 PMCID: PMC4498097 DOI: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00183] [Citation(s) in RCA: 52] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/02/2015] [Accepted: 06/29/2015] [Indexed: 01/11/2023] Open
Abstract
Spontaneous recognition of a novel object is a popular measure of exploratory behavior, perception and recognition memory in rodent models. Because of its relative simplicity and speed of testing, the variety of stimuli that can be used, and its ecological validity across species, it is also an attractive task for comparative research. To date, variants of this test have been used with vertebrate and invertebrate species, but the methods have seldom been sufficiently standardized to allow cross-species comparison. Here, we review the methods necessary for the study of novel object recognition in mammalian and non-mammalian models, as well as the results of these experiments. Critical to the use of this test is an understanding of the organism's initial response to a novel object, the modulation of exploration by context, and species differences in object perception and exploratory behaviors. We argue that with appropriate consideration of species differences in perception, object affordances, and natural exploratory behaviors, the spontaneous object recognition test can be a valid and versatile tool for translational research with non-mammalian models.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rachel Blaser
- Department of Psychological Sciences, University of San DiegoSan Diego, CA, USA
| | - Charles Heyser
- Behavioral Testing Core, Department of Neurosciences, University of California, San DiegoSan Diego, CA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
203
|
Ferrari MC, McCormick MI, Allan BJM, Choi RB, Ramasamy RA, Chivers DP. The effects of background risk on behavioural lateralization in a coral reef fish. Funct Ecol 2015. [DOI: 10.1111/1365-2435.12483] [Citation(s) in RCA: 33] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Maud C.O. Ferrari
- Department of Biomedical Sciences WCVM University of Saskatchewan Saskatoon SK S7N 5E4 Canada
| | - Mark I. McCormick
- ARC Centre of Excellence for Coral Reef Studies and College of Marine & Environmental Sciences James Cook University Townsville Qld 4810 Australia
| | - Bridie J. M. Allan
- ARC Centre of Excellence for Coral Reef Studies and College of Marine & Environmental Sciences James Cook University Townsville Qld 4810 Australia
| | - Rebecca B. Choi
- Department of Biology Macalester College St. Paul MN 55105 USA
| | - Ryan A. Ramasamy
- ARC Centre of Excellence for Coral Reef Studies and College of Marine & Environmental Sciences James Cook University Townsville Qld 4810 Australia
| | - Douglas P. Chivers
- Department of Biology University of Saskatchewan Saskatoon SK S7N 5E2 Canada
| |
Collapse
|
204
|
Bierbach D, Sommer-Trembo C, Hanisch J, Wolf M, Plath M. Personality affects mate choice: bolder males show stronger audience effects under high competition. Behav Ecol 2015. [DOI: 10.1093/beheco/arv079] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/14/2022] Open
|
205
|
Schrandt MN, Powers SP. Facilitation and Dominance in a Schooling Predator: Foraging Behavior of Florida Pompano, Trachinotus carolinus. PLoS One 2015; 10:e0130095. [PMID: 26068114 PMCID: PMC4466254 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0130095] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/09/2014] [Accepted: 05/15/2015] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Presumably an individual's risk of predation is reduced by group membership and this 'safety in numbers' concept has been readily applied to investigations of schooling prey; however, foraging in groups may also be beneficial. We tested the hypothesis that, when feeding in groups, foraging of a coastal fish (Florida Pompano, Trachinotus carolinus) on a benthic prey source would be facilitated (i.e. fish feeding in groups will consume more prey items). Although this question has been addressed for other fish species, it has not been previously addressed for Florida Pompano, a fish known to exhibit schooling behavior and that is used for aquaculture, where understanding the feeding ecology is important for healthy and efficient grow-out. In this experiment, juvenile Florida Pompano were offered a fixed number of coquina clams (Donax spp.) for one hour either in a group or as individuals. The following day they were tested in the opposite configuration. Fish in groups achieved greater consumption (average of 26 clams consumed by the entire group) than the individuals comprising the group (average of 14 clams consumed [sum of clams consumed by all individuals of the group]). Fish in groups also had fewer unsuccessful foraging attempts (2.75 compared to 4.75 hr(-1)) and tended to have a shorter latency until the first feeding activity. Our results suggest fish in groups were more comfortable feeding and more successful in their feeding attempts. Interestingly, the consumption benefit of group foraging was not shared by all--not all fish within a group consumed equal numbers of clams. Taken together, the results support our hypothesis that foraging in a group provides facilitation, but the short-term benefits are not equally shared by all individuals.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Meagan N. Schrandt
- Department of Marine Sciences, University of South Alabama, Mobile, Alabama, United States of America
- Dauphin Island Sea Lab, Dauphin Island, Alabama, United States of America
- * E-mail:
| | - Sean P. Powers
- Department of Marine Sciences, University of South Alabama, Mobile, Alabama, United States of America
- Dauphin Island Sea Lab, Dauphin Island, Alabama, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
206
|
Sopinka NM, Hinch SG, Healy SJ, Harrison PM, Patterson DA. Egg cortisol treatment affects the behavioural response of coho salmon to a conspecific intruder and threat of predation. Anim Behav 2015. [DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.03.011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
|
207
|
|
208
|
Way GP, Kiesel AL, Ruhl N, Snekser JL, McRobert SP. Sex differences in a shoaling-boldness behavioral syndrome, but no link with aggression. Behav Processes 2015; 113:7-12. [DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2014.12.014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/03/2014] [Revised: 12/22/2014] [Accepted: 12/31/2014] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
|
209
|
Griffin AS, Guillette LM, Healy SD. Cognition and personality: an analysis of an emerging field. Trends Ecol Evol 2015; 30:207-14. [DOI: 10.1016/j.tree.2015.01.012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 234] [Impact Index Per Article: 23.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/26/2014] [Revised: 01/22/2015] [Accepted: 01/30/2015] [Indexed: 01/25/2023]
|
210
|
Space-use and sociability are not related to public-information use in ninespine sticklebacks. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 2015. [DOI: 10.1007/s00265-015-1901-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
|
211
|
Larsen MH, Johnsson JI, Winberg S, Wilson ADM, Hammenstig D, Thörnqvist PO, Midwood JD, Aarestrup K, Höglund E. Effects of emergence time and early social rearing environment on behaviour of Atlantic salmon: consequences for juvenile fitness and smolt migration. PLoS One 2015; 10:e0119127. [PMID: 25747862 PMCID: PMC4352035 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0119127] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/08/2014] [Accepted: 01/09/2015] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Consistent individual differences in behaviour have been well documented in a variety of animal taxa, but surprisingly little is known about the fitness and life-history consequences of such individual variation. In wild salmonids, the timing of fry emergence from gravel spawning nests has been suggested to be coupled with individual behavioural traits. Here, we further investigate the link between timing of spawning nest emergence and behaviour of Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar), test effects of social rearing environment on behavioural traits in fish with different emergence times, and assess whether behavioural traits measured in the laboratory predict growth, survival, and migration status in the wild. Atlantic salmon fry were sorted with respect to emergence time from artificial spawning nest into three groups: early, intermediate, and late. These emergence groups were hatchery-reared separately or in co-culture for four months to test effects of social rearing environment on behavioural traits. Twenty fish from each of the six treatment groups were then subjected to three individual-based behavioural tests: basal locomotor activity, boldness, and escape response. Following behavioural characterization, the fish were released into a near-natural experimental stream. Results showed differences in escape behaviour between emergence groups in a net restraining test, but the social rearing environment did not affect individual behavioural expression. Emergence time and social environment had no significant effects on survival, growth, and migration status in the stream, although migration propensity was 1.4 to 1.9 times higher for early emerging individuals that were reared separately. In addition, despite individuals showing considerable variation in behaviour across treatment groups, this was not translated into differences in growth, survival, and migration status. Hence, our study adds to the view that fitness (i.e., growth and survival) and life-history predictions from laboratory measures of behaviour should be made with caution and ideally tested in nature.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Martin H. Larsen
- National Institute of Aquatic Resources, Section for Freshwater Fisheries and Ecology, Technical University of Denmark, Silkeborg, Denmark
| | - Jörgen I. Johnsson
- Department of Biological and Environmental Sciences, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden
| | - Svante Winberg
- Department of Neuroscience, Uppsala University, Uppsala, Sweden
| | - Alexander D. M. Wilson
- Fish Ecology and Conservation Physiology Laboratory, Department of Biology and Institute of Environmental Science, Carleton University, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada
| | - David Hammenstig
- Department of Biological and Environmental Sciences, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden
| | | | - Jonathan D. Midwood
- Fish Ecology and Conservation Physiology Laboratory, Department of Biology and Institute of Environmental Science, Carleton University, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada
| | - Kim Aarestrup
- National Institute of Aquatic Resources, Section for Freshwater Fisheries and Ecology, Technical University of Denmark, Silkeborg, Denmark
| | - Erik Höglund
- National Institute of Aquatic Resources, Section for Aquaculture, Technical University of Denmark, Hirtshals, Denmark
- Research Secretariat, University of Agder, Kristiansand, Norway
| |
Collapse
|
212
|
Backström T, Heynen M, Brännäs E, Nilsson J, Magnhagen C. The effect of anesthetics on carotenoid pigmentation and behavior in Arctic char (Salvelinus alpinus). J Vet Behav 2015. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jveb.2014.11.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/13/2023]
|
213
|
Kortet R, Sirkka I, Lai YT, Vainikka A, Kekäläinen J. Personality differences in two minnow populations that differ in their parasitism and predation risk. Front Ecol Evol 2015. [DOI: 10.3389/fevo.2015.00009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
|
214
|
Campbell JM, Carter PA, Wheeler PA, Thorgaard GH. Aggressive behavior, brain size and domestication in clonal rainbow trout lines. Behav Genet 2015; 45:245-54. [PMID: 25647468 DOI: 10.1007/s10519-014-9696-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/28/2014] [Accepted: 11/22/2014] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
Domestication causes behavior and brain size changes in many species. We addressed three questions using clonal rainbow trout lines: What are the mirror-elicited aggressive tendencies in lines with varying degrees of domestication? How does brain size relate to genotype and domestication level? Finally, is there a relationship between aggressive behavior and brain size? Clonal lines, although sampling a limited subset of the species variation, provide us with a reproducible experimental system with which we can develop hypotheses for further research. We performed principal component analyses on 12 continuous behavior and brain/body size variables and one discrete behavioral variable ("yawn") and detected several aggression syndromes. Two behaviors, "freeze" and "escape", associated with high domestication; "display" and "yawn" behavior associated with wild lines and "swim against the mirror" behavior associated with semi-wild and domestic lines. Two brain size traits, total brain and olfactory volume, were significantly related to domestication level when taking total body size into account, with domesticated lines having larger total brain volume and olfactory regions. The aggression syndromes identified indicate that future QTL mapping studies on domestication-related traits would likely be fruitful.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Janet M Campbell
- School of Biological Sciences and Center for Reproductive Biology, Washington State University, PO Box 644236, Pullman, WA, 99164-4236, USA
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
215
|
|
216
|
Chaverri G, Gillam EH. Repeatability in the contact calling system of Spix's disc-winged bat (Thyroptera tricolor). ROYAL SOCIETY OPEN SCIENCE 2015; 2:140197. [PMID: 26064578 PMCID: PMC4448792 DOI: 10.1098/rsos.140197] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/24/2014] [Accepted: 12/22/2014] [Indexed: 06/04/2023]
Abstract
Spix's disc-winged bat (Thyroptera tricolor) forms cohesive groups despite using an extremely ephemeral roost, partly due to the use of two acoustic signals that help individuals locate roost sites and group members. While the calls that aid in group cohesion are commonly used, some bats rarely or never produce them. Here, we examine whether the differences observed in the contact calling behaviour of T. tricolor are repeatable; that is, whether individual differences are consistent. We recorded contact calls of individuals in the field and rates and patterns of vocalization. To determine whether measured variables were consistent within individuals, we estimated repeatability (R), which compares within-individual to among-individual variance in behavioural traits. Our results show that repeatability for call variables was moderate but significant, and that repeatability was highest for the average number of calls produced (R=0.46-0.49). Our results demonstrate important individual differences in the contact calling behaviour of T. tricolor; we discuss how these could be the result of mechanisms such as frequency-dependent selection that favour groups composed of individuals with diverse vocal strategies. Future work should address whether changes in social environment, specifically group membership and social status, affect vocal behaviour.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Erin H. Gillam
- Department of Biological Sciences, North Dakota State University, Fargo, ND 58108, USA
| |
Collapse
|
217
|
Tudorache C, ter Braake A, Tromp M, Slabbekoorn H, Schaaf MJM. Behavioral and physiological indicators of stress coping styles in larval zebrafish. Stress 2015; 18:121-8. [PMID: 25407298 DOI: 10.3109/10253890.2014.989205] [Citation(s) in RCA: 32] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Different individuals cope with stressors in different ways. Stress coping styles are defined as a coherent set of individual behavioral and physiological differences in the response to a stressor which remain consistent across time and context. In the present study, we have investigated coping styles in larval zebrafish (Danio rerio) at 8 days post-fertilization. Larvae were separated into two groups, according to the emergence sequence from a darkened into a novel well-lit environment, early (EE) and late (LE) emergers. We used brief periods of netting as a stressor. Swimming behavior and kinematics before and after netting stress were analyzed, as were whole-body cortisol levels before and at 10, 30 and 60 min after the stress event. The results show that general swimming activity was different between EE and LE larvae, with lower baseline cumulative distance and more erratic swimming movements in EE than in LE larvae. EE larvae showed a faster recovery to baseline levels after stress than LE larvae. Cortisol baseline levels were not different between EE and LE larvae, but peak levels after stress were higher and the recovery towards basal levels was faster in EE than in LE larvae. This study shows that coping styles are manifest in zebrafish larvae, and that behavior and swimming kinematics are associated with different cortisol responses to stress. A better understanding of the expression of coping styles may be of great value for medical applications, animal welfare issues and conservation.
Collapse
|
218
|
Harrison PM, Gutowsky LFG, Martins EG, Patterson DA, Cooke SJ, Power M. Personality-dependent spatial ecology occurs independently from dispersal in wild burbot (Lota lota). Behav Ecol 2014. [DOI: 10.1093/beheco/aru216] [Citation(s) in RCA: 80] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/02/2023] Open
|
219
|
Stamps JA, Krishnan VV. Combining Information from Ancestors and Personal Experiences to Predict Individual Differences in Developmental Trajectories. Am Nat 2014; 184:647-57. [DOI: 10.1086/678116] [Citation(s) in RCA: 92] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/03/2022]
|
220
|
Stamps JA, Krishnan VV. Individual differences in the potential and realized developmental plasticity of personality traits. Front Ecol Evol 2014. [DOI: 10.3389/fevo.2014.00069] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/22/2023] Open
|
221
|
|
222
|
Shark personalities? Repeatability of social network traits in a widely distributed predatory fish. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 2014. [DOI: 10.1007/s00265-014-1805-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 40] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/21/2023]
|
223
|
Silva PIM, Martins CIM, Höglund E, Gjøen HM, Øverli Ø. Feeding motivation as a personality trait in Nile tilapia (Oreochromis niloticus): role of serotonergic neurotransmission. FISH PHYSIOLOGY AND BIOCHEMISTRY 2014; 40:1547-1557. [PMID: 24858238 DOI: 10.1007/s10695-014-9947-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/18/2014] [Accepted: 05/08/2014] [Indexed: 06/03/2023]
Abstract
Consistent individual variation in behaviour and physiology (i.e. animal personality or coping style) has emerged as a central topic in many biological disciplines. Yet, underlying mechanisms of crucial personality traits like feeding behaviour in novel environments remain unclear. Comparative studies, however, reveal a strong degree of evolutionary conservation of neural mechanisms controlling such behaviours throughout the vertebrate lineage. Previous studies have indicated duration of stress-induced anorexia as a consistent individual characteristic in teleost fishes. This study aims to determine to what degree brain 5-hydroxytryptamine (5-HT, serotonin) activity pertains to this aspect of animal personality, as a correlate to feed anticipatory behaviour and recovery of feed intake after transfer to a novel environment. Crucial to the definition of animal personality, a strong degree of individual consistency in different measures of feeding behaviour (feeding latency and feeding score), was demonstrated. Furthermore, low serotonergic activity in the hypothalamus was highly correlated with a personality characterized by high feeding motivation, with feeding motivation represented as an overall measure incorporating several behavioural parameters in a Principle Component Analyses (PCA). This study thus confirms individual variation in brain 5-HT neurotransmission as a correlate to complex behavioural syndromes related to feeding motivation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Patricia I M Silva
- Department of Animal and Aquacultural Sciences, Norwegian University of Life Sciences, P.O. Box 5003, 1432, Aas, Norway,
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
224
|
Dzieweczynski TL, Campbell BA, Marks JM, Logan B. Acute exposure to 17α-ethinylestradiol alters boldness behavioral syndrome in female Siamese fighting fish. Horm Behav 2014; 66:577-84. [PMID: 25161058 DOI: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2014.08.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/09/2014] [Revised: 08/13/2014] [Accepted: 08/18/2014] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
The role of anthropogenic sources in generating, maintaining, and influencing behavioral syndromes has recently been identified as an important area of future research. Endocrine disrupting chemicals are prevalent and persistent in aquatic ecosystems worldwide. These chemicals are known to have marked effects on the morphology and behavior of exposed individuals and, as such, may serve as a potential influence on behavioral syndromes. However, both the effects of exposure on behaviors beyond courtship and aggression and how exposure might affect behavioral variation at the individual level are understudied. To address this question, we examined boldness behavior in female Siamese fighting fish in three different assays (Novel Environment, Empty Tank, Shoaling) both before and after they were exposed to the estrogen mimic, 17α-ethinylestradiol (EE2). EE2 influences courtship, aggression, and boldness in males of this species but its effects have not been examined in females, to our knowledge. Females were tested multiple times in each assay before and after exposure so that behavioral consistency could be examined. A behavioral syndrome for boldness and activity level occurred across the three assays. The reductions in boldness and loss of the behavioral syndrome that resulted from EE2 exposure were surprising and suggest that the effects of EE2 exposure on female behavior and physiology should be examined more frequently. This study is one of the first to examine the effects of EE2 in females as well as on correlated behaviors and emphasizes the importance of examining the effects of endocrine disrupting chemicals on individual behavioral variation and consistency.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Teresa L Dzieweczynski
- Department of Psychology, University of New England, 11 Hills Beach Rd, Biddeford, ME 04005, USA.
| | - Brennah A Campbell
- Department of Psychology, University of New England, 11 Hills Beach Rd, Biddeford, ME 04005, USA
| | - Jodi M Marks
- Department of Psychology, University of New England, 11 Hills Beach Rd, Biddeford, ME 04005, USA
| | - Brittney Logan
- Department of Psychology, University of New England, 11 Hills Beach Rd, Biddeford, ME 04005, USA
| |
Collapse
|
225
|
Holley ED, Ludwig KJ, McEwen DC, Wisenden BD. Predictability of food supply, but not ration, increases exploratory behaviour in zebrafish Danio rerio. JOURNAL OF FISH BIOLOGY 2014; 85:597-604. [PMID: 24976249 DOI: 10.1111/jfb.12441] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/07/2014] [Accepted: 05/07/2014] [Indexed: 06/03/2023]
Abstract
Individual zebrafish Danio rerio were assayed for exploratory tendency in a serial open field test before and after being maintained on one of four diet treatments that differed in ration and in predictability of food delivery. Danio rerio became more exploratory after being maintained on a diet with a predictable delivery schedule. There was no effect of ration. Thus, exploratory behaviour is inducible by environmental influences independent of genetic predisposition or social interactions. These results have implications for management of correlated behavioural syndromes of exploratory and boldness of animals reared in captivity for later release into wild populations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- E D Holley
- Department of Biosciences, Minnesota State University Moorhead, 1104 7th Ave S, Moorhead, MN, 56563, U.S.A
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
226
|
Toms CN, Echevarria DJ. Back to Basics: Searching for a Comprehensive Framework for Exploring Individual Differences in Zebrafish (Danio Rerio) Behavior. Zebrafish 2014; 11:325-40. [DOI: 10.1089/zeb.2013.0952] [Citation(s) in RCA: 46] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/05/2023] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Christina N. Toms
- Department of Biology, The University of Central Florida, Orlando, Florida
| | - David J. Echevarria
- Department of Psychology, The University of Southern Mississippi, Hattiesburg, Mississippi
| |
Collapse
|
227
|
Hiadlovská Z, Macholán M, Mikula O, Vošlajerová Bímová B. The meek inherit the earth: less aggressive wild mice are more successful in challenging situations. Biol J Linn Soc Lond 2014. [DOI: 10.1111/bij.12307] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Zuzana Hiadlovská
- Laboratory of Mammalian Evolutionary Genetics; Institute of Animal Physiology and Genetics; Academy of Sciences of the Czech Republic; Brno Czech Republic
- Institute of Botany and Zoology; Faculty of Sciences; Masaryk University; Brno Czech Republic
| | - Miloš Macholán
- Laboratory of Mammalian Evolutionary Genetics; Institute of Animal Physiology and Genetics; Academy of Sciences of the Czech Republic; Brno Czech Republic
- Institute of Botany and Zoology; Faculty of Sciences; Masaryk University; Brno Czech Republic
| | - Ondřej Mikula
- Laboratory of Mammalian Evolutionary Genetics; Institute of Animal Physiology and Genetics; Academy of Sciences of the Czech Republic; Brno Czech Republic
- Institute of Vertebrate Biology; Academy of Sciences of the Czech Republic; Brno Czech Republic
| | - Barbora Vošlajerová Bímová
- Laboratory of Mammalian Evolutionary Genetics; Institute of Animal Physiology and Genetics; Academy of Sciences of the Czech Republic; Brno Czech Republic
- Institute of Vertebrate Biology; Academy of Sciences of the Czech Republic; Brno Czech Republic
| |
Collapse
|
228
|
Manuel R, Gorissen M, Roca CP, Zethof J, van de Vis H, Flik G, van den Bos R. Inhibitory avoidance learning in zebrafish (Danio rerio): effects of shock intensity and unraveling differences in task performance. Zebrafish 2014; 11:341-52. [PMID: 25004302 DOI: 10.1089/zeb.2013.0970] [Citation(s) in RCA: 37] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/28/2022] Open
Abstract
The zebrafish (Danio rerio) is increasingly used as a model in neurobehavioral and neuroendocrine studies. The inhibitory avoidance paradigm has been proposed as tool to study mechanisms underlying learning and memory in zebrafish. In this paradigm subjects receive a shock after entering the black compartment of a black-white box. On the next day, latency to enter the black compartment is assessed; higher latencies are indicative of increased avoidance learning. Here, we aimed to understand the effects of different shock intensities (0, 1, 3, and 9 V) and to unravel variation in inhibitory avoidance learning in an in-house reared Tuebingen Long-Fin zebrafish (D. rerio) strain. While median latencies had increased in the 1, 3, and 9 V groups, no increase in median latency was found in the 0 V group. In addition, higher shock intensities resulted in a higher number of avoiders (latency ≥180 s) over nonavoiders (latency <60 s). Both changes are indicative of increased avoidance learning. We assessed whole-body cortisol content and the expression levels of genes relevant to stress, anxiety, fear, and learning 2 h after testing. Shock intensity was associated with whole-body cortisol content and the expression of glucocorticoid receptor alpha [nr3c1(alpha)], cocaine- and amphetamine-regulated transcript (cart4), and mineralocorticoid receptor (nr3c2), while avoidance behavior was associated with whole-body cortisol content only. The inhibitory avoidance paradigm in combination with measuring whole-body cortisol content and gene expression is suitable to unravel (genetic) mechanisms of fear avoidance learning. Our data further show differences in brain-behavior relationships underlying fear avoidance learning and memory in zebrafish. These findings serve as starting point for further unraveling differences in brain-behavior relationships underlying (fear avoidance) learning and memory in zebrafish.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Remy Manuel
- 1 Department of Animal Physiology, Institute of Water and Wetland Research, Radboud University Nijmegen , Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
229
|
The link between social network density and rank-order consistency of aggressiveness in juvenile eels. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 2014. [DOI: 10.1007/s00265-014-1719-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
|
230
|
Taylor MK, Cooke SJ. Repeatability of movement behaviour in a wild salmonid revealed by telemetry. JOURNAL OF FISH BIOLOGY 2014; 84:1240-1246. [PMID: 24689676 DOI: 10.1111/jfb.12334] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/30/2013] [Accepted: 01/08/2014] [Indexed: 06/03/2023]
Abstract
Movement estimates derived from sub-daily tracking of radio-tagged bull trout Salvelinus confluentus on the Columbia River, British Columbia, Canada, were used to test whether interindividual variation in behaviour was repeatable among contexts, both short- and long-term. Interindividual variation in S. confluentus behaviour was consistent across contexts. These findings emphasize the potential for telemetry as a tool in animal personality and temperament research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- M K Taylor
- Fish Ecology and Conservation Physiology Laboratory, Department of Biology, Carleton University, 1125 Colonel By Drive, Ottawa, ON, K1S 5B6 Canada
| | | |
Collapse
|
231
|
Araújo A, Oliveira J, Barros N, Yamamoto M, Chellappa S. Dinâmica do Comportamento Territorial de Crenicichla menezesi (Osteichthyes: Perciformes: Cichlidae). BIOTA AMAZÔNIA 2014. [DOI: 10.18561/2179-5746/biotaamazonia.v4n1p37-44] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022] Open
|
232
|
Magnhagen C, Wacker S, Forsgren E, Cats Myhre L, Espy E, Amundsen T. Context consistency and seasonal variation in boldness of male two-spotted gobies. PLoS One 2014; 9:e93354. [PMID: 24671255 PMCID: PMC3966876 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0093354] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/27/2013] [Accepted: 03/05/2014] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
In order to attribute the behaviour of an animal to its personality it is important to study whether certain behavioural traits show up consistently across a variety of contexts. The aim of this study was to investigate whether breeding state males of the two-spotted goby, Gobiusculus flavescens, showed consistent degree of boldness when tested in four different behaviour assays. We also wanted to investigate whether boldness varied over the breeding season in accordance with changes in male-male competition for matings. We used two standard assays (the emergence test and the open field test), and two simple assays related to threat response. Repeated runs of each of the tests were highly correlated, and we found significant correlations between all four assays. Thus, we have documented both a within and a between-context consistency in risk-taking behaviour. Furthermore, we found that goby males studied during the middle of the breeding season were bolder than males studied at the end of the season. Since male two-spotted gobies face strongly decreasing male-male competition as the season progresses, the benefit of being bold for the mating success of the males may differ over the time of the breeding season. The difference in behaviour found over the season thus corresponds well with the sexual dynamics of this model species.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Carin Magnhagen
- Department of Wildlife, Fish, and Environmental Studies, Swedish University of Agricultural Sciences, Umeå, Sweden
| | - Sebastian Wacker
- Department of Biology, Norwegian University of Science and Technology (NTNU), Trondheim, Norway
| | | | - Lise Cats Myhre
- Department of Biology, Norwegian University of Science and Technology (NTNU), Trondheim, Norway
| | - Elizabeth Espy
- Department of Biology, Norwegian University of Science and Technology (NTNU), Trondheim, Norway
| | - Trond Amundsen
- Department of Biology, Norwegian University of Science and Technology (NTNU), Trondheim, Norway
| |
Collapse
|
233
|
|
234
|
Martins EP, Bhat A. Population-level personalities in zebrafish: aggression-boldness across but not within populations. Behav Ecol 2014. [DOI: 10.1093/beheco/aru007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 35] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/25/2022] Open
|
235
|
Lacasse J, Aubin-Horth N. Population-dependent conflict between individual sociability and aggressiveness. Anim Behav 2014. [DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.10.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
|
236
|
Hebert OL, Lavin LE, Marks JM, Dzieweczynski TL. The effects of 17α-ethinyloestradiol on boldness and its relationship to decision making in male Siamese fighting fish. Anim Behav 2014. [DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.10.032] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/07/2023]
|
237
|
White JR, McCormick MI, Meekan MG. Syndromes or flexibility: behavior during a life history transition of a coral reef fish. PLoS One 2013; 8:e84262. [PMID: 24386358 PMCID: PMC3874005 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0084262] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/18/2013] [Accepted: 11/21/2013] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
The theory of behavioral syndromes focuses on quantifying variation in behavior within and among individual organisms and attempts to account for the maintenance of differences in behavior that occur in a consistent manner among individuals. Behavioral syndromes have potentially important ecological consequences (e.g. survivorship tradeoffs) and can be shaped by population dynamics through selective mortality. Here, we search for any evidence for consistency of behavior across situations in juveniles of a common damselfish, Pomacentrus amboinensis (Pomacentridae) at the transition between larval habitats in the plankton and juvenile habitats on the reef. Naïve fish leaving the pelagic phase to settle on reefs were caught by light traps and their behaviors observed using similar methods across three different situations (small aquaria, large aquaria, field setting); all of which represent low risk and well-sheltered environments. Seven behavioral traits were compared within and among individuals across situations to determine if consistent behavioral syndromes existed. No consistency was found in any single or combination of behavioral traits for individuals across all situations. We suggest that high behavioral flexibility is likely beneficial for newly-settled fish at this ontogenetic transition and it is possible that consistent behavioral syndromes are unlikely to emerge in juveniles until environmental experience is gained or certain combinations of behaviors are favored by selective mortality.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- James R. White
- School of Marine and Tropical Biology, James Cook University, Townsville, Queensland, Australia
| | - Mark I. McCormick
- School of Marine and Tropical Biology, James Cook University, Townsville, Queensland, Australia
| | - Mark G. Meekan
- Australian Institute of Marine Science, Botany Building, The University of Western Australia, Crawley, Western Australia, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
238
|
Hammar L, Andersson S, Eggertsen L, Haglund J, Gullström M, Ehnberg J, Molander S. Hydrokinetic turbine effects on fish swimming behaviour. PLoS One 2013; 8:e84141. [PMID: 24358334 PMCID: PMC3866181 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0084141] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/03/2013] [Accepted: 11/12/2013] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Hydrokinetic turbines, targeting the kinetic energy of fast-flowing currents, are under development with some turbines already deployed at ocean sites around the world. It remains virtually unknown as to how these technologies affect fish, and rotor collisions have been postulated as a major concern. In this study the effects of a vertical axis hydrokinetic rotor with rotational speeds up to 70 rpm were tested on the swimming patterns of naturally occurring fish in a subtropical tidal channel. Fish movements were recorded with and without the rotor in place. Results showed that no fish collided with the rotor and only a few specimens passed through rotor blades. Overall, fish reduced their movements through the area when the rotor was present. This deterrent effect on fish increased with current speed. Fish that passed the rotor avoided the near-field, about 0.3 m from the rotor for benthic reef fish. Large predatory fish were particularly cautious of the rotor and never moved closer than 1.7 m in current speeds above 0.6 ms(-1). The effects of the rotor differed among taxa and feeding guilds and it is suggested that fish boldness and body shape influenced responses. In conclusion, the tested hydrokinetic turbine rotor proved non-hazardous to fish during the investigated conditions. However, the results indicate that arrays comprising multiple turbines may restrict fish movements, particularly for large species, with possible effects on habitat connectivity if migration routes are exploited. Arrays of the investigated turbine type and comparable systems should therefore be designed with gaps of several metres width to allow large fish to pass through. In combination with further research the insights from this study can be used for guiding the design of hydrokinetic turbine arrays where needed, so preventing ecological impacts.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Linus Hammar
- Department of Energy and Environment, Chalmers University of Technology, Gothenburg, Sweden
- * E-mail:
| | | | - Linda Eggertsen
- Department of Ecology, Environment and Plant Sciences, Stockholm University, Stockholm, Sweden
| | - Johan Haglund
- Department of Ecology, Environment and Plant Sciences, Stockholm University, Stockholm, Sweden
| | - Martin Gullström
- Department of Ecology, Environment and Plant Sciences, Stockholm University, Stockholm, Sweden
| | - Jimmy Ehnberg
- Department of Energy and Environment, Chalmers University of Technology, Gothenburg, Sweden
| | - Sverker Molander
- Department of Energy and Environment, Chalmers University of Technology, Gothenburg, Sweden
| |
Collapse
|
239
|
King AJ, Fürtbauer I, Mamuneas D, James C, Manica A. Sex-differences and temporal consistency in stickleback fish boldness. PLoS One 2013; 8:e81116. [PMID: 24324664 PMCID: PMC3852232 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0081116] [Citation(s) in RCA: 63] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/26/2013] [Accepted: 10/15/2013] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Behavioural traits that co-vary across contexts or situations often reflect fundamental trade-offs which individuals experience in different contexts (e.g. fitness trade-offs between exploration and predation risk). Since males tend to experience greater variance in reproductive success than females, there may be considerable fitness benefits associated with "bolder" behavioural types, but only recently have researchers begun to consider sex-specific and life-history strategies associated with these. Here we test the hypothesis that male three-spined sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus) show high risk but potentially high return behaviours compared to females. According to this hypothesis we predicted that male fish would show greater exploration of their environment in a foraging context, and be caught sooner by an experimenter than females. We found that the time fish spent out of cover exploring their environment was correlated over two days, and males spent significantly more time out of cover than females. Also, the order in which fish were net-caught from their holding aquarium by an experimenter prior to experiments was negatively correlated with the time spent out of cover during tests, and males tended to be caught sooner than females. Moreover, we found a positive correlation between the catch number prior to our experiments and nine months after, pointing towards consistent, long-term individual differences in behaviour.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Andrew J. King
- Department of Biosciences, College of Science, Swansea University, Swansea, United Kingdom
- The Structure and Motion Laboratory, The Royal Veterinary College, Hatfield, Hertfordshire, United Kingdom
- Evolutionary Ecology Group, Department of Zoology, Cambridge University, Cambridge, United Kingdom
- * E-mail:
| | - Ines Fürtbauer
- Department of Biosciences, College of Science, Swansea University, Swansea, United Kingdom
| | - Diamanto Mamuneas
- The Structure and Motion Laboratory, The Royal Veterinary College, Hatfield, Hertfordshire, United Kingdom
| | - Charlotte James
- The Structure and Motion Laboratory, The Royal Veterinary College, Hatfield, Hertfordshire, United Kingdom
| | - Andrea Manica
- Evolutionary Ecology Group, Department of Zoology, Cambridge University, Cambridge, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
240
|
Muraco JJ, Aspbury AS, Gabor CR. Does male behavioral type correlate with species recognition and stress? Behav Ecol 2013. [DOI: 10.1093/beheco/art106] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/28/2022] Open
|
241
|
Tudorache C, Schaaf MJM, Slabbekoorn H. Covariation between behaviour and physiology indicators of coping style in zebrafish (Danio rerio). J Endocrinol 2013; 219:251-8. [PMID: 24198397 DOI: 10.1530/joe-13-0225] [Citation(s) in RCA: 49] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
All vertebrates exhibit physiological responses to a wide variety of stressors. The amplitude and profile of the response depend on the intensity, duration, controllability and predictability of the stressor, but there is also individual variation in the response, termed coping style. A better understanding of the expression of coping styles is of great value for medical applications, animal welfare issues and conservation. Here, we investigated the effect of repeated netting stress on proactive and reactive zebrafish (Danio rerio) as an upcoming model system for stress research. Fish were separated by coping styles according to the order of entering a novel environment. Subsequently, repeated netting stress was applied as stressor, over a period of 21 days. Full-body cortisol levels were determined at 0, 15, 30, 60 and 120 min after the last repeated stress event. Our results show that reactive fish display i) increased basal cortisol concentrations after being repeatedly stressed, ii) higher cortisol secretion over time and iii) slow recovery of cortisol concentration towards basal levels after the last repeated stress event. This study shows for the first time in zebrafish that different coping styles are associated with different cortisol responses during the recovery from stress over time and that coping styles can explain otherwise unaccounted variation in physiological stress responses.
Collapse
MESH Headings
- Allostasis
- Animals
- Behavior, Animal
- Brain/growth & development
- Brain/metabolism
- Disease Models, Animal
- Disease Susceptibility
- Female
- Gene Expression Regulation, Developmental
- Hydrocortisone/metabolism
- Male
- Nerve Tissue Proteins/biosynthesis
- Nerve Tissue Proteins/genetics
- Nerve Tissue Proteins/metabolism
- Neurons/metabolism
- RNA, Messenger/metabolism
- Receptors, Glucocorticoid/biosynthesis
- Receptors, Glucocorticoid/genetics
- Receptors, Glucocorticoid/metabolism
- Receptors, Mineralocorticoid/biosynthesis
- Receptors, Mineralocorticoid/genetics
- Receptors, Mineralocorticoid/metabolism
- Stress, Physiological
- Stress, Psychological/metabolism
- Zebrafish/growth & development
- Zebrafish/physiology
- Zebrafish Proteins/biosynthesis
- Zebrafish Proteins/genetics
- Zebrafish Proteins/metabolism
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Christian Tudorache
- Departments of Molecular Cell Biology Behavioral Biology, Institute of Biology, Leiden University, Sylviusweg 72, 2333 BE Leiden, The Netherlands
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
242
|
Giske J, Eliassen S, Fiksen Ø, Jakobsen PJ, Aksnes DL, Jørgensen C, Mangel M. Effects of the emotion system on adaptive behavior. Am Nat 2013; 182:689-703. [PMID: 24231532 DOI: 10.1086/673533] [Citation(s) in RCA: 36] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/03/2022]
Abstract
A central simplifying assumption in evolutionary behavioral ecology has been that optimal behavior is unaffected by genetic or proximate constraints. Observations and experiments show otherwise, so that attention to decision architecture and mechanisms is needed. In psychology, the proximate constraints on decision making and the processes from perception to behavior are collectively described as the emotion system. We specify a model of the emotion system in fish that includes sensory input, neuronal computation, developmental modulation, and a global organismic state and restricts attention during decision making for behavioral outcomes. The model further includes food competition, safety in numbers, and a fluctuating environment. We find that emergent strategies in evolved populations include common emotional appraisal of sensory input related to fear and hunger and also include frequency-dependent rules for behavioral responses. Focused attention is at times more important than spatial behavior for growth and survival. Spatial segregation of the population is driven by personality differences. By coupling proximate and immediate influences on behavior with ultimate fitness consequences through the emotion system, this approach contributes to a unified perspective on the phenotype, by integrating effects of the environment, genetics, development, physiology, behavior, life history, and evolution.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jarl Giske
- Department of Biology, University of Bergen, Postboks 7803, 5020 Bergen, Norway
| | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
243
|
Tran S, Gerlai R. Individual differences in activity levels in zebrafish (Danio rerio). Behav Brain Res 2013; 257:224-9. [PMID: 24084583 DOI: 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.09.040] [Citation(s) in RCA: 64] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/26/2013] [Revised: 09/21/2013] [Accepted: 09/23/2013] [Indexed: 12/17/2022]
Abstract
Individual differences and variation in behavioral responses have been identified in many animal species. These differences may be the result of genetic or environmental factors or the interaction between them. Analysis of individual differences in behavior may be important for many reasons. The zebrafish is a powerful model organism that is rapidly gaining popularity in behavioral brain research. However, individual differences have rarely been explored in zebrafish although significant variation in their performance has been reported. In the current study we identified individual differences in activity levels of zebrafish using a genetically heterogeneous population. Groups of zebrafish classified as high, medium, or low activity performers demonstrated consistent activity levels over a period of 7 days, and also in a subsequent open field task, suggesting stable individual differences as opposed to stochastic variation among subjects. We also uncovered a sex dependent relationship between behavioral measures. Female zebrafish in the high activity group preferred the top portion of the tank, whereas low activity females preferred the lower portion but males did not show such a relationship. The relationship between these two behaviors in females implies the potential existence of a behavioral syndrome persisting between contexts. Furthermore, females demonstrated a higher level of consistency in their behavior as compared to males, and the behavioral differences were found to be independent of both body size and weight of the tested subjects. The identification of individual differences in activity levels in zebrafish will allow the investigation of underlying genetic and/or environmental underpinnings.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Steven Tran
- Department of Cell and Systems Biology, University of Toronto at Mississauga, Canada
| | | |
Collapse
|
244
|
Haage M, Bergvall UA, Maran T, Kiik K, Angerbjörn A. Situation and context impacts the expression of personality: the influence of breeding season and test context. Behav Processes 2013; 100:103-9. [PMID: 23988476 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2013.08.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/28/2012] [Revised: 08/16/2013] [Accepted: 08/17/2013] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
Abstract
Non-human animal personality is defined as consistent behavioural differences across time and situations/contexts. Behaviours are, however, often plastic and to explain how plasticity and personality may coexist an adaptive framework has been developed. Still, there is little information on how personality is impacted by situations and contexts on an individual level. We investigated this in the European mink (Mustela lutreola) by performing a set of five experiments in two situations consisting of non-breeding and breeding season, and by using different test contexts. Three personality trait domains were identified; boldness, exploration and sociability. The levels of boldness and exploration changed between seasons but remained repeatable, which implies behavioural reaction norms and supports that the concept of personality remained applicable despite plasticity. Whilst males became bolder and more explorative in the breeding season females became shyer, which reflects European mink breeding behaviour. Furthermore, behaviours performed in mirror stimulus tests fell into different domains depending on whether, the test was conducted in the own territory or not, suggesting plasticity in the response towards conspecifics. To conclude, our results highlight the importance of situation and context for the expression of personality, and the significance of measuring multiple personality trait domains with several methods.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marianne Haage
- Department of Zoology, Stockholm University, SE-106 91 Stockholm, Sweden.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
245
|
Koeck B, Alós J, Caro A, Neveu R, Crec'hriou R, Saragoni G, Lenfant P. Contrasting fish behavior in artificial seascapes with implications for resources conservation. PLoS One 2013; 8:e69303. [PMID: 23935978 PMCID: PMC3728332 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0069303] [Citation(s) in RCA: 31] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/07/2013] [Accepted: 06/07/2013] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Artificial reefs are used by many fisheries managers as a tool to mitigate the impact of fisheries on coastal fish communities by providing new habitat for many exploited fish species. However, the comparison between the behavior of wild fish inhabiting either natural or artificial habitats has received less attention. Thus the spatio-temporal patterns of fish that establish their home range in one habitat or the other and their consequences of intra-population differentiation on life-history remain largely unexplored. We hypothesize that individuals with a preferred habitat (i.e. natural vs. artificial) can behave differently in terms of habitat use, with important consequences on population dynamics (e.g. life-history, mortality, and reproductive success). Therefore, using biotelemetry, 98 white seabream (Diplodus sargus) inhabiting either artificial or natural habitats were tagged and their behavior was monitored for up to eight months. Most white seabreams were highly resident either on natural or artificial reefs, with a preference for the shallow artificial reef subsets. Connectivity between artificial and natural reefs was limited for resident individuals due to great inter-habitat distances. The temporal behavioral patterns of white seabreams differed between artificial and natural reefs. Artificial-reef resident fish had a predominantly nocturnal diel pattern, whereas natural-reef resident fish showed a diurnal diel pattern. Differences in diel behavioral patterns of white seabream inhabiting artificial and natural reefs could be the expression of realized individual specialization resulting from differences in habitat configuration and resource availability between these two habitats. Artificial reefs have the potential to modify not only seascape connectivity but also the individual behavioral patterns of fishes. Future management plans of coastal areas and fisheries resources, including artificial reef implementation, should therefore consider the potential effect of habitat modification on fish behavior, which could have key implications on fish dynamics.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Barbara Koeck
- Univ. Perpignan Via Domitia, CEntre de Formation et de Recherche sur les Environnements Méditerranéens, Perpignan, France.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
246
|
Wong MYL, Medina A, Uppaluri C, Arnold S, Seymour JR, Buston PM. Consistent behavioural traits and behavioural syndromes in pairs of the false clown anemonefish Amphiprion ocellaris. JOURNAL OF FISH BIOLOGY 2013; 83:207-213. [PMID: 23808701 DOI: 10.1111/jfb.12133] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/03/2013] [Accepted: 03/20/2013] [Indexed: 06/02/2023]
Abstract
Using the social clown anemonefish Amphiprion ocellaris, whether individuals exhibited consistency in activity levels, boldness and sociability in a paired context, and whether these three behavioural traits were positively correlated within a single behavioural syndrome, was investigated. The results highlight that consistent individual differences in behaviour are expressed in a social fish and suggest that consistent behavioural traits and behavioural syndromes could influence the structure and functioning of their societies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- M Y L Wong
- School of Biological Sciences, University of Wollongong, Northfields Avenue, NSW 2518, Australia.
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
247
|
Oswald ME, Singer M, Robison BD. The quantitative genetic architecture of the bold-shy continuum in zebrafish, Danio rerio. PLoS One 2013; 8:e68828. [PMID: 23840902 PMCID: PMC3698077 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0068828] [Citation(s) in RCA: 34] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/18/2013] [Accepted: 06/03/2013] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
In studies of consistent individual differences (personality) along the bold-shy continuum, a pattern of behavioral correlations frequently emerges: individuals towards the bold end of the continuum are more likely to utilize risky habitat, approach potential predators, and feed under risky conditions. Here, we address the hypothesis that observed phenotypic correlations among component behaviors of the bold-shy continuum are a result of underlying genetic correlations (quantitative genetic architecture). We used a replicated three-generation pedigree of zebrafish (Danio rerio) to study three putative components of the bold-shy continuum: horizontal position, swim level, and feeding latency. We detected significant narrow-sense heritabilities as well as significant genetic and phenotypic correlations among all three behaviors, such that fish selected for swimming at the front of the tank swam closer to the observer, swam higher in the water column, and fed more quickly than fish selected for swimming at the back of the tank. Further, the lines varied in their initial open field behavior (swim level and activity level). The quantitative genetic architecture of the bold-shy continuum indicates that the multivariate behavioral phenotype characteristic of a “bold” personality type may be a result of correlated evolution via underlying genetic correlations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mary E. Oswald
- Department of Biological Sciences, University of Idaho, Moscow, Idaho, United States of America
| | - Mathew Singer
- Department of Biological Sciences, University of Idaho, Moscow, Idaho, United States of America
| | - Barrie D. Robison
- Department of Biological Sciences, University of Idaho, Moscow, Idaho, United States of America
- * E-mail:
| |
Collapse
|
248
|
Garamszegi LZ, Markó G, Herczeg G. A meta-analysis of correlated behaviors with implications for behavioral syndromes: relationships between particular behavioral traits. Behav Ecol 2013. [DOI: 10.1093/beheco/art033] [Citation(s) in RCA: 53] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/19/2022] Open
|
249
|
Frost AJ, Thomson JS, Smith C, Burton HC, Davis B, Watts PC, Sneddon LU. Environmental change alters personality in the rainbow trout, Oncorhynchus mykiss. Anim Behav 2013. [DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.03.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 33] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
|
250
|
Massen JJM, Antonides A, Arnold AMK, Bionda T, Koski SE. A behavioral view on chimpanzee personality: exploration tendency, persistence, boldness, and tool-orientation measured with group experiments. Am J Primatol 2013; 75:947-58. [PMID: 23649750 DOI: 10.1002/ajp.22159] [Citation(s) in RCA: 69] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/02/2012] [Revised: 03/25/2013] [Accepted: 04/07/2013] [Indexed: 12/31/2022]
Abstract
Human and nonhuman animals show personality: temporal and contextual consistency in behavior patterns that vary among individuals. In contrast to most other species, personality of chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, has mainly been studied with non-behavioral methods. We examined boldness, exploration tendency, persistence and tool-orientation in 29 captive chimpanzees using repeated experiments conducted in an ecologically valid social setting. High temporal repeatability and contextual consistency in all these traits indicated they reflected personality. In addition, Principal Component Analysis revealed two independent syndromes, labeled exploration-persistence and boldness. We found no sex or rank differences in the trait scores, but the scores declined with age. Nonetheless, there was considerable inter-individual variation within age-classes, suggesting that behavior was not merely determined by age but also by dispositional effects. In conclusion, our study complements earlier rating studies and adds new traits to the chimpanzee personality, thereby supporting the existence of multiple personality traits among chimpanzees. We stress the importance of ecologically valid behavioral research to assess multiple personality traits and their association, as it allows inclusion of ape studies in the comparison of personality structures across species studied behaviorally, and furthers our attempts to unravel the causes and consequences of animal personality.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jorg J M Massen
- Department of Cognitive Biology, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|